SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Master of Applied Linguistics

       **** Topics in Syntax ****

           Radford, A ( 1997)

         Chapter (5) : Checking

                Handout

              Presented to:
       DR. AHMAD Al-SAKARNEH
Presented by: Eslam Yousef AL- Matarneh
             First semester
               2011 / 2012
    Kingdom of Jordan Mut’ah University
Outlines
    5.1 Overview
    5.2 Interpretable and uninterpretable features
    5.3 Checking
    5.4 phrases
    5.5 percolation
    5.6 determiner phrase
    5.7 PRO subjects
    5.8 Objective subjects
    5.9 Bare phrase structure
    5.10 Summary
5.1 overview
In chapter one , we came up with a conclusion that (all grammatical operations) are
(structure-dependent principle)
And when covered chapter two, we saw that (structure-dependant principle)
determines that all grammatical operations in natural languages are category-based , that
any word belongs to certain category, for example, (Noun category) shares the same
grammatical properties and features of the same words belong to (Noun category).
In chapter three, we will look at :
    A ) : The ways in which words can be combined together to form phrases and
sentences .
    B) : how we can represent the structure of the phrases and sentences thereby
formed 4- In chapter four,we have seen that empty categories (i.e. categories which
have no overt phonetic form) play a central role in syntactic theory .
5- In chapter five, we will discuss how the grammatical features carried by words are
checked.
5.2 Interpretable and uninterpretable features:

Exampl(1)

                                    1

                                    2




          For any given sentence, a descriptively adequate grammar should provide us with a PF
representation (the phonetic form that specifying how a sentence is pronounced) and with a LP
representation (the logical form that specifying what it means) and to know how that is done let’s
have a look at this diagram into details.
 1) According to this diagram, phrases and sentences are formed according to, two operations
which are: 1) Selection: a process of comprising sets of phonetic, semantic and grammatical
features. 2) Merger: by which constituents are combined together in a pair wise fashion.
2) Then we come to spellout : the point at which the phrase structures are generated by the process
of selection and merger and then to have first, {the PF operations} that process the phonetic
features . Second, {the LF operations} that process the semantic and grammatical features
3) Then the outputs of these operations are: A) PF representation that serves as input to
articulatory –perceptual systems B) LP representation that serves as input to conceptual-
intentional systems.
 According to the assumption, PF representation contains only phonetically interpretable features
and LP representation contains only semantically interpretable features , so what about the
grammatical features ? according to the assumption , the grammatical features play a role in
both 1) syntax and 2) morphology .
The grammatical features of syntax are :
Number :(singular/plural)features since these play an obvious role in agreement(these/*this books)
Gender: (masculine/feminine/inanimate)features since they play a role in the syntax of reflexive
anaphors?(he/*she/*It turned himself into a giant)
 Person features : which play a role in the syntax of subject-verb agreement(He/*I/*you likes
syntax)
The grammatical features in morphology are:
The case features of pronouns ( He/*Him/ likes Me/*I )
The inflectional features of verbs (He has gone/*go/going)
So let’s have this example to illustrate these grammatical features
Example( 2) : she has gone
The grammatical features of{ she} indicate that it is third person, feminine, singular, nomanitive
determiner and those of {has}indicate that it is third person, singular, present-tense auxiliary and
those of {gone} indicate that it is a {n-participle},some of these features are interpretable at LF in the
sense that they have semantic content and so contribute to determining meaning , whereas others
are uninterpretable at LF in that they have no semantic content and so make no contribution to
meaning .Foe example, the fact that she is a third person singular expression plays a role at LF
since it tells us e.g. that she can refer to the girl next door but not to the curtains next door. By
contrast, the fact that she is nominative does not as we can see from sentences such as the
following :
Example (3) : (a)They excpected {she will win}
                (b)They excpected {her to win}
The italicized subject (she, her) of the bracketed complement clauses play the same semantic role
in both sentences (as the subject of the win-clause), even though it has the nominative form(she)
in (3a) and the objective form(her) in (3b) and this suggests that case is an uninterpretable
feature. In much the same way that fact that {has} is a present-tense auxiliary has a role to play at
LF but the fact that {has} is third person singular seems to play no role at LF so {has} is simply a
consequence
of the grammatical requirement for {has} to agree with its subject he. Likewise the fact that {gone}
is a participle seems to have no role to play at LF but rather it simply a consequence of the fact that
  {have} requires a complement headed by a verb in the {+n participle} so we can come up with a
                                                conclusion that
 1) case-features                        2) agreement                         3) participle inflections
  Of verbs have no role to play in semantic interpretation which means to be erased as we will see
                                                    later on
                                 Example (4): (a) He/*Him/*They has gone
                                                (b) He has gone/*going/*go
   The examples in (4a) show that {has}can have a third person singular nominative pronoun like
   {he}as its specifier, but not a third person singular objective pronoun like{Him}or a third person
        plural nominative pronoun like{They} in other word, a finite auxiliary like{Has}imposes
     person/number/case restrictions on its specifier/subject .The example in (4b) show that
{has}allows as its complement a verb in the N-participle form, but not verb in the ing-participle form,
     or a verb in its infinitive form : in other words, {Has} imposes morphological restriction on its
                                                 complement .
5.3 Checking




We will apply the checking process to this example in order to understand what we mean in
checking, according to grammatical features , the head must have three features which are:
1)head- features           2) specifier-features             3) complement-features
The head {has} has three features : 1) head-features: {present tense}, 2) specifier-features: the
head {has} imposes to be his specifier {3SNom}as we can see here the specifier-features of the
head {has} is {3FSNom}(she) ,3) complement-features: the head {has}as a verb imposes to be his
complement past participle verb {+n} as we can see the complement {gone}
so the process of checking goes like this :
6) The specifier-features of a head {has} are checked against the head-features of its
specifier {she} likewise, the complement-features of a head {has} are checked against the
head-features of its complement.
Then we apply the checking test by erasing the features of the specifer{she} and the
complement{gone} that are similar to these features that the head{has} takes and as we know that
we erase the features that aren’t interpretable and these features related to 1) case 2)agreement
3)participle inflections of verbs and when apply the checking test we will have the following diagram
:




            As we can see that the only grammatical features { 3FS / Pres} that survived at LF are
      interpretable head-features since these survived features in example 7 are interpretable and
                                                        satisfy the principle of full interpretation.
Now let’s see how checking breaks down in an ungrammatical structure such as :




As we can see here the {3}specifier-feature of {has}can be erased because its specifier {them} is
a third person pronoun.But the {S} feature of has which requires its subject to be singular so can’t
be erased because the subject of has is the plural pronoun{them} and so remains unchecked
,likewise, the{Nom} specifier-feature of the head {has} requires to be its specifer a nominative
subject so also it remains unchecked because it isn’t compaitable with the case mark carried by the
pronoun {them}which is {Obj}, in addition, the {+n}complement-feature of the head {has} requires
to be its
complement a participle complement and therefore it can’t be erased since the feature carried by
the verb {go} is infinitival feature {Inf} so checking here is partial (incomplete) and therefore it
doesn’t satisfy the principle of full interpretation so in other words it means that this sentence is
ungrammatical as we can see in the following example (9)




             Let’s now move on to the know the specifier-features that are carried by the modals like
          (can/could, may/might, will/would, shall/should and must ) so let’s have this example :
Example: (10) They can swim




As we can see that {can} imposes 1) To be its specifier a nominative subject regardless to person and number of the subject it
takes so the subject could be first {I / We} or second{You} or third {he/she/they} and also its subject could be {singular or plural} .
2) To be its complement {Inf} so let’s apply the checking test on this sentence to see if it’s correct or not:
5.4 Phrases :
The examples discussed earlier were simple in that the specifiers and complements were single
words but now Let’s have a complex sentences where the complement and determiners are
phrases ,so let’s have this example :
Example (12) They are getting old
As we can see here that the head{are}imposes to be its specifier a second person or plural and also
to be its complement a verb in its {+ing}form {getting} but here the whole phrase{getting old} is
considered as a complement of the head {are} and within this complement there is a head {getting}
that imposes to be its complement an adjective {old} so after applying the checking test we will
have the following diagram :
Let’s now see how to treat the {phrasal specifier} in a similar fashion by giving the following
  example




           The {1PNom} head-features of the determiner {we} indicate that it is a first person plural
   nominative determiner, its {PN} complement-feature indicates that it requires a plural noun as its
         complement in this type of use not a singular noun(i.e. we *student). The head features of
    {students} indicate that it is a plural noun. The head-features of {are} tell us that it is a present-
  tense auxiliary; its specifier-features indicate that it requires a second person or plural nominative
             expression as its specifer and its complement-features indicate that it requires an {ing-
complement},finally the head features of {protesting} indicate that it is an {ing-participle}and when
 applying the checking test we consider {we students} as a one phrase according to the assumption
    that says phrases (i.e. we student) share the head-features of their heads (i.e.we) and once the
   head{we} is {PNom} the complement is also{PNom} so the head-feature of the whole phrase{we
                                                  student} is {PN} and then we will have the following :
Let’s consider how checking works in more complex structures :
Example: (15)   he might have been helping them




According to this diagram , the {Nom}specifier-feature of{ might} checked against the corresponding
{Nom} head-feature of {he}, leading to erasure of both {Nom} case-features. The {Inf} complement-
feature of {might} tells us that it requires an infinitival complement as we can see that such a
requirement is satisfied by {have}that carry the head-feature of being {Inf}. {have} requires a
participle complement and it’s satisfied by the {+n} which is {been} . {been} needs {+ing}
complement and it’s satisfied by {helping}. {helping} needs an object complement as we can see in
{them} and since the inflectional features( Nom /+n/+ing / Inf} are uninterpretable we erase them to
have the following diagram :
So let’s now consider the corresponding ungrammatical example of the sentence just mentioned
above
Example : (18) Him might having be helped they




as we can see that none of the specifier and complement features can be erased because the
{Nom} specifer-feature of {might}is incompatible with the {Obj} head-feature carried by the objective
pronoun {him}so neither case-feature can be erased. Likewise, the {Inf} complement-feature
{might}is incompatible with the {+ing} head-feature of the phrase headed by {having};
the{+n}complement-feature of {having} is incompatible with the{Inf}head-feature of the phrase
headed by {be}, the {+ing} complement-feature of {be}is incompatible with the{+n} head-feature of
the phrase headed by {helped}; and the {obj} complement-feature of {helped} is incompatible with
the {Nom} head-feature of the nominative case or inflectional features, the resulting LF
rrepresentation violates the principle of full interpretation , so causing the derivation to crash at LF.
5.5 Percolation :
Thus far we have considered how checking works in clauses headed by a {finite auxiliary} but what about {auxiliariless finite clauses}
such as the following :
Example (22) : she hates him




It is clear that the objective complement-feature of {hates} can be checked against the objective
head-features of him ,but if we assume that the specifier-features of a head are checked against the
head-features of its specifer, we can’t check the specifier-features of {hates}against the head-
feature of {she}and that is because the two are contained within different phrases , {she}being the
specifier of {IP} and {hates}being the head of {VP} so that{she} isn’t the specifer of {hates} and if the
relevant features remain unchecked ,the derivation will crash at LF and therefore will wrongly
predicate that sentence like (22) above is ungrammatical .
The question now is how to overcome this problem?
In 4.4, we outlined a possible solution, we suggested that in auxiliariless finite clauses, the
grammatical features carried by the verb percolate from V to I . however, clearly we don’t want to
assume that the complement-features of the verb {hates} percolate to I since these are checked
internally within {VP},so let’s assume that the complement-features of {hates} are first checked
internally within {VP} and erased, and then the remaining unchecked features of {hates} (i.e. its
head and specifier-features ) percolate from {V} to {INFL}, as shown by the arrows in (24) where we
assume that the objective complement-features of {hates} and the objective head-feature of {him}
have already checked and erased internally within {VP}
we can suppose that the {Pres} present-tense head-feature of {hates} percolate from {V} to
{INFL} in order to ensure{LF}convergence ,and if we make the reasonable assumption that
{INFL}is only interpretable at LF if it carries a tense-feature, so percolation of the {Pres}feature from
{V} to {I} is the way that provides a way of ensuring that {INFL} acquires a tense-feature and can
therefore be interpreted at LF. The assumption that the present- tense head –feature of {hates}
percolates up to {INFL} accounts for the fact that the corresponding tag-sentence {she hates him,
doesn’t she?}contains a present-tense auxiliary {doesn’t}since the auxiliary in a (tag )generally
carries the same tense-features as the head {INFL}constituent of the clause to which the tag is
attached (i.e. she has finished, hasn’t she ?) .
      In much the same way, we might suppose that the {3SNom} specifier-features of {hates} also
percolate to {INFL} in order to ensure LF convergence: since specifier features are uninterpretable,
they must be checked and erased in the course of deriving the relevant LF representation. But
since {hates}and {she}are within different phrases( hates is the head V of {VP}, and she is the
specifier of {IP}) the only way for the specifier-features to be checked is for them to percolate up to
{INFL},where they can then be checked against the corresponding {3SNom} head-feature of
{she}in that being in the same IP-projection ,so checking will result in erasure of the uninterpretable
{3SNom} specifier-features of {hates} and of the uninterpretable {Nom} head-feature of {she} so
after applying the checking test we will have the following diagram :
Example (26) : She does hate him




So let’s apply the checking test by erasing the specifer- and complement-features in along with nominative/objective-case head-
features of she/him and the infinitival head-feature of the verb {hate} which derives the LF representation (28) below :




            An interesting question which arises from our discussion here is why sentences
            such as the following (29) below should be ungrammatical:
Example (29): she does hates him
Lets assume that (29) has the following diagram as in
(30)




Actually, what makes the sentence (29) to crash at LF is two reasons:
2)That the complement-features of the {does} must be {Inf} and this can’t be satisfied since the head-features carried by {hates} is
not {inf} but it’s a present-tense feature .
3)The specifier-features of {does}and {hates}require them each to have a {3SNom} and the single subject pronoun {she} can’t serve
as the subject of both verbs. But why not?
Answer: because if the nominative-case specifier-features of {does}is checked against the nominative-case head-feature of{she},
the nominative case-feature of {she} will thereby be erased because it is unintrepretable and this will mean that the nominative
specifier-feature of {hates} can not be erased even if it percolates from V to INFL because the nominative head-feature of {she} has
already been erased.
5.6 Determiner phrases :
So far we have looked at structures that mainly involved arguments which are (personal pronouns). But how do deal with nominal
argument like:
(31)    The students are complaining.
The specifier feature of {are} requires its specifier phrase (the students) to be nominative and plural or 2 person subject. The
determiner {the} is the head of {DP} so in this sentence it can only bind with 3rd person reflexive anaphor not 1st or 2nd which
means that it is 3rd person expression :
( 32 )   The students take themselves/*ourselves/ *yourselves too seriously .
and since the whole phrase carries the head-features of its head{the}the {DP} then must be {3PNom}
Note: {The}: has variable number and case properties in that, it can come with plural/singular in number and with nominative/
objective in case, so how we determined the complement feature in sentence like : (33) ?




-The specifier feature of {are} which is [NomP] is checked against the head feature of the [NomP] in this particular usage of the ( it is
variable )
-The reasons for giving the complement feature[PN] not [SN] to {the} are :
(1)- The head {are} requires being its specifier-features {3NomP} and that is satisfied by the head-features of the {DP}
(2)- Or by supposing that the number feature of students percolate from N to D.
So let’s apply the assumption of the overt determiners as in (33)to the covert determiners(Null determiners) in sentence like :
( 34 )      Students are complaining
Here{students} is a {DP} headed by a {null determiner Ǿ} which has person, number, and case properties of its own (and here it is
3rd person to agree with the students ) and which proves that is the 3rd reflexive that {students} can only bind with in sentence (35)


( 35 ) Students are preparing themselves/*ourselves/ *yourselves for exam .
So the {null determiner} must be nominative plural in order to satisfy the requirement of {are} to have a nominative plural subject,
that is why sentences like 34 and 33 have the same structure except that the determiner is {over} in one and {covert} in the other .
Let’s now move on to see how checking is done within the internal {Determiner Phrase }and to do so let’s have a look at example
(36) below
( 36 ) [The numerous opposition allegations of incompetence] are unsettling the prime minister.
{Numerous} here is an attributive adjective serves as a specifier of a functional head {F}. This sentence has the structure as in
diagram (37) below:




                                     Let’s now have a look at 1) number-features of {DP} 2) agreement between its constituents
1- The number feature of {DP} : since {allegations} is plural and the adjective numerous is intrinsically plural ( its meaning ) so the
   D{ the} must be plural in order to make the whole DP be plural to agree with the AUX{are} . ( agreement between The, numerous
                                                                                                                       and allegations )
2- (a) The agreement between {numerous} and {allegations} – the plural noun feature of allegations [P] percolates up the tree to
                             the head F so it is checked against the number feature of {numerous}[P] as we can see in example (38)
(b) The agreement between {allegations} and {The}: according to the assumption which says that the determiners agree with the
nouns they modify the [P] feature of {allegations} percolates up from N to F and then from F to D and then it is checked against
the [P] feature of the {D} the: Or we can say that the has a [P] feature by percolation from the noun allegations as we can see in
39
5.7 PRO subjects :
As we know that {pronouns} carry case-feature whether it is overt or covert {Null}, so let’s see what the case-feature carried by the
(Null PRO) in the following example (40) is.
( 40 ) They are trying [ PRO to escape].




 According to Chomsky and lansik, they claim that the null-case carried by PRO is checked by {infinitival to} which means that the
  {infinitival to} requires its specifier in IPs like this (40) to be Null subject and therefore the specifier-feature of {to} will be checked
                                                                                         and erased with the head-feature of the Null PRO.
The [Nom] case of the specifier-feature of {has} is checked against the [Nom] case of the head- feature of
        {she}. So in both examples (41-42) checking involves a spec-head relation.
                      The finite constituent checks nominative case and so requires a nominative
    subject


                                                              Whereas
                          An infinitival to checks Null case and requires a PRO subject

           There are some verbs that controls their IPs complements to be Null-case to like the verb {try} in the previous example
           (40) but there are also verbs that don’t control their IPs complements to be a Null-case to and therefore the IP
           complement must have an objective subject as we can see in the following examples (43a ,44a)
            ( 43 ) (a) They believe [him to be wrong]

                They believe [PRO to be wrong]

          ( 44 ) (a)   They consider [him to have cheated]

(b) They consider [PRO to have cheated]
So there are two different uses of the {infinitival to} which are:
1)- When {to} checks a null case it requires a PRO subject with null case (with control verbs like try which select an IP complement
headed by a null case (to)
 2)- When it doesn’t check a null case it doesn't allow a PRO subject (with verbs like believe which don't allow it is IP complement to
have a null -case (to).
5.8 Objective subject :
The following examples are infinitival IPs with objective subjects like those bracketed in (45)
( 45 ) (a) They had expected [us to counterattack]
(c)We don't consider [him to be suitable]
        (a) He wouldn't have liked [me to reconsider]
        (b) We are keen for [them to take a part]
These kind of IPs occur only:
 (1) as a complement of transitive verbs like :(expect ,consider, like)
 (2)as a complement of complementizer{ for} .
 The case of the {objective subject}is then checked externally by a transitive verb or a complementizer{for}so, {us} is checked
by{ expected} ,{ him} by {consider} , {me} by liked , {them} by {for}
So sentences like the following(46)are ungrammatical:


( 46 ) (a)   *[them to abandon syntax] would be a mistake.




                                                                               (b) *He may be anxious [them to make amends]
                                                            (c) *Brigadier blunderbuss gave the order [them to cease fire]
                                  Because the (infinitive clauses in the brackets) are not the complement of transitive verbs or
  complementizer {for} .The bracketed IP in {46a} is the subject of {would}. In{46b} it is the complement of intransitive adjective
                    (anxious}. In {46c} it is the complement of the noun {order} (as we know that nouns too are intransitive).
The difference between the way of checking null PRO subject case and the objective subject case - The null PRO subject are
 checked internally (within the IP) by the null case infinitive (to).
                                         But
 - The case of an objective subject is checked externally by a transitive verb or complementizer(for)
 * Exceptional Case Marking: that the subject has it's case externally checked from outside of it's containing IP. So the
 complement with the objective subject is referred to as ECM complement. the verb which selects this kind of complements is
 called ECM-Verb.




 Let’s now have a look at the different ways in which the case properties of {null
 PRO} and {objective subjects} are checked:
          1)- In active and passive structures:

        (47) (a) They had decided [PRO to postpone the meeting]
 (e)It had been decided [PRO to postpone the meeting]
 the verb like{decided} is a control verb which takes an IP complement headed by (null case to) in both active and passive.
        (48) (a) People genuinely believed [him to be innocent ]
 •*It was genuinely believed [him to be innocent]


Whereas, a verb like {believed} can only function as an ECM verb taking Infinitive complement with an objective subject when
used in active sentences because if this verb is used in passive it will be intransitive and then will not allow an objective subject
but to have a null Pro case to
2)- Adverb position :


The {adverbs} that modifying the control verbs can be positioned between the control verb and its
IP as we can see in (49) below:
         (49) He tried hard [PRO to convince her]
   Unlike the adverbs modifying ECM verbs which can't be positioned between ECM -V and its IP
complement as we can see in (50) below:
         (50) *She believes sincerely [him to be innocent]
Because in(49) the adverb doesn't prevent the case of PRO from being checked (internally) but in
(50) it is ungrammatical because it’s intervening between the the matrix verb and the objective
subject so the transitive verb{believes}in (50) doesn’t immediately precede the pronoun {him} so the
adverb(sincerely) blocks or prevents the transitive verb from giving ECM for the objective
subject(him) which means that such sentence is ungrammatical .
Note: another evidence that intervening adverbs block a verb from checking the objective case of
its complement as we can see in (51) below
( 51) (a) He plays chess well                 (b) *He plays well chess
In (51a) is grammatical because the verb precedes its complement immediately but in (51b) is not
because the adverb is blocking the checking (intervening between the verb and its complement )
Conclusion : the objective case carried by the subject of an ECM infinitive is externally
checked from outside the infinitive complement by an immediately preceding transitive matrix verb
or complementizer and so this conclusion raise the following two results which are :
- Infinitival to in ECM doesn’t check the objective case carried by the infinitive subject ,but rather is
a caseless particlt .
The Objective case of an ECM infinitive subject can’t be checked from inside IP by infinitival to
and hence exceptionally has to be checked from outside IP by an immediately preceding transitive
matrix verb or complementizer
* There are three types of verb that take infinitival complement :
    1- verbs like (hope) allows null case {to}complement with PRO subject only (intransitive)
  ( 52) (a) She was hoping [PRO to win]             (b) *She was hoping [ him to win ]
    2- verbs like (believe) allows an IP complement with an objective subject only (transitive)
  ( 53) (a) She believes [him to be innocent] (b) *She believes [ PRO to be innocent ]
    3- verbs like (expect) which allows an Infinitival IP complement with either PRO or objective subject (can be transitive and
intransitive):
  ( 54) (a) She expects [PRO to be win] (b) She expects [him to be win]
Conclusion:
The analysis of infinitival IPs presented earlier makes four crucial assumptions :
1- ) there are two different kinds of infinitival to:
A) One checks null-case B) another which has no checking –case properties.
2-) there are different kinds of verbs which select different kinds of infinitive complements ( some select an IP headed by null-case
to, others and IP headed by caseless to .
3- ) Some verbs are transitive that check objective-case; others are intransitive; and yet others can be either transitive or
intransitive.
4- ) The case of an infinitive subject is checked externally by an immediately preceding the transitive matrix verb or
complementizer in structures where it can’t be checked internally
5-) the infinitival to differ not only in case-checking properties but also in their tense properties as we can see in example(55) below:
Example (55)
 A) Mary hope {PRO to be an actress}
 B) He believes{ her to be an actress}
In (55) the {to-close} seems to carry different temporal properties from the hopes-clause: the hopes-clause refers to the present
whereas the to-clause refers to the future, so (55a) is paraphrase able as “Mary hopes she will be an actress”. By Contrast, the
to-clause in (55b) seems to have the same temporal properties as the believes-clause (both have present time reference, as we see
from the fact that (55b) can be paraphrased as “He believes that she is an actress”
We can conclude that:
1) {to} in control infinitives has tense properties independent of those in the matrix-clause
2) {to} in control infinitives often has future time-reference, whereas {to} in ECM infinitives has tense properties determined by
those all the matrix clause
5.9 Bare phrases structure :
Sentence like (5) will have the structure :




        In this structure there is a potential notational inconsistency : that the categorical properties are presented by category labels
   attached to terminal nodes whereas, other grammatical properties of words are presented by sets of features attached to words
                                                                                                                              themselves .
 The fact that {she} has the categorical status of a determiner is indicated by the label D attached to terminal node carrying the word
       {she}whereas, that fact that {she} is third person femimine singular nominative is indicated by the bracketed [3FSNom] head-
                                                                                                                  features carried by {she}

    To eliminate this we would incorporate the categorical properties of a word into the head features of the word (the categories
                            labels are no longer used):and the non-terminal nodes are replaced with the heads of their constituent :
{D} of {she} is indicated in the head feature of {she} [3FSNomD]. {I} is indicated in the head
    feature of {has} [ PresI]. {V} is indicated in the head feature of {gone} [V +n]. This shows the
           economy principle in checking theory (to proscribe the superfluous symbols in structural
                                                                                    representations.
We might go further ans ask whether the category labels carried by non-terminal nodes should also
  be eliminated or not since they encode redundant information. And also whether the nonterminal
nodes need to carry any label or the structure can not be presented without them as we can see in
                                                       the following unlabelled example (58) below :
The argument is by saying that lexical entries make it predictable to us to know the terminal and
non-terminal nodes as the following :{ has gone} is a projection of {has} not{ gone}in that {has}
takes{ n- participle complement}and gone = does not allow for itself to take an AUX as its
specifier and {she has gone} is not the projection of {she} since it doesn't allow specifier of any
kind.
5.10 Summary:
         This chapter discussed how grammatical features are checked and the PF
 representation (phonetic form) and LF presentation (Logical form). In 5.2 it handled the
principles of full interpretation (PFI) that requires the Pf representation to contain only
    phonetic features and the LF representation to contain only sematic features so the
     derivation will be converge not crash. According to this chapter some grammatical
   features are interpretable and some are not and those which aren’t must be checked
 (erased). In 5.3 it handled the idea that words carry three sets of grammatical features
which are head feature, specifier feature and complement feature and the features which
    are not interpretable are to be erased and checked in checking process. In 5.4 the
  chapter discussed how checking works in phrasal specifier and complement and how
projections carry the head features of their head. In 5.5 it discussed the problem of finite
non auxiliaries verbs and how its specifier features percolate up to the unfilled I so it can
    be checked with the head feature of its specifier. In 5.6 we discussed how features
   carried by determiner, attributive adjectives and nouns are checked. 5.8 handled the
syntax of exceptional case-marking containing an infinitival IP with an objective subject.
  In 5.9 we discussed the fact that the category labels carried by terminal nodes can be
   eliminated and inserted in terms of head feature. Also the nonterminal nodes can be
  eliminated too so we develop a bare phrase structure ( unlabeled tree diagram ).

More Related Content

PPTX
Syntax: grammar of clauses; higher constituents
PPTX
Morphology # Productivity in Word-Formation
PDF
Lecture 2 sentence structure constituents
PPT
词汇学
DOC
Semnatics unit 12 and 19
PPTX
Morphology
DOC
Assignment on morphology
PPTX
morphology and syntax
Syntax: grammar of clauses; higher constituents
Morphology # Productivity in Word-Formation
Lecture 2 sentence structure constituents
词汇学
Semnatics unit 12 and 19
Morphology
Assignment on morphology
morphology and syntax

What's hot (20)

PPT
Theoretical grammar of_the_english_language (4)
PDF
Basic concepts-Sarah Saneei
PPT
6200933223111
PPTX
Morphology Dr Sabri alkatib
PPTX
Identification Of Morphemes
PPT
Morphology 2018
PPTX
Intro. to Linguistics_9 Morphology
PPTX
Morphology
PDF
Word Compounding
PPTX
Ppt morphology
PPTX
Aula 3 deriv. vs. infl morphs, affixes, grammatical morphemes, content and ...
PPTX
Morphology
DOC
Theory grammar-velyan
PPT
Introduction to linguistic (6)
PPSX
Linguistic morp
PPT
Additive morpheme
PPTX
Morphology presentation
PPT
Cohesion And Coherence Relations
PDF
Morphological rules- Sarah Saneei
Theoretical grammar of_the_english_language (4)
Basic concepts-Sarah Saneei
6200933223111
Morphology Dr Sabri alkatib
Identification Of Morphemes
Morphology 2018
Intro. to Linguistics_9 Morphology
Morphology
Word Compounding
Ppt morphology
Aula 3 deriv. vs. infl morphs, affixes, grammatical morphemes, content and ...
Morphology
Theory grammar-velyan
Introduction to linguistic (6)
Linguistic morp
Additive morpheme
Morphology presentation
Cohesion And Coherence Relations
Morphological rules- Sarah Saneei
Ad

Similar to Topics in syntax - checking (20)

PPTX
Syntax and Phrase: From Words to Major Phrase Types.pptx
PPTX
Grammatical categories and word classes
PPTX
Relasi gramatikal i ii (2)
PPTX
Relasi gramatikal 1 dan 2
PDF
Book review of analyzing grammar an introduction
PPT
Syntactic analysis-1211248481598474-9
PPTX
Chapter 5 meyer maryam noori khorasani
PDF
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
PPTX
morphosyntacsis/grammatical function
PPTX
General Education 2015 Strucure of English.pptx
PPT
PPTX
Unit 1 introductory categories and concepts (1)
DOCX
ThGr.Gap Lecture 4.docx
PPTX
Functional grammar
PPT
violencia en adolescentes
PPT
Structural analysis of english syntax
PPTX
Grammar Syntax(1).pptx
PDF
Answers to Problems for Syntax 4th Edition by Andrew Carnie
RTF
Tugas morfo lengkap
Syntax and Phrase: From Words to Major Phrase Types.pptx
Grammatical categories and word classes
Relasi gramatikal i ii (2)
Relasi gramatikal 1 dan 2
Book review of analyzing grammar an introduction
Syntactic analysis-1211248481598474-9
Chapter 5 meyer maryam noori khorasani
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
morphosyntacsis/grammatical function
General Education 2015 Strucure of English.pptx
Unit 1 introductory categories and concepts (1)
ThGr.Gap Lecture 4.docx
Functional grammar
violencia en adolescentes
Structural analysis of english syntax
Grammar Syntax(1).pptx
Answers to Problems for Syntax 4th Edition by Andrew Carnie
Tugas morfo lengkap
Ad

Recently uploaded (20)

PPTX
Cell Structure & Organelles in detailed.
PPTX
Orientation - ARALprogram of Deped to the Parents.pptx
PPTX
Radiologic_Anatomy_of_the_Brachial_plexus [final].pptx
PDF
OBE - B.A.(HON'S) IN INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE -Ar.MOHIUDDIN.pdf
DOC
Soft-furnishing-By-Architect-A.F.M.Mohiuddin-Akhand.doc
PDF
GENETICS IN BIOLOGY IN SECONDARY LEVEL FORM 3
PPTX
Tissue processing ( HISTOPATHOLOGICAL TECHNIQUE
PDF
2.FourierTransform-ShortQuestionswithAnswers.pdf
PPTX
UV-Visible spectroscopy..pptx UV-Visible Spectroscopy – Electronic Transition...
PDF
Practical Manual AGRO-233 Principles and Practices of Natural Farming
PDF
RMMM.pdf make it easy to upload and study
PDF
01-Introduction-to-Information-Management.pdf
PDF
grade 11-chemistry_fetena_net_5883.pdf teacher guide for all student
PDF
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
PDF
RTP_AR_KS1_Tutor's Guide_English [FOR REPRODUCTION].pdf
PDF
LNK 2025 (2).pdf MWEHEHEHEHEHEHEHEHEHEHE
PPTX
Introduction-to-Literarature-and-Literary-Studies-week-Prelim-coverage.pptx
PDF
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
PDF
Chapter 2 Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth.pdf
PPTX
Microbial diseases, their pathogenesis and prophylaxis
Cell Structure & Organelles in detailed.
Orientation - ARALprogram of Deped to the Parents.pptx
Radiologic_Anatomy_of_the_Brachial_plexus [final].pptx
OBE - B.A.(HON'S) IN INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE -Ar.MOHIUDDIN.pdf
Soft-furnishing-By-Architect-A.F.M.Mohiuddin-Akhand.doc
GENETICS IN BIOLOGY IN SECONDARY LEVEL FORM 3
Tissue processing ( HISTOPATHOLOGICAL TECHNIQUE
2.FourierTransform-ShortQuestionswithAnswers.pdf
UV-Visible spectroscopy..pptx UV-Visible Spectroscopy – Electronic Transition...
Practical Manual AGRO-233 Principles and Practices of Natural Farming
RMMM.pdf make it easy to upload and study
01-Introduction-to-Information-Management.pdf
grade 11-chemistry_fetena_net_5883.pdf teacher guide for all student
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
RTP_AR_KS1_Tutor's Guide_English [FOR REPRODUCTION].pdf
LNK 2025 (2).pdf MWEHEHEHEHEHEHEHEHEHEHE
Introduction-to-Literarature-and-Literary-Studies-week-Prelim-coverage.pptx
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
Chapter 2 Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth.pdf
Microbial diseases, their pathogenesis and prophylaxis

Topics in syntax - checking

  • 1. Master of Applied Linguistics **** Topics in Syntax **** Radford, A ( 1997) Chapter (5) : Checking Handout Presented to: DR. AHMAD Al-SAKARNEH Presented by: Eslam Yousef AL- Matarneh First semester 2011 / 2012 Kingdom of Jordan Mut’ah University
  • 2. Outlines 5.1 Overview 5.2 Interpretable and uninterpretable features 5.3 Checking 5.4 phrases 5.5 percolation 5.6 determiner phrase 5.7 PRO subjects 5.8 Objective subjects 5.9 Bare phrase structure 5.10 Summary
  • 3. 5.1 overview In chapter one , we came up with a conclusion that (all grammatical operations) are (structure-dependent principle) And when covered chapter two, we saw that (structure-dependant principle) determines that all grammatical operations in natural languages are category-based , that any word belongs to certain category, for example, (Noun category) shares the same grammatical properties and features of the same words belong to (Noun category). In chapter three, we will look at : A ) : The ways in which words can be combined together to form phrases and sentences . B) : how we can represent the structure of the phrases and sentences thereby formed 4- In chapter four,we have seen that empty categories (i.e. categories which have no overt phonetic form) play a central role in syntactic theory . 5- In chapter five, we will discuss how the grammatical features carried by words are checked.
  • 4. 5.2 Interpretable and uninterpretable features: Exampl(1) 1 2 For any given sentence, a descriptively adequate grammar should provide us with a PF representation (the phonetic form that specifying how a sentence is pronounced) and with a LP representation (the logical form that specifying what it means) and to know how that is done let’s have a look at this diagram into details. 1) According to this diagram, phrases and sentences are formed according to, two operations which are: 1) Selection: a process of comprising sets of phonetic, semantic and grammatical features. 2) Merger: by which constituents are combined together in a pair wise fashion. 2) Then we come to spellout : the point at which the phrase structures are generated by the process of selection and merger and then to have first, {the PF operations} that process the phonetic features . Second, {the LF operations} that process the semantic and grammatical features
  • 5. 3) Then the outputs of these operations are: A) PF representation that serves as input to articulatory –perceptual systems B) LP representation that serves as input to conceptual- intentional systems. According to the assumption, PF representation contains only phonetically interpretable features and LP representation contains only semantically interpretable features , so what about the grammatical features ? according to the assumption , the grammatical features play a role in both 1) syntax and 2) morphology . The grammatical features of syntax are : Number :(singular/plural)features since these play an obvious role in agreement(these/*this books) Gender: (masculine/feminine/inanimate)features since they play a role in the syntax of reflexive anaphors?(he/*she/*It turned himself into a giant) Person features : which play a role in the syntax of subject-verb agreement(He/*I/*you likes syntax) The grammatical features in morphology are: The case features of pronouns ( He/*Him/ likes Me/*I ) The inflectional features of verbs (He has gone/*go/going)
  • 6. So let’s have this example to illustrate these grammatical features Example( 2) : she has gone The grammatical features of{ she} indicate that it is third person, feminine, singular, nomanitive determiner and those of {has}indicate that it is third person, singular, present-tense auxiliary and those of {gone} indicate that it is a {n-participle},some of these features are interpretable at LF in the sense that they have semantic content and so contribute to determining meaning , whereas others are uninterpretable at LF in that they have no semantic content and so make no contribution to meaning .Foe example, the fact that she is a third person singular expression plays a role at LF since it tells us e.g. that she can refer to the girl next door but not to the curtains next door. By contrast, the fact that she is nominative does not as we can see from sentences such as the following : Example (3) : (a)They excpected {she will win} (b)They excpected {her to win} The italicized subject (she, her) of the bracketed complement clauses play the same semantic role in both sentences (as the subject of the win-clause), even though it has the nominative form(she) in (3a) and the objective form(her) in (3b) and this suggests that case is an uninterpretable feature. In much the same way that fact that {has} is a present-tense auxiliary has a role to play at LF but the fact that {has} is third person singular seems to play no role at LF so {has} is simply a consequence
  • 7. of the grammatical requirement for {has} to agree with its subject he. Likewise the fact that {gone} is a participle seems to have no role to play at LF but rather it simply a consequence of the fact that {have} requires a complement headed by a verb in the {+n participle} so we can come up with a conclusion that 1) case-features 2) agreement 3) participle inflections Of verbs have no role to play in semantic interpretation which means to be erased as we will see later on Example (4): (a) He/*Him/*They has gone (b) He has gone/*going/*go The examples in (4a) show that {has}can have a third person singular nominative pronoun like {he}as its specifier, but not a third person singular objective pronoun like{Him}or a third person plural nominative pronoun like{They} in other word, a finite auxiliary like{Has}imposes person/number/case restrictions on its specifier/subject .The example in (4b) show that {has}allows as its complement a verb in the N-participle form, but not verb in the ing-participle form, or a verb in its infinitive form : in other words, {Has} imposes morphological restriction on its complement .
  • 8. 5.3 Checking We will apply the checking process to this example in order to understand what we mean in checking, according to grammatical features , the head must have three features which are: 1)head- features 2) specifier-features 3) complement-features The head {has} has three features : 1) head-features: {present tense}, 2) specifier-features: the head {has} imposes to be his specifier {3SNom}as we can see here the specifier-features of the head {has} is {3FSNom}(she) ,3) complement-features: the head {has}as a verb imposes to be his complement past participle verb {+n} as we can see the complement {gone}
  • 9. so the process of checking goes like this : 6) The specifier-features of a head {has} are checked against the head-features of its specifier {she} likewise, the complement-features of a head {has} are checked against the head-features of its complement. Then we apply the checking test by erasing the features of the specifer{she} and the complement{gone} that are similar to these features that the head{has} takes and as we know that we erase the features that aren’t interpretable and these features related to 1) case 2)agreement 3)participle inflections of verbs and when apply the checking test we will have the following diagram : As we can see that the only grammatical features { 3FS / Pres} that survived at LF are interpretable head-features since these survived features in example 7 are interpretable and satisfy the principle of full interpretation.
  • 10. Now let’s see how checking breaks down in an ungrammatical structure such as : As we can see here the {3}specifier-feature of {has}can be erased because its specifier {them} is a third person pronoun.But the {S} feature of has which requires its subject to be singular so can’t be erased because the subject of has is the plural pronoun{them} and so remains unchecked ,likewise, the{Nom} specifier-feature of the head {has} requires to be its specifer a nominative subject so also it remains unchecked because it isn’t compaitable with the case mark carried by the pronoun {them}which is {Obj}, in addition, the {+n}complement-feature of the head {has} requires to be its
  • 11. complement a participle complement and therefore it can’t be erased since the feature carried by the verb {go} is infinitival feature {Inf} so checking here is partial (incomplete) and therefore it doesn’t satisfy the principle of full interpretation so in other words it means that this sentence is ungrammatical as we can see in the following example (9) Let’s now move on to the know the specifier-features that are carried by the modals like (can/could, may/might, will/would, shall/should and must ) so let’s have this example :
  • 12. Example: (10) They can swim As we can see that {can} imposes 1) To be its specifier a nominative subject regardless to person and number of the subject it takes so the subject could be first {I / We} or second{You} or third {he/she/they} and also its subject could be {singular or plural} . 2) To be its complement {Inf} so let’s apply the checking test on this sentence to see if it’s correct or not:
  • 13. 5.4 Phrases : The examples discussed earlier were simple in that the specifiers and complements were single words but now Let’s have a complex sentences where the complement and determiners are phrases ,so let’s have this example : Example (12) They are getting old
  • 14. As we can see here that the head{are}imposes to be its specifier a second person or plural and also to be its complement a verb in its {+ing}form {getting} but here the whole phrase{getting old} is considered as a complement of the head {are} and within this complement there is a head {getting} that imposes to be its complement an adjective {old} so after applying the checking test we will have the following diagram :
  • 15. Let’s now see how to treat the {phrasal specifier} in a similar fashion by giving the following example The {1PNom} head-features of the determiner {we} indicate that it is a first person plural nominative determiner, its {PN} complement-feature indicates that it requires a plural noun as its complement in this type of use not a singular noun(i.e. we *student). The head features of {students} indicate that it is a plural noun. The head-features of {are} tell us that it is a present- tense auxiliary; its specifier-features indicate that it requires a second person or plural nominative expression as its specifer and its complement-features indicate that it requires an {ing- complement},finally the head features of {protesting} indicate that it is an {ing-participle}and when applying the checking test we consider {we students} as a one phrase according to the assumption that says phrases (i.e. we student) share the head-features of their heads (i.e.we) and once the head{we} is {PNom} the complement is also{PNom} so the head-feature of the whole phrase{we student} is {PN} and then we will have the following :
  • 16. Let’s consider how checking works in more complex structures :
  • 17. Example: (15) he might have been helping them According to this diagram , the {Nom}specifier-feature of{ might} checked against the corresponding {Nom} head-feature of {he}, leading to erasure of both {Nom} case-features. The {Inf} complement- feature of {might} tells us that it requires an infinitival complement as we can see that such a requirement is satisfied by {have}that carry the head-feature of being {Inf}. {have} requires a participle complement and it’s satisfied by the {+n} which is {been} . {been} needs {+ing} complement and it’s satisfied by {helping}. {helping} needs an object complement as we can see in {them} and since the inflectional features( Nom /+n/+ing / Inf} are uninterpretable we erase them to have the following diagram :
  • 18. So let’s now consider the corresponding ungrammatical example of the sentence just mentioned above
  • 19. Example : (18) Him might having be helped they as we can see that none of the specifier and complement features can be erased because the {Nom} specifer-feature of {might}is incompatible with the {Obj} head-feature carried by the objective pronoun {him}so neither case-feature can be erased. Likewise, the {Inf} complement-feature {might}is incompatible with the {+ing} head-feature of the phrase headed by {having}; the{+n}complement-feature of {having} is incompatible with the{Inf}head-feature of the phrase headed by {be}, the {+ing} complement-feature of {be}is incompatible with the{+n} head-feature of the phrase headed by {helped}; and the {obj} complement-feature of {helped} is incompatible with the {Nom} head-feature of the nominative case or inflectional features, the resulting LF rrepresentation violates the principle of full interpretation , so causing the derivation to crash at LF.
  • 20. 5.5 Percolation : Thus far we have considered how checking works in clauses headed by a {finite auxiliary} but what about {auxiliariless finite clauses} such as the following : Example (22) : she hates him It is clear that the objective complement-feature of {hates} can be checked against the objective head-features of him ,but if we assume that the specifier-features of a head are checked against the head-features of its specifer, we can’t check the specifier-features of {hates}against the head- feature of {she}and that is because the two are contained within different phrases , {she}being the specifier of {IP} and {hates}being the head of {VP} so that{she} isn’t the specifer of {hates} and if the relevant features remain unchecked ,the derivation will crash at LF and therefore will wrongly predicate that sentence like (22) above is ungrammatical .
  • 21. The question now is how to overcome this problem? In 4.4, we outlined a possible solution, we suggested that in auxiliariless finite clauses, the grammatical features carried by the verb percolate from V to I . however, clearly we don’t want to assume that the complement-features of the verb {hates} percolate to I since these are checked internally within {VP},so let’s assume that the complement-features of {hates} are first checked internally within {VP} and erased, and then the remaining unchecked features of {hates} (i.e. its head and specifier-features ) percolate from {V} to {INFL}, as shown by the arrows in (24) where we assume that the objective complement-features of {hates} and the objective head-feature of {him} have already checked and erased internally within {VP}
  • 22. we can suppose that the {Pres} present-tense head-feature of {hates} percolate from {V} to {INFL} in order to ensure{LF}convergence ,and if we make the reasonable assumption that {INFL}is only interpretable at LF if it carries a tense-feature, so percolation of the {Pres}feature from {V} to {I} is the way that provides a way of ensuring that {INFL} acquires a tense-feature and can therefore be interpreted at LF. The assumption that the present- tense head –feature of {hates} percolates up to {INFL} accounts for the fact that the corresponding tag-sentence {she hates him, doesn’t she?}contains a present-tense auxiliary {doesn’t}since the auxiliary in a (tag )generally carries the same tense-features as the head {INFL}constituent of the clause to which the tag is attached (i.e. she has finished, hasn’t she ?) . In much the same way, we might suppose that the {3SNom} specifier-features of {hates} also percolate to {INFL} in order to ensure LF convergence: since specifier features are uninterpretable, they must be checked and erased in the course of deriving the relevant LF representation. But since {hates}and {she}are within different phrases( hates is the head V of {VP}, and she is the specifier of {IP}) the only way for the specifier-features to be checked is for them to percolate up to {INFL},where they can then be checked against the corresponding {3SNom} head-feature of {she}in that being in the same IP-projection ,so checking will result in erasure of the uninterpretable {3SNom} specifier-features of {hates} and of the uninterpretable {Nom} head-feature of {she} so after applying the checking test we will have the following diagram :
  • 23. Example (26) : She does hate him So let’s apply the checking test by erasing the specifer- and complement-features in along with nominative/objective-case head- features of she/him and the infinitival head-feature of the verb {hate} which derives the LF representation (28) below : An interesting question which arises from our discussion here is why sentences such as the following (29) below should be ungrammatical:
  • 24. Example (29): she does hates him Lets assume that (29) has the following diagram as in (30) Actually, what makes the sentence (29) to crash at LF is two reasons: 2)That the complement-features of the {does} must be {Inf} and this can’t be satisfied since the head-features carried by {hates} is not {inf} but it’s a present-tense feature . 3)The specifier-features of {does}and {hates}require them each to have a {3SNom} and the single subject pronoun {she} can’t serve as the subject of both verbs. But why not? Answer: because if the nominative-case specifier-features of {does}is checked against the nominative-case head-feature of{she}, the nominative case-feature of {she} will thereby be erased because it is unintrepretable and this will mean that the nominative specifier-feature of {hates} can not be erased even if it percolates from V to INFL because the nominative head-feature of {she} has already been erased.
  • 25. 5.6 Determiner phrases : So far we have looked at structures that mainly involved arguments which are (personal pronouns). But how do deal with nominal argument like: (31) The students are complaining. The specifier feature of {are} requires its specifier phrase (the students) to be nominative and plural or 2 person subject. The determiner {the} is the head of {DP} so in this sentence it can only bind with 3rd person reflexive anaphor not 1st or 2nd which means that it is 3rd person expression : ( 32 ) The students take themselves/*ourselves/ *yourselves too seriously . and since the whole phrase carries the head-features of its head{the}the {DP} then must be {3PNom} Note: {The}: has variable number and case properties in that, it can come with plural/singular in number and with nominative/ objective in case, so how we determined the complement feature in sentence like : (33) ? -The specifier feature of {are} which is [NomP] is checked against the head feature of the [NomP] in this particular usage of the ( it is variable ) -The reasons for giving the complement feature[PN] not [SN] to {the} are : (1)- The head {are} requires being its specifier-features {3NomP} and that is satisfied by the head-features of the {DP} (2)- Or by supposing that the number feature of students percolate from N to D.
  • 26. So let’s apply the assumption of the overt determiners as in (33)to the covert determiners(Null determiners) in sentence like : ( 34 ) Students are complaining Here{students} is a {DP} headed by a {null determiner Ǿ} which has person, number, and case properties of its own (and here it is 3rd person to agree with the students ) and which proves that is the 3rd reflexive that {students} can only bind with in sentence (35) ( 35 ) Students are preparing themselves/*ourselves/ *yourselves for exam . So the {null determiner} must be nominative plural in order to satisfy the requirement of {are} to have a nominative plural subject, that is why sentences like 34 and 33 have the same structure except that the determiner is {over} in one and {covert} in the other . Let’s now move on to see how checking is done within the internal {Determiner Phrase }and to do so let’s have a look at example (36) below ( 36 ) [The numerous opposition allegations of incompetence] are unsettling the prime minister. {Numerous} here is an attributive adjective serves as a specifier of a functional head {F}. This sentence has the structure as in diagram (37) below: Let’s now have a look at 1) number-features of {DP} 2) agreement between its constituents 1- The number feature of {DP} : since {allegations} is plural and the adjective numerous is intrinsically plural ( its meaning ) so the D{ the} must be plural in order to make the whole DP be plural to agree with the AUX{are} . ( agreement between The, numerous and allegations ) 2- (a) The agreement between {numerous} and {allegations} – the plural noun feature of allegations [P] percolates up the tree to the head F so it is checked against the number feature of {numerous}[P] as we can see in example (38)
  • 27. (b) The agreement between {allegations} and {The}: according to the assumption which says that the determiners agree with the nouns they modify the [P] feature of {allegations} percolates up from N to F and then from F to D and then it is checked against the [P] feature of the {D} the: Or we can say that the has a [P] feature by percolation from the noun allegations as we can see in 39
  • 28. 5.7 PRO subjects : As we know that {pronouns} carry case-feature whether it is overt or covert {Null}, so let’s see what the case-feature carried by the (Null PRO) in the following example (40) is. ( 40 ) They are trying [ PRO to escape]. According to Chomsky and lansik, they claim that the null-case carried by PRO is checked by {infinitival to} which means that the {infinitival to} requires its specifier in IPs like this (40) to be Null subject and therefore the specifier-feature of {to} will be checked and erased with the head-feature of the Null PRO.
  • 29. The [Nom] case of the specifier-feature of {has} is checked against the [Nom] case of the head- feature of {she}. So in both examples (41-42) checking involves a spec-head relation. The finite constituent checks nominative case and so requires a nominative subject Whereas An infinitival to checks Null case and requires a PRO subject There are some verbs that controls their IPs complements to be Null-case to like the verb {try} in the previous example (40) but there are also verbs that don’t control their IPs complements to be a Null-case to and therefore the IP complement must have an objective subject as we can see in the following examples (43a ,44a) ( 43 ) (a) They believe [him to be wrong] They believe [PRO to be wrong] ( 44 ) (a) They consider [him to have cheated] (b) They consider [PRO to have cheated] So there are two different uses of the {infinitival to} which are: 1)- When {to} checks a null case it requires a PRO subject with null case (with control verbs like try which select an IP complement headed by a null case (to) 2)- When it doesn’t check a null case it doesn't allow a PRO subject (with verbs like believe which don't allow it is IP complement to have a null -case (to).
  • 30. 5.8 Objective subject : The following examples are infinitival IPs with objective subjects like those bracketed in (45) ( 45 ) (a) They had expected [us to counterattack] (c)We don't consider [him to be suitable] (a) He wouldn't have liked [me to reconsider] (b) We are keen for [them to take a part] These kind of IPs occur only: (1) as a complement of transitive verbs like :(expect ,consider, like) (2)as a complement of complementizer{ for} . The case of the {objective subject}is then checked externally by a transitive verb or a complementizer{for}so, {us} is checked by{ expected} ,{ him} by {consider} , {me} by liked , {them} by {for} So sentences like the following(46)are ungrammatical: ( 46 ) (a) *[them to abandon syntax] would be a mistake. (b) *He may be anxious [them to make amends] (c) *Brigadier blunderbuss gave the order [them to cease fire] Because the (infinitive clauses in the brackets) are not the complement of transitive verbs or complementizer {for} .The bracketed IP in {46a} is the subject of {would}. In{46b} it is the complement of intransitive adjective (anxious}. In {46c} it is the complement of the noun {order} (as we know that nouns too are intransitive).
  • 31. The difference between the way of checking null PRO subject case and the objective subject case - The null PRO subject are checked internally (within the IP) by the null case infinitive (to). But - The case of an objective subject is checked externally by a transitive verb or complementizer(for) * Exceptional Case Marking: that the subject has it's case externally checked from outside of it's containing IP. So the complement with the objective subject is referred to as ECM complement. the verb which selects this kind of complements is called ECM-Verb. Let’s now have a look at the different ways in which the case properties of {null PRO} and {objective subjects} are checked: 1)- In active and passive structures: (47) (a) They had decided [PRO to postpone the meeting] (e)It had been decided [PRO to postpone the meeting] the verb like{decided} is a control verb which takes an IP complement headed by (null case to) in both active and passive. (48) (a) People genuinely believed [him to be innocent ] •*It was genuinely believed [him to be innocent] Whereas, a verb like {believed} can only function as an ECM verb taking Infinitive complement with an objective subject when used in active sentences because if this verb is used in passive it will be intransitive and then will not allow an objective subject but to have a null Pro case to
  • 32. 2)- Adverb position : The {adverbs} that modifying the control verbs can be positioned between the control verb and its IP as we can see in (49) below: (49) He tried hard [PRO to convince her] Unlike the adverbs modifying ECM verbs which can't be positioned between ECM -V and its IP complement as we can see in (50) below: (50) *She believes sincerely [him to be innocent] Because in(49) the adverb doesn't prevent the case of PRO from being checked (internally) but in (50) it is ungrammatical because it’s intervening between the the matrix verb and the objective subject so the transitive verb{believes}in (50) doesn’t immediately precede the pronoun {him} so the adverb(sincerely) blocks or prevents the transitive verb from giving ECM for the objective subject(him) which means that such sentence is ungrammatical .
  • 33. Note: another evidence that intervening adverbs block a verb from checking the objective case of its complement as we can see in (51) below ( 51) (a) He plays chess well (b) *He plays well chess In (51a) is grammatical because the verb precedes its complement immediately but in (51b) is not because the adverb is blocking the checking (intervening between the verb and its complement ) Conclusion : the objective case carried by the subject of an ECM infinitive is externally checked from outside the infinitive complement by an immediately preceding transitive matrix verb or complementizer and so this conclusion raise the following two results which are : - Infinitival to in ECM doesn’t check the objective case carried by the infinitive subject ,but rather is a caseless particlt . The Objective case of an ECM infinitive subject can’t be checked from inside IP by infinitival to and hence exceptionally has to be checked from outside IP by an immediately preceding transitive matrix verb or complementizer
  • 34. * There are three types of verb that take infinitival complement : 1- verbs like (hope) allows null case {to}complement with PRO subject only (intransitive) ( 52) (a) She was hoping [PRO to win] (b) *She was hoping [ him to win ] 2- verbs like (believe) allows an IP complement with an objective subject only (transitive) ( 53) (a) She believes [him to be innocent] (b) *She believes [ PRO to be innocent ] 3- verbs like (expect) which allows an Infinitival IP complement with either PRO or objective subject (can be transitive and intransitive): ( 54) (a) She expects [PRO to be win] (b) She expects [him to be win] Conclusion: The analysis of infinitival IPs presented earlier makes four crucial assumptions : 1- ) there are two different kinds of infinitival to: A) One checks null-case B) another which has no checking –case properties. 2-) there are different kinds of verbs which select different kinds of infinitive complements ( some select an IP headed by null-case to, others and IP headed by caseless to . 3- ) Some verbs are transitive that check objective-case; others are intransitive; and yet others can be either transitive or intransitive. 4- ) The case of an infinitive subject is checked externally by an immediately preceding the transitive matrix verb or complementizer in structures where it can’t be checked internally 5-) the infinitival to differ not only in case-checking properties but also in their tense properties as we can see in example(55) below: Example (55) A) Mary hope {PRO to be an actress} B) He believes{ her to be an actress} In (55) the {to-close} seems to carry different temporal properties from the hopes-clause: the hopes-clause refers to the present whereas the to-clause refers to the future, so (55a) is paraphrase able as “Mary hopes she will be an actress”. By Contrast, the to-clause in (55b) seems to have the same temporal properties as the believes-clause (both have present time reference, as we see from the fact that (55b) can be paraphrased as “He believes that she is an actress” We can conclude that: 1) {to} in control infinitives has tense properties independent of those in the matrix-clause 2) {to} in control infinitives often has future time-reference, whereas {to} in ECM infinitives has tense properties determined by those all the matrix clause
  • 35. 5.9 Bare phrases structure : Sentence like (5) will have the structure : In this structure there is a potential notational inconsistency : that the categorical properties are presented by category labels attached to terminal nodes whereas, other grammatical properties of words are presented by sets of features attached to words themselves . The fact that {she} has the categorical status of a determiner is indicated by the label D attached to terminal node carrying the word {she}whereas, that fact that {she} is third person femimine singular nominative is indicated by the bracketed [3FSNom] head- features carried by {she} To eliminate this we would incorporate the categorical properties of a word into the head features of the word (the categories labels are no longer used):and the non-terminal nodes are replaced with the heads of their constituent :
  • 36. {D} of {she} is indicated in the head feature of {she} [3FSNomD]. {I} is indicated in the head feature of {has} [ PresI]. {V} is indicated in the head feature of {gone} [V +n]. This shows the economy principle in checking theory (to proscribe the superfluous symbols in structural representations. We might go further ans ask whether the category labels carried by non-terminal nodes should also be eliminated or not since they encode redundant information. And also whether the nonterminal nodes need to carry any label or the structure can not be presented without them as we can see in the following unlabelled example (58) below :
  • 37. The argument is by saying that lexical entries make it predictable to us to know the terminal and non-terminal nodes as the following :{ has gone} is a projection of {has} not{ gone}in that {has} takes{ n- participle complement}and gone = does not allow for itself to take an AUX as its specifier and {she has gone} is not the projection of {she} since it doesn't allow specifier of any kind.
  • 38. 5.10 Summary: This chapter discussed how grammatical features are checked and the PF representation (phonetic form) and LF presentation (Logical form). In 5.2 it handled the principles of full interpretation (PFI) that requires the Pf representation to contain only phonetic features and the LF representation to contain only sematic features so the derivation will be converge not crash. According to this chapter some grammatical features are interpretable and some are not and those which aren’t must be checked (erased). In 5.3 it handled the idea that words carry three sets of grammatical features which are head feature, specifier feature and complement feature and the features which are not interpretable are to be erased and checked in checking process. In 5.4 the chapter discussed how checking works in phrasal specifier and complement and how projections carry the head features of their head. In 5.5 it discussed the problem of finite non auxiliaries verbs and how its specifier features percolate up to the unfilled I so it can be checked with the head feature of its specifier. In 5.6 we discussed how features carried by determiner, attributive adjectives and nouns are checked. 5.8 handled the syntax of exceptional case-marking containing an infinitival IP with an objective subject. In 5.9 we discussed the fact that the category labels carried by terminal nodes can be eliminated and inserted in terms of head feature. Also the nonterminal nodes can be eliminated too so we develop a bare phrase structure ( unlabeled tree diagram ).