SlideShare a Scribd company logo
3 EVER-PRESENT ISSUES IN
PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGICAL RESEARCH
• BIAS
• CONFOUNDING
• CAUSALITY
BIAS & CONFOUNDING
 Major objective of pharmacoepidemiological research is to estimate drugs’
effects (when prescribed) after marketing.
 Drug exposure:
 is not a stable phenomenon;
 may be associated with factors related to the outcome of interest (e.g.,
indication for prescribing, patient compliance, publicity, and natural
course of the disease).
 Challenge of pharmacoepidemiological research is to obtain an accurate
estimate (without error), of the relationship between drug exposure and
health status.
 2 types of errors:
• Random error is related to precision and reliability,
• Systematic error is related to validity and bias.
[Accuracy is the absence of both random and systematic error].
Bias, confounding and causality in p'coepidemiological research
1981 – National Childhood Encephalopathy Study (NCES)
 Results presented by Alderslade and Miller;
 A nationwide case–control study conducted in the UK by the
Committee on Safety of Medicines and the Joint Committee on
Vaccination and Immunization.
 Research Question: Any possible association between DTP vaccine
and the subsequent development of neurologic disorders?
 Findings:
 Risk of a severe acute neurologic event was significantly increased
within the seven days following DTP vaccine.
[RR 2.3; 95%(CI) 1.4–3.2],
 One year later, of the 241 cases in whom the disorder had begun
within the seven days following a DTP vaccine, 7 (2.9%) cases either
died or had a developmental deficit .
 In controls: only 3 of 478 controls (0.6%) died or had developmental
deficit.
 These results were used in many court trials by parents of disabled
children who were seeking compensation.
BUT…..
 Credibility of the study was compromised by suspicions of bias.
 Numerous potential biases were identified and were responsible
(either fully or partially), for the results observed.
 Referral bias: physicians were aware of the study objectives and this
might have influenced their referral of cases and increased the
apparent relative risk.
 Information bias:
• interviewers were not blinded to study objectives, subjects’ clinical
status;
• date of onset of the neurological disorder was occasionally difficult
to establish (potentially increasing the apparent relative risk).
 Protopathic bias: possible presence of subclinical neurological
disease prior to vaccination, could have falsely increased the relative
risk.
 Lack of precise disease definitions and inclusion criteria thought not
related to DTP vaccine, caused misinterpretation of results (Reye’s
syndrome, hypsarrhythmia, or acute viral encephalopathies).
 Issues in study design can affect the validity of results in
pharmacoepidemiology research.
 Pharmacoepidemiology studies may be affected by particular biases
more often than other epidemiologic studies.
BIASES IN PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGY
 3 categories:
 Selection bias (related to the recruitment of study subjects or loss of
study subjects in follow-up)
 Information bias (related to the accuracy of information collected on
exposure, health status);
 Confounding (covariates or effect modifiers related to the
pathophysiology of disease development, whereby one factor (or
several factors acting together) can produce an observed effect that
may be incorrectly attributed to an exposure of interest).
SELECTION BIAS (Sample distortion bias)
 Due to the selection (inclusion) of groups of subjects into the study
who differ in characteristics from those in the target population,
causing distortion of the measurement of an effect (outcome).
 4 types of selection bias:
 Referral bias
 Self-selection
 Prevalence study bias
 Protopathic bias
REFERRAL BIAS
 Can occur if the reasons for referring a patient by the physician to the
study are related to the patient’s exposure to (use of) the drug.
 Problematic when an illness presents in such a manner that an
accurate diagnosis is not always obtained.
 E.g.1, Hospital ‘XYZ’ with 2 groups of patients:
• Group 1: 1000 patients on NSAIDs presenting w/ abdominal pain
may be more likely to be suspected as having a GU.
• Group 2: 10 patients with similar pain who are not using NSAIDs
• Group1 patients are more likely to be tested for GU than Group2.
• A study using these patients in Hospital XYZ will show a strong, but
biased, association between mild non-bleeding GUs and NSAIDs
use.
WHY??????
 E.g.2, Association b/n DVT and oral contraceptives
• The association b/n DVT and oral contraceptives is already well
known.
• The use of oral contraceptives is a vital factor in this study.
• Exposed women (women on oral contraceptives) may be more likely
to be tested for DVT than women not on oral contraceptives.
• Earlier studies reporting a positive association b/n drug (oral
contraceptives) and disease (DVT) can begin the referral bias
phenomenon.
*** Identifying the potential for referral bias in initial stages of any study
is important for that study, as well as for future similar studies.
SELF-SELECTION BIAS
 May occur when study participants themselves decide to participate
in, or to leave a study (based on drug exposure effects, change in
health status of participants, personal reasons).
 So, the association observed in the study sample may not be
representative of the real association in the source population.
 This bias is very important in case–control studies or cohort studies.
 E.g., Association b/n drugs used during pregnancy and birth defects
• 2 groups:
• Group 1: mothers of ‘affected’ children, who used medications during
pregnancy.
• Group 2: mothers of ‘normal’ children, who used medications during
pregnancy.
• Group 1 will be more willing to participate in the study than group 2.
• Solution: systematically identify and recruit all eligible cases (for both
groups).
 Losses to follow-up (study participants dropping out) in cohort studies
can also induce bias, if those who drop out belong to a special
disease–exposure category (those who fulfill the Inclusion criteria).
Solution: Use population-based registries
PREVALENCE BIAS
 A type of selection bias that may occur in case–control studies when
prevalent cases (rather than new cases) are selected for a study.
 Prevalence is proportional to both incidence and duration of the
disease (But, it is related more to the duration of the disease rather
than to the incidence).
 In a group of incidence cases, significant association with prevalent
cases might not be confirmed.
 Recruiting only incident cases with recent documented data is
relevant only to disease incidence, not to prevalence.
PROTOPATHIC BIAS
 Feinstein (1985) – may occur “if a particular treatment or exposure
was started, stopped, or otherwise changed because of the baseline
manifestation caused by another disease or other factor.”
 If some other disease or risk factor produces the same symptoms or
signs that the researcher is analysing.
 E.g., Studying the association between blood in stool as an indicator
for colorectal cancer.
 BUT… excessive use of aspirin can also cause blood in stool
 Haemorrhoids cause blood in stool.
INFORMATION AND MISCLASSIFICATION BIAS
 Errors can occur if cases in a study are classified with regard to their
exposure and disease status…..
• unexposed people may be considered exposed and vice versa.
• health status may also be incorrectly classified.
 This type of error may lead to a misclassification bias.
 Equally affects case–control and cohort studies.
 Non-differential misclassification:
• When the misclassification error occurs randomly (i.e., independent
of the exposure–outcome relationship).
• Mostly occurs if study instrument is not very reliable.
• It may lead to a decrease in the strength of the association between
drug and outcome (bias toward the null hypothesis)
Exposure timing
• Inaccuracy in properly defining the exposure time can result in
information bias which may lead to a non-significant association
overall, even when there is a very strong association between the
drug and the outcome, within a specific time window.
• E.g., Anaphylactic reactions occur rapidly after drug exposure, very
high risk during this short time period, and null after this initial period.
• The risk mostly decreases with time.
• E.g., Sometimes, chronic long-term users of NSAIDs are likely to be
at a lower risk of gastrointestinal bleeding than new users, because
of a ‘survivor effect’.
• Sometimes, the risk steadily increases with time, due to the
cumulative effect of drug exposure
• E.g., risk of myocardial toxicity after the use of doxorubicin.
Differential misclassification:
 When this error is influenced by knowledge of the exposure (drug /
disease) and the outcome status.
 E.g.1, during data collection in cohort studies, knowledge of the
exposure influences the quality of the information collected
 E.g.2, in case–control studies when knowledge of the disease status
influences the quality of the information collected about exposure, it is
also called information bias.
 2 situations: Differential recall bias and Differential detection bias.
Differential Recall bias:
• mostly seen in retrospective studies,
• in case–control studies, cases and controls may have a selective
memory of their past exposures.
• E.g., In studies of birth defects, mothers with an impaired child may
give a more valid and complete report of their exposure to drugs
during pregnancy as a result of devoting more time to contemplating
the cause of the birth defect.
• This type of bias may be minimized by selecting controls who are
likely to have the same cognitive processes affecting memory of past
drug exposures.
Differential detection bias:
• can affect either cohort or case–control studies.
• In case–control studies: occurs when the procedures for exposure
assessment is more thorough among cases than controls.
• In cohort studies: occurs mostly due to difference in the follow-up for
detecting adverse events.
• E.g., women taking postmenopausal hormonal supplements are likely
to see their doctors more often than other women. They are more
likely to be examined for breast or endometrial cancer, or risk of CV
disease.
• This may lead to an excess number of diagnosed diseases in the
‘treated’ group (women who took postmenopausal hormonal
supplements) and a falsely elevated risk
CONFOUNDING
 Occurs when the association between drug exposure and health
status is distorted by the effect of one or several extraneous
variables that are also risk factors for the outcome of interest.
 E.g., Study of relationship b/n use of NSAIDs and GU
• Potential Confounders: Personal Hx of GU in patient, Chronic
alcoholism.
 For a variable to be a confounder, it must be associated with both the
drug exposure and the outcome of interest.
Study: Death associated with use of Drug A;
Comparison group: Patients treated with Drug B
Study: Risk of allergy associated with use of a drug;
Comparison group: Patients not exposed (treated with) the drug.
EFFECT
MODIFICATION
Confounding by Indication for Prescription
 Synonyms: Indication bias, Contraindication bias, Channeling,
Confounding by severity.
 Indication for a prescription is the most important confounder in
pharmacoepidemiological research. WHY????
 Because there is always a ‘reason’ for a prescription and the reason
is often associated with the outcome of interest.
 Can induce selection bias in drug efficacy studies.
 Difficult to control.
 Often impossible to obtain a sufficiently accurate estimate of the
confounder’s effect.
 Miettinen (1983) – preventive use of warfarin was associated with a
27-fold increase in the risk of thrombotic events (conditions that
should actually be prevented by warfarin). This paradoxical result was
because only highly susceptible patients, or those already
experiencing the first symptoms of thrombosis were included in the
study.
Confounding by Comedication and Other Cofactors
 Patients often take more than one drug at a time and it is sometimes
difficult to isolate the effect of a specific drug, in research studies.
 Coronary Drug Project (1980) –the risks of death in placebo group
after 5 years were 15% (compliant cases) and 28.2% (non-compliant
cases).
Potential Confounders: Selection bias; patients compliant with one
drug were very likely to be compliant with other interventions (e.g.,
other very effective drugs, diet, physical exercise, etc.).
SOLUTIONS FOR SELECTION BIAS
 Must be prevented at the design stage, because it cannot be
corrected at the analysis stage.
 Selection bias can result in over- or under-representation of the
people who have a drug exposure–outcome relationship.
 Strategies:
• Random sampling of the cases and controls from the source
population.
• Systematically recruiting a series of consecutive subjects (to
prevent self-selection).
• Minimizing the number of subjects lost to follow-up in cohort
studies.
• Tracking procedure for drop-out cases (to identify the reasons).
• Selecting only incident cases of the condition.
• Assigning random allocation of drug exposure – Follow the
procedure (to prevent self-selection and referral bias).
SOLUTIONS FOR INFORMATION BIAS
 Must be resolved at the design stage.
 Strategies:
• Blinding (or masking) of relevant study personnel;
• Standardization of the measurement process for both cases and
controls
• use of standard structured questionnaires,
• specific training of interviewers,
• different observers for different measurements
• Standardized criteria for defining drug exposure and disease
outcomes.
SOLUTIONS FOR CONFOUNDING
 It is possible to control the effect of confounding at both the design
and the analysis levels.
 Strategies:
@ Design level:
• Randomization,
• Matching – ensure similarities in both case and control groups; be
cautious of ‘over matching’,
• Restrict confounding by studying only one level – e.g., studying
the drug effect among only one category of age will prevent against
the occurrence of confounding by age.
@ Analysis levels:
• Standardization
• Stratification
CAUSALITY
• Cause: a stimulus that produces an effect or outcome.
• Change in host-agent-environmental balance produces an
effect or outcome.
• Cause (statistical definition): a factor which varies (either
proportional or inversely proportional) to the health condition
of interest (health condition studied).
Statistical relationship
• E.g., a disease with risk factors X, Y and Z
• Is there a ‘statistical relationship’ b/n factors X and Y?
• Meaning: any association b/n X and Y occurs by chance or not
by chance (greater frequency than that by chance?)
• Next step: tests for independence or association by Chi-square
test or Correlation coefficient test
• If the test results are statistically significant, then X and Y are
not independent, but have an association that is not entirely
due to chance.
• Study: rates of developing complications after mastectomy for
women with and without anxious personalities.
• Test Result: Chi-square test is statistically significant at p<0.05
• Meaning: 95 times out of 100, this difference in complication
rates between the two samples is not due to chance.
• Inference: Personality and complication rates are not
independent.
• They have a significant statistical association.
• BUT….. This does not mean that only those with anxious
personality type suffer from complications post-mastectomy.
SAMPLE COMPLICATION RATES
(after mastectomy)
Women with anxious
personality
100 nos.
Women without anxious
personality
40 nos.
• It means that a woman with anxious personality is more
likely to suffer from complications after mastectomy than a
woman without anxious personality.
• Determining the ‘statistical significant association’ is the first
step in determining whether the relationship is causal.
Causal relationship
• Statistically significant factors (non-independent) can have non-
causal relationship or causal relationship.
Non-causal relationship
• Hypothetical variable varies with actual causal variable
• E.g., Relation b/n paternal age and infant birth weight
• paternal age (Hypothetical variable)
• Maternal weight (actual causal variable )and infant birth weight
• Even if there is a statistical significant relation b/n paternal age
and infant birth weight, there isn’t any logical explanation.
Causal relationships
• 2 types: a) direct and b) indirect
a) Direct causal relationship
• A factor directly causes a disease with no other intervening
factor.
• Causal factor Outcome
• E.g., Tubercle bacilli TB
• Sometimes, what is considered a direct causal association may
later on be identified as indirect causal association.
• E.g.1, Cholera outbreak in England (1883) – Dr. John Snow –
identified certain water sources as the causative factor;
Drinking water from those sources was banned. Later on, it was
identified that V.cholerae was the causative factor. Water was
only the vector.
E.g.2, Toxic Shock Syndrome (TSS)
• When TSS first emerged, clinicians identified tampons as the
causative factor.
• Later on, Staphylococcal spp. was identified as the causative
factor.
• Tampons were the vector (indirect contributory cause).
• Education programmes aimed at eliminating the use of
tampons; changing the way tampons were used;
• Women were advised to avoid super-absorbent tampons,
change tampons frequently, practice good hygiene.
b) Indirect causal relationships
• Extra variable/s (intervening variables) occupies/y an
intermediate stage b/n cause and effect.
• A B C D
• ‘A’ is causally associated with ‘D’, only after the interposition of
variables ‘B’ and ‘C’.
• E.g., Relationship b/n cigarette smoke and chronic bronchitis
Breathing the air polluted by cigarette smoke (A)
Damage to the respiratory epithelium (B)
Increased susceptibility of respiratory epithelium to infection (C)
Chronic bronchitis (D)
Multiple causative factors
• Diseases have many risk factors, all of which are involved in
development of the disease.
• Exposure to multiple causative factors can have additive or
multiplicative effect.
• E.g.1, Even though smoking is a major cause of lung cancer, it
is not the only cause. Non-smokers (either active or passive)
can also get lung cancer.
• Risk of cancer is higher among non-smokers exposed to
asbestos than smokers not exposed to asbestos.
• E.g.2, Automobile accidents can occur due to speeding, faulty
equipments, heavy traffic, poor visibility, driver’s inexperience,
drinking etc.
• Web of causation: association b/n all the causative factors
which have impact on the risk of developing a disease.
Establishing causality
• Through epidemiological studies and clinical trials.
• A factor is considered causal, when reducing it’s amount or
frequency, reduces the frequency of the effect.
• E.g., If treating hypertensive patients (keeping their BP low) can
reduce the frequency of stroke when compared to an
equivalent , untreated group of hypertensive patients, then HTN
is considered a risk factor for stroke (HTN is a causal factor for
stroke).
Naranjo Algorithm (ADR) Probability Scale
• A method by which to assess whether there is a causal relationship
between a DRUG and ADR.
• Use a simple questionnaire (10 qns.) to assign probability scores.
• Answers are either Yes, No, or “Do not know”.
• Different point values (-1, 0, +1 or +2) are assigned to each answer.
• Total scores range from -4 to +13;
– Definite reaction: 9 to 13; Probable reaction: 5 to 8;
– Possible reaction: 1 to 4; Doubtful reaction: 0 or less.
• The response ‘Do not know’ should be used
– sparingly;
– only when the quality of the data does not permit a ‘Yes’ or ‘No’
answer.
– if the information is not available
– if the question is inapplicable to the case.
• When more than one drug is involved or suspected,
• the ADR Probability Scale is applied separately to each of the
possible etiologic agents (drugs);
• the drug with the highest score should be considered the
causative agent;
• the potential of interaction should be evaluated;
Question Yes No
Do Not
Know
Score
1. Are there previous conclusive reports on
this reaction?
+1 0 0
2. Did the adverse event appear after the
suspected drug was administered?
+2 -1 0
3. Did the adverse event improve when the
drug was discontinued or a specific
antagonist was administered?
+1 0 0
4. Did the adverse event reappear when the
drug was readministered?
+2 -1 0
5. Are there alternative causes that could on
their own have caused the reaction?
-1 +2 0
Question Yes No
Do Not
Know
Score
6. Did the reaction reappear when a placebo was
given?
-1 +1 0
7. Was the drug detected in blood or other fluids
in concentrations known to be toxic?
+1 0 0
8. Was the reaction more severe when the dose
was increased or less severe when the dose was
decreased?
+1 0 0
9. Did the patient have a similar reaction to the
same or similar drugs in any previous exposure?
+1 0 0
10. Was the adverse event confirmed by any
objective evidence?
+1 0 0
Total Score:
THANK YOU!!!
Courtesy: Textbook of Pharmacoepidemiology by
Strom and Kimmel

More Related Content

PPTX
Cross sectional study-dr.wah
PPTX
Analyzing the randomised control trial (rct)
PPTX
Bias and errors
PPTX
Bias in epidemiology uploaded
PPTX
Nested case control,
PPTX
Biases in epidemiology
Cross sectional study-dr.wah
Analyzing the randomised control trial (rct)
Bias and errors
Bias in epidemiology uploaded
Nested case control,
Biases in epidemiology

What's hot (20)

PPT
Part 2 Cox Regression
PPTX
Confounding and interaction seminar
PPTX
Error, confounding and bias
PPTX
Confounder and effect modification
PPTX
Cross sectional study
PPTX
cohort study
PPTX
Randomisation
PPTX
Error, bias and confounding
PDF
Cross sectional study
PDF
Cross sectional studies
PPT
05 intervention studies
PPTX
Cross sectional study by Dr Abhishek Kumar
PDF
Bias and Confounding
PPTX
Cross sectional study
PPTX
DOCX
Adaptive method OR dosing with feedback
PPTX
Cross-Sectional Study.pptx
PPTX
3.5.1 information bias
 
PPTX
Odds ratio and confidence interval
PPTX
Nested case control study
Part 2 Cox Regression
Confounding and interaction seminar
Error, confounding and bias
Confounder and effect modification
Cross sectional study
cohort study
Randomisation
Error, bias and confounding
Cross sectional study
Cross sectional studies
05 intervention studies
Cross sectional study by Dr Abhishek Kumar
Bias and Confounding
Cross sectional study
Adaptive method OR dosing with feedback
Cross-Sectional Study.pptx
3.5.1 information bias
 
Odds ratio and confidence interval
Nested case control study
Ad

Viewers also liked (10)

PPTX
Bias and confounding
PPTX
5.2.2 dags for confounding
 
PDF
4 Threats to validity from confounding bias and effect modification
 
PPTX
Confounding and Directed Acyclic Graphs
PPTX
Bias in health research
PPT
Errors and Error Measurements
PPTX
Errors in research
PPTX
4.3.1. controlling confounding matching
 
PPSX
Bias, confounding and fallacies in epidemiology
PPTX
Presentation on bias and confouinding
Bias and confounding
5.2.2 dags for confounding
 
4 Threats to validity from confounding bias and effect modification
 
Confounding and Directed Acyclic Graphs
Bias in health research
Errors and Error Measurements
Errors in research
4.3.1. controlling confounding matching
 
Bias, confounding and fallacies in epidemiology
Presentation on bias and confouinding
Ad

Similar to Bias, confounding and causality in p'coepidemiological research (20)

PDF
COMMON BIASES IN PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGICAL RESEARCH.pdf
PPTX
Bias and confounder
PPTX
Validity and bias in epidemiological study
PPTX
Seminar on Bias, Confounding, and Interaction.pptx
PPTX
BIAS_Lecture_updated explaining different types of bias
PPTX
BIASES IN EPIDEMOLODY-1.pptx
PPT
Judgment of causality2 kaleab
PPTX
Bias advanced presentation.pptx
PPTX
Bias in medical research and journal.2.pptx
PPTX
Bias confounders
PPTX
12. Introduction to bias.pptx
PDF
Bias and confounding
PPT
Bias and error.final(1).ppt
PPTX
Bias, Confounding, and Interaction.pptx
PPT
BIAS AND CHANCE AND CONFOUNDING
PPTX
Types of bias
PPTX
Etiology Research.pptx
PPTX
10 Evaluation of evidence for epidemiology purpose
PPTX
Bias in clinical research
PPTX
Bias, confounding and error part bias.pptx
COMMON BIASES IN PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGICAL RESEARCH.pdf
Bias and confounder
Validity and bias in epidemiological study
Seminar on Bias, Confounding, and Interaction.pptx
BIAS_Lecture_updated explaining different types of bias
BIASES IN EPIDEMOLODY-1.pptx
Judgment of causality2 kaleab
Bias advanced presentation.pptx
Bias in medical research and journal.2.pptx
Bias confounders
12. Introduction to bias.pptx
Bias and confounding
Bias and error.final(1).ppt
Bias, Confounding, and Interaction.pptx
BIAS AND CHANCE AND CONFOUNDING
Types of bias
Etiology Research.pptx
10 Evaluation of evidence for epidemiology purpose
Bias in clinical research
Bias, confounding and error part bias.pptx

More from samthamby79 (20)

PDF
THE 7-STAR PHARMACIST.pdf
PDF
VARIOUS LAB TESTS INTERPRETATION - AN INTRO..pdf
PDF
INTERPRETATION OF RENAL FUNCTION TESTS.pdf
PDF
INTERPRETATION OF HEPATIC FUNCTION TESTS.pdf
PDF
INTERPRETATION OF PFTs.pdf
PDF
HAEMATOLOGICAL TESTS INTERPRETATION.pdf
PDF
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR ISCHEMIC STROKE [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PDF
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR ANXIETY & AFFECTIVE DISORDERS [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PDF
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR SCHIZOPHRENIA [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PDF
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR ATHEROSCLEROSIS [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PDF
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR HTN (MALAYSIAN CPGs).pdf
PDF
Interpretation of Clinical Lab Data [PFTs] for Newbies.pdf
PDF
Interpretation of Clinical Lab Data [CARDIAC] for newbies.pdf
PDF
TDM Pointers - Salicylates & Paracetamol Poisoning.pdf
PDF
TDM POINTERS [GERIATIC & PAEDIATRIC PATIENTS].pdf
PDF
Dosage adjustment in Hepatic Failure.pdf
PDF
Drug Dosing in Renal Failure.pdf
PDF
BIOAVAILABILITY IN A NUTSHELL.pdf
PDF
GENERIC AND SPECIFIC INSTRUMENTS IN PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGICAL RESEARCH.pdf
PPT
Drug Distribution: Pointers for newbies
THE 7-STAR PHARMACIST.pdf
VARIOUS LAB TESTS INTERPRETATION - AN INTRO..pdf
INTERPRETATION OF RENAL FUNCTION TESTS.pdf
INTERPRETATION OF HEPATIC FUNCTION TESTS.pdf
INTERPRETATION OF PFTs.pdf
HAEMATOLOGICAL TESTS INTERPRETATION.pdf
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR ISCHEMIC STROKE [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR ANXIETY & AFFECTIVE DISORDERS [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR SCHIZOPHRENIA [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR ATHEROSCLEROSIS [MALAYSIAN CPGs].pdf
PHARMACOTHERAPY POINTERS FOR HTN (MALAYSIAN CPGs).pdf
Interpretation of Clinical Lab Data [PFTs] for Newbies.pdf
Interpretation of Clinical Lab Data [CARDIAC] for newbies.pdf
TDM Pointers - Salicylates & Paracetamol Poisoning.pdf
TDM POINTERS [GERIATIC & PAEDIATRIC PATIENTS].pdf
Dosage adjustment in Hepatic Failure.pdf
Drug Dosing in Renal Failure.pdf
BIOAVAILABILITY IN A NUTSHELL.pdf
GENERIC AND SPECIFIC INSTRUMENTS IN PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGICAL RESEARCH.pdf
Drug Distribution: Pointers for newbies

Recently uploaded (20)

PDF
Dermatology diseases Index August 2025.pdf
PDF
Dr Masood Ahmed Expertise And Sucess Story
PPTX
Galactosemia pathophysiology, clinical features, investigation and treatment ...
PPT
KULIAH UG WANITA Prof Endang 121110 (1).ppt
PDF
CHAPTER 9 MEETING SAFETY NEEDS FOR OLDER ADULTS.pdf
PDF
2E-Learning-Together...PICS-PCISF con.pdf
PPTX
community services team project 2(4).pptx
PPTX
General Pharmacology by Nandini Ratne, Nagpur College of Pharmacy, Hingna Roa...
PPTX
First Aid and Basic Life Support Training.pptx
PPTX
Importance of Immediate Response (1).pptx
PDF
Dr. Jasvant Modi - Passionate About Philanthropy
PPTX
Infection prevention and control for medical students
PPTX
1. Drug Distribution System.pptt b pharmacy
PPTX
Rheumatic heart diseases with Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus
PPTX
PE and Health 7 Quarter 3 Lesson 1 Day 3,4 and 5.pptx
PPTX
Current Treatment Of Heart Failure By Dr Masood Ahmed
PPTX
CBT FOR OCD TREATMENT WITHOUT MEDICATION
PPTX
Bronchial_Asthma_in_acute_exacerbation_.pptx
PPTX
NUTRITIONAL PROBLEMS, CHANGES NEEDED TO PREVENT MALNUTRITION
PPT
Recent advances in Diagnosis of Autoimmune Disorders
Dermatology diseases Index August 2025.pdf
Dr Masood Ahmed Expertise And Sucess Story
Galactosemia pathophysiology, clinical features, investigation and treatment ...
KULIAH UG WANITA Prof Endang 121110 (1).ppt
CHAPTER 9 MEETING SAFETY NEEDS FOR OLDER ADULTS.pdf
2E-Learning-Together...PICS-PCISF con.pdf
community services team project 2(4).pptx
General Pharmacology by Nandini Ratne, Nagpur College of Pharmacy, Hingna Roa...
First Aid and Basic Life Support Training.pptx
Importance of Immediate Response (1).pptx
Dr. Jasvant Modi - Passionate About Philanthropy
Infection prevention and control for medical students
1. Drug Distribution System.pptt b pharmacy
Rheumatic heart diseases with Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus
PE and Health 7 Quarter 3 Lesson 1 Day 3,4 and 5.pptx
Current Treatment Of Heart Failure By Dr Masood Ahmed
CBT FOR OCD TREATMENT WITHOUT MEDICATION
Bronchial_Asthma_in_acute_exacerbation_.pptx
NUTRITIONAL PROBLEMS, CHANGES NEEDED TO PREVENT MALNUTRITION
Recent advances in Diagnosis of Autoimmune Disorders

Bias, confounding and causality in p'coepidemiological research

  • 1. 3 EVER-PRESENT ISSUES IN PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGICAL RESEARCH • BIAS • CONFOUNDING • CAUSALITY
  • 3.  Major objective of pharmacoepidemiological research is to estimate drugs’ effects (when prescribed) after marketing.  Drug exposure:  is not a stable phenomenon;  may be associated with factors related to the outcome of interest (e.g., indication for prescribing, patient compliance, publicity, and natural course of the disease).  Challenge of pharmacoepidemiological research is to obtain an accurate estimate (without error), of the relationship between drug exposure and health status.  2 types of errors: • Random error is related to precision and reliability, • Systematic error is related to validity and bias. [Accuracy is the absence of both random and systematic error].
  • 5. 1981 – National Childhood Encephalopathy Study (NCES)  Results presented by Alderslade and Miller;  A nationwide case–control study conducted in the UK by the Committee on Safety of Medicines and the Joint Committee on Vaccination and Immunization.  Research Question: Any possible association between DTP vaccine and the subsequent development of neurologic disorders?  Findings:  Risk of a severe acute neurologic event was significantly increased within the seven days following DTP vaccine. [RR 2.3; 95%(CI) 1.4–3.2],  One year later, of the 241 cases in whom the disorder had begun within the seven days following a DTP vaccine, 7 (2.9%) cases either died or had a developmental deficit .  In controls: only 3 of 478 controls (0.6%) died or had developmental deficit.
  • 6.  These results were used in many court trials by parents of disabled children who were seeking compensation. BUT…..  Credibility of the study was compromised by suspicions of bias.  Numerous potential biases were identified and were responsible (either fully or partially), for the results observed.  Referral bias: physicians were aware of the study objectives and this might have influenced their referral of cases and increased the apparent relative risk.  Information bias: • interviewers were not blinded to study objectives, subjects’ clinical status; • date of onset of the neurological disorder was occasionally difficult to establish (potentially increasing the apparent relative risk).
  • 7.  Protopathic bias: possible presence of subclinical neurological disease prior to vaccination, could have falsely increased the relative risk.  Lack of precise disease definitions and inclusion criteria thought not related to DTP vaccine, caused misinterpretation of results (Reye’s syndrome, hypsarrhythmia, or acute viral encephalopathies).  Issues in study design can affect the validity of results in pharmacoepidemiology research.  Pharmacoepidemiology studies may be affected by particular biases more often than other epidemiologic studies.
  • 8. BIASES IN PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGY  3 categories:  Selection bias (related to the recruitment of study subjects or loss of study subjects in follow-up)  Information bias (related to the accuracy of information collected on exposure, health status);  Confounding (covariates or effect modifiers related to the pathophysiology of disease development, whereby one factor (or several factors acting together) can produce an observed effect that may be incorrectly attributed to an exposure of interest).
  • 9. SELECTION BIAS (Sample distortion bias)  Due to the selection (inclusion) of groups of subjects into the study who differ in characteristics from those in the target population, causing distortion of the measurement of an effect (outcome).  4 types of selection bias:  Referral bias  Self-selection  Prevalence study bias  Protopathic bias
  • 10. REFERRAL BIAS  Can occur if the reasons for referring a patient by the physician to the study are related to the patient’s exposure to (use of) the drug.  Problematic when an illness presents in such a manner that an accurate diagnosis is not always obtained.  E.g.1, Hospital ‘XYZ’ with 2 groups of patients: • Group 1: 1000 patients on NSAIDs presenting w/ abdominal pain may be more likely to be suspected as having a GU. • Group 2: 10 patients with similar pain who are not using NSAIDs • Group1 patients are more likely to be tested for GU than Group2. • A study using these patients in Hospital XYZ will show a strong, but biased, association between mild non-bleeding GUs and NSAIDs use. WHY??????
  • 11.  E.g.2, Association b/n DVT and oral contraceptives • The association b/n DVT and oral contraceptives is already well known. • The use of oral contraceptives is a vital factor in this study. • Exposed women (women on oral contraceptives) may be more likely to be tested for DVT than women not on oral contraceptives. • Earlier studies reporting a positive association b/n drug (oral contraceptives) and disease (DVT) can begin the referral bias phenomenon. *** Identifying the potential for referral bias in initial stages of any study is important for that study, as well as for future similar studies.
  • 12. SELF-SELECTION BIAS  May occur when study participants themselves decide to participate in, or to leave a study (based on drug exposure effects, change in health status of participants, personal reasons).  So, the association observed in the study sample may not be representative of the real association in the source population.  This bias is very important in case–control studies or cohort studies.  E.g., Association b/n drugs used during pregnancy and birth defects • 2 groups: • Group 1: mothers of ‘affected’ children, who used medications during pregnancy. • Group 2: mothers of ‘normal’ children, who used medications during pregnancy. • Group 1 will be more willing to participate in the study than group 2. • Solution: systematically identify and recruit all eligible cases (for both groups).
  • 13.  Losses to follow-up (study participants dropping out) in cohort studies can also induce bias, if those who drop out belong to a special disease–exposure category (those who fulfill the Inclusion criteria). Solution: Use population-based registries
  • 14. PREVALENCE BIAS  A type of selection bias that may occur in case–control studies when prevalent cases (rather than new cases) are selected for a study.  Prevalence is proportional to both incidence and duration of the disease (But, it is related more to the duration of the disease rather than to the incidence).  In a group of incidence cases, significant association with prevalent cases might not be confirmed.  Recruiting only incident cases with recent documented data is relevant only to disease incidence, not to prevalence.
  • 15. PROTOPATHIC BIAS  Feinstein (1985) – may occur “if a particular treatment or exposure was started, stopped, or otherwise changed because of the baseline manifestation caused by another disease or other factor.”  If some other disease or risk factor produces the same symptoms or signs that the researcher is analysing.  E.g., Studying the association between blood in stool as an indicator for colorectal cancer.  BUT… excessive use of aspirin can also cause blood in stool  Haemorrhoids cause blood in stool.
  • 16. INFORMATION AND MISCLASSIFICATION BIAS  Errors can occur if cases in a study are classified with regard to their exposure and disease status….. • unexposed people may be considered exposed and vice versa. • health status may also be incorrectly classified.  This type of error may lead to a misclassification bias.  Equally affects case–control and cohort studies.  Non-differential misclassification: • When the misclassification error occurs randomly (i.e., independent of the exposure–outcome relationship). • Mostly occurs if study instrument is not very reliable. • It may lead to a decrease in the strength of the association between drug and outcome (bias toward the null hypothesis)
  • 17. Exposure timing • Inaccuracy in properly defining the exposure time can result in information bias which may lead to a non-significant association overall, even when there is a very strong association between the drug and the outcome, within a specific time window. • E.g., Anaphylactic reactions occur rapidly after drug exposure, very high risk during this short time period, and null after this initial period. • The risk mostly decreases with time. • E.g., Sometimes, chronic long-term users of NSAIDs are likely to be at a lower risk of gastrointestinal bleeding than new users, because of a ‘survivor effect’. • Sometimes, the risk steadily increases with time, due to the cumulative effect of drug exposure • E.g., risk of myocardial toxicity after the use of doxorubicin.
  • 18. Differential misclassification:  When this error is influenced by knowledge of the exposure (drug / disease) and the outcome status.  E.g.1, during data collection in cohort studies, knowledge of the exposure influences the quality of the information collected  E.g.2, in case–control studies when knowledge of the disease status influences the quality of the information collected about exposure, it is also called information bias.  2 situations: Differential recall bias and Differential detection bias. Differential Recall bias: • mostly seen in retrospective studies, • in case–control studies, cases and controls may have a selective memory of their past exposures. • E.g., In studies of birth defects, mothers with an impaired child may give a more valid and complete report of their exposure to drugs during pregnancy as a result of devoting more time to contemplating the cause of the birth defect.
  • 19. • This type of bias may be minimized by selecting controls who are likely to have the same cognitive processes affecting memory of past drug exposures. Differential detection bias: • can affect either cohort or case–control studies. • In case–control studies: occurs when the procedures for exposure assessment is more thorough among cases than controls. • In cohort studies: occurs mostly due to difference in the follow-up for detecting adverse events. • E.g., women taking postmenopausal hormonal supplements are likely to see their doctors more often than other women. They are more likely to be examined for breast or endometrial cancer, or risk of CV disease. • This may lead to an excess number of diagnosed diseases in the ‘treated’ group (women who took postmenopausal hormonal supplements) and a falsely elevated risk
  • 20. CONFOUNDING  Occurs when the association between drug exposure and health status is distorted by the effect of one or several extraneous variables that are also risk factors for the outcome of interest.  E.g., Study of relationship b/n use of NSAIDs and GU • Potential Confounders: Personal Hx of GU in patient, Chronic alcoholism.  For a variable to be a confounder, it must be associated with both the drug exposure and the outcome of interest.
  • 21. Study: Death associated with use of Drug A; Comparison group: Patients treated with Drug B
  • 22. Study: Risk of allergy associated with use of a drug; Comparison group: Patients not exposed (treated with) the drug. EFFECT MODIFICATION
  • 23. Confounding by Indication for Prescription  Synonyms: Indication bias, Contraindication bias, Channeling, Confounding by severity.  Indication for a prescription is the most important confounder in pharmacoepidemiological research. WHY????  Because there is always a ‘reason’ for a prescription and the reason is often associated with the outcome of interest.  Can induce selection bias in drug efficacy studies.  Difficult to control.  Often impossible to obtain a sufficiently accurate estimate of the confounder’s effect.  Miettinen (1983) – preventive use of warfarin was associated with a 27-fold increase in the risk of thrombotic events (conditions that should actually be prevented by warfarin). This paradoxical result was because only highly susceptible patients, or those already experiencing the first symptoms of thrombosis were included in the study.
  • 24. Confounding by Comedication and Other Cofactors  Patients often take more than one drug at a time and it is sometimes difficult to isolate the effect of a specific drug, in research studies.  Coronary Drug Project (1980) –the risks of death in placebo group after 5 years were 15% (compliant cases) and 28.2% (non-compliant cases). Potential Confounders: Selection bias; patients compliant with one drug were very likely to be compliant with other interventions (e.g., other very effective drugs, diet, physical exercise, etc.).
  • 25. SOLUTIONS FOR SELECTION BIAS  Must be prevented at the design stage, because it cannot be corrected at the analysis stage.  Selection bias can result in over- or under-representation of the people who have a drug exposure–outcome relationship.  Strategies: • Random sampling of the cases and controls from the source population. • Systematically recruiting a series of consecutive subjects (to prevent self-selection). • Minimizing the number of subjects lost to follow-up in cohort studies. • Tracking procedure for drop-out cases (to identify the reasons). • Selecting only incident cases of the condition. • Assigning random allocation of drug exposure – Follow the procedure (to prevent self-selection and referral bias).
  • 26. SOLUTIONS FOR INFORMATION BIAS  Must be resolved at the design stage.  Strategies: • Blinding (or masking) of relevant study personnel; • Standardization of the measurement process for both cases and controls • use of standard structured questionnaires, • specific training of interviewers, • different observers for different measurements • Standardized criteria for defining drug exposure and disease outcomes.
  • 27. SOLUTIONS FOR CONFOUNDING  It is possible to control the effect of confounding at both the design and the analysis levels.  Strategies: @ Design level: • Randomization, • Matching – ensure similarities in both case and control groups; be cautious of ‘over matching’, • Restrict confounding by studying only one level – e.g., studying the drug effect among only one category of age will prevent against the occurrence of confounding by age. @ Analysis levels: • Standardization • Stratification
  • 29. • Cause: a stimulus that produces an effect or outcome. • Change in host-agent-environmental balance produces an effect or outcome. • Cause (statistical definition): a factor which varies (either proportional or inversely proportional) to the health condition of interest (health condition studied). Statistical relationship • E.g., a disease with risk factors X, Y and Z • Is there a ‘statistical relationship’ b/n factors X and Y? • Meaning: any association b/n X and Y occurs by chance or not by chance (greater frequency than that by chance?) • Next step: tests for independence or association by Chi-square test or Correlation coefficient test • If the test results are statistically significant, then X and Y are not independent, but have an association that is not entirely due to chance.
  • 30. • Study: rates of developing complications after mastectomy for women with and without anxious personalities. • Test Result: Chi-square test is statistically significant at p<0.05 • Meaning: 95 times out of 100, this difference in complication rates between the two samples is not due to chance. • Inference: Personality and complication rates are not independent. • They have a significant statistical association. • BUT….. This does not mean that only those with anxious personality type suffer from complications post-mastectomy. SAMPLE COMPLICATION RATES (after mastectomy) Women with anxious personality 100 nos. Women without anxious personality 40 nos.
  • 31. • It means that a woman with anxious personality is more likely to suffer from complications after mastectomy than a woman without anxious personality. • Determining the ‘statistical significant association’ is the first step in determining whether the relationship is causal. Causal relationship • Statistically significant factors (non-independent) can have non- causal relationship or causal relationship. Non-causal relationship • Hypothetical variable varies with actual causal variable • E.g., Relation b/n paternal age and infant birth weight • paternal age (Hypothetical variable) • Maternal weight (actual causal variable )and infant birth weight • Even if there is a statistical significant relation b/n paternal age and infant birth weight, there isn’t any logical explanation.
  • 32. Causal relationships • 2 types: a) direct and b) indirect a) Direct causal relationship • A factor directly causes a disease with no other intervening factor. • Causal factor Outcome • E.g., Tubercle bacilli TB • Sometimes, what is considered a direct causal association may later on be identified as indirect causal association. • E.g.1, Cholera outbreak in England (1883) – Dr. John Snow – identified certain water sources as the causative factor; Drinking water from those sources was banned. Later on, it was identified that V.cholerae was the causative factor. Water was only the vector.
  • 33. E.g.2, Toxic Shock Syndrome (TSS) • When TSS first emerged, clinicians identified tampons as the causative factor. • Later on, Staphylococcal spp. was identified as the causative factor. • Tampons were the vector (indirect contributory cause). • Education programmes aimed at eliminating the use of tampons; changing the way tampons were used; • Women were advised to avoid super-absorbent tampons, change tampons frequently, practice good hygiene.
  • 34. b) Indirect causal relationships • Extra variable/s (intervening variables) occupies/y an intermediate stage b/n cause and effect. • A B C D • ‘A’ is causally associated with ‘D’, only after the interposition of variables ‘B’ and ‘C’. • E.g., Relationship b/n cigarette smoke and chronic bronchitis Breathing the air polluted by cigarette smoke (A) Damage to the respiratory epithelium (B) Increased susceptibility of respiratory epithelium to infection (C) Chronic bronchitis (D)
  • 35. Multiple causative factors • Diseases have many risk factors, all of which are involved in development of the disease. • Exposure to multiple causative factors can have additive or multiplicative effect. • E.g.1, Even though smoking is a major cause of lung cancer, it is not the only cause. Non-smokers (either active or passive) can also get lung cancer. • Risk of cancer is higher among non-smokers exposed to asbestos than smokers not exposed to asbestos. • E.g.2, Automobile accidents can occur due to speeding, faulty equipments, heavy traffic, poor visibility, driver’s inexperience, drinking etc. • Web of causation: association b/n all the causative factors which have impact on the risk of developing a disease.
  • 36. Establishing causality • Through epidemiological studies and clinical trials. • A factor is considered causal, when reducing it’s amount or frequency, reduces the frequency of the effect. • E.g., If treating hypertensive patients (keeping their BP low) can reduce the frequency of stroke when compared to an equivalent , untreated group of hypertensive patients, then HTN is considered a risk factor for stroke (HTN is a causal factor for stroke).
  • 37. Naranjo Algorithm (ADR) Probability Scale • A method by which to assess whether there is a causal relationship between a DRUG and ADR. • Use a simple questionnaire (10 qns.) to assign probability scores. • Answers are either Yes, No, or “Do not know”. • Different point values (-1, 0, +1 or +2) are assigned to each answer. • Total scores range from -4 to +13; – Definite reaction: 9 to 13; Probable reaction: 5 to 8; – Possible reaction: 1 to 4; Doubtful reaction: 0 or less. • The response ‘Do not know’ should be used – sparingly; – only when the quality of the data does not permit a ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ answer. – if the information is not available – if the question is inapplicable to the case.
  • 38. • When more than one drug is involved or suspected, • the ADR Probability Scale is applied separately to each of the possible etiologic agents (drugs); • the drug with the highest score should be considered the causative agent; • the potential of interaction should be evaluated;
  • 39. Question Yes No Do Not Know Score 1. Are there previous conclusive reports on this reaction? +1 0 0 2. Did the adverse event appear after the suspected drug was administered? +2 -1 0 3. Did the adverse event improve when the drug was discontinued or a specific antagonist was administered? +1 0 0 4. Did the adverse event reappear when the drug was readministered? +2 -1 0 5. Are there alternative causes that could on their own have caused the reaction? -1 +2 0
  • 40. Question Yes No Do Not Know Score 6. Did the reaction reappear when a placebo was given? -1 +1 0 7. Was the drug detected in blood or other fluids in concentrations known to be toxic? +1 0 0 8. Was the reaction more severe when the dose was increased or less severe when the dose was decreased? +1 0 0 9. Did the patient have a similar reaction to the same or similar drugs in any previous exposure? +1 0 0 10. Was the adverse event confirmed by any objective evidence? +1 0 0 Total Score:
  • 41. THANK YOU!!! Courtesy: Textbook of Pharmacoepidemiology by Strom and Kimmel