SlideShare a Scribd company logo
James T. Todd, Christoph H. Ronacher, and Lorraine E. Bahrick
Florida International University
Assessing the Development of Infant Attention to Dynamic Events: A Simple
Procedure
Presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, March 2011, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. This research was supported by grants from NICHD R01 HD053776, KO2 HDO64943, and R03 HD052602, NIMH R01 MH62226,
NSF SLC SBE0350201, and Autism Speaks #1906. Requests for reprints should be sent to the first author at jtodd@fiu.edu.
.
Background
Although it is widely agreed that infants prefer social over nonsocial events,
there has been little systematic assessment of the basis for this preference or when
it emerges in infancy. Our recent findings suggest that heightened attention to social
events emerges, in part, as a result of the attentional salience of intersensory
redundancy (Bahrick et al., 2009). According to the Intersensory Redundancy
Hypothesis (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2002, in press), infants show heightened attention to
bimodal, audiovisual events as compared with unimodal (visual, auditory) events
because they provide intersensory redundancy (e.g., synchrony, rhythm, and tempo
invariant across the senses). Relative to nonsocial events, social events provide a
great amount of intersensory redundancy, particularly across face, speech, and
gesture. Bahrick et al. (2009) demonstrated that although infant processing time to
events decreases across age, infants show greater attention (longer processing time,
longer looks, fewer disengagements) to bimodal (audiovisual, redundant) than
unimodal (visual, nonredundant) events, and to social than nonsocial events, with
differences increasing across age. Thus, preferences for dynamic, multimodal social
events emerge gradually across infancy with increasing experience in the social
world.
These findings have potential applications for identifying atypical
developmental patterns, including infants at risk for autism. However, our previous
results were based on data from infant control habituation sessions which are
complex and lengthy to administer, particularly outside research lab settings. The
present study explored a shorter, simpler alternative for measuring infant attention
by using only the first two infant-controlled trials (baseline interest) of the habituation
procedure.
Method
Data from Bahrick et al. (2009), which included a number of infant-control
habituation studies conducted in our lab over the past decade, with 718 infants of 2,
3, 4-5, or 6-8 months (N = 167, 189, 205, 157, respectively) were rescored and
analyzed. Infants had been habituated to one of four types of dynamic events,
bimodal audiovisual or unimodal visual (silent) social events (women speaking using
infant-directed speech), or bimodal audiovisual or unimodal visual nonsocial events
(toy hammer tapping a rhythm; see Figure. 1). Three measures of attention were
computed from the first two trials of habituation (defined by a 1.5 s look-away):
mean total looking time (processing time), mean length of look, and mean number of
looks away per minute (disengagement).
Conclusions
Results of the present study using two infant-controlled trials replicated
those of our prior study based on complete habituation data for infants of 2-8
months of age. Consistent with predictions of the Intersensory Redundancy
Hypothesis, infants showed greater attention (longer processing time, longer looks,
fewer disengagements) to bimodal audiovisual social events than to all other event
types (unimodal visual, nonsocial), with differences emerging gradually across
infancy. They also showed greater attention to bimodal events (that provide
intersensory redundancy) than to unimodal visual events (that provide no
redundancy). These findings have important theoretical and methodological
implications. They demonstrate the feasibility of assessing three fundamental
indices of infant attention in a relatively simple procedure based on two infant-
controlled looking trials (an average of 68 s), making assessments in applied
settings possible. Further, they indicate that attentional differences as a function
of infant age, event type, and intersensory redundancy are evident in early
processing, when events are most novel.
Results
Results from the first two habituation trials are displayed in Figure 2a, b, c,
alongside results from the overall habituation phase (Figure 3a, b, c). Age (2, 3, 4-
5, 6-8 months) by event type (social, nonsocial) by type of stimulation (bimodal,
unimodal) ANOVAs were conducted (all factors between subjects) for each of the
three indices of attention. ANOVAs revealed findings similar to those of our prior
study, including decreasing looking times across age (ps < .001) and greater
attention (longer processing time, longer looks, fewer disengagements) to bimodal
than unimodal stimulation, and to social than nonsocial events (ps < .002). In
addition, the effects of condition and event type were increasingly apparent across
age (ps < .04), with greatest attention (processing time) to bimodal social events
than all other event types (nonsocial or unimodal) emerging by 6-8 months of age
(ps < .001).
Figure 2 (a). Mean Looking Time
References
Bahrick, L.E. & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy guides early
perceptual and cognitive development. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in Child
Development and Behavior, 30 (pp. 153-187). New York: Academic Press.
Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (in press). The role of intersensory redundancy in early
perceptual, cognitive, and social development. In A. Bremner, D. J.
Lewkowicz, & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. xx-xx). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Bahrick, L. E., Todd, J., Castellanos, I., Sorondo, B., Vaillant-Molina, M., &
Argumosa, M. A. (2009, May). The role of intersensory redundancy in the
typical development of socal orienting across infancy: A new hypothesis for
autism. Poster presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research,
Chicago, IL.
Figure 2 (b). Mean Length of Look
Figure 2 (c). Number of Looks Away
per Minute
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
2 3 4-5 6-8
ProcessingTime(Secondstohabituation)
Age (Months)
Soc Bi
Nonsoc Bi
Soc Uni
Non Uni
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
2 3 4-5 6-8
AverageLengthofLook(insec)
Age (Months)
Soc Bi
Nonsoc Bi
Soc Uni
Nonsoc Uni
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
18
2 3 4-5 6-8
Disengagement(LooksAwayperMinute)
Age (Months)
Soc Bi
Nonsoc Bi
Nonsoc Uni
Soc Uni
First Two Trials of Habituation Overall Habituation (Bahrick et al., 2009)
0
50
100
150
200
250
300
2 3 4-5 6-8
ProcessingTime(Secondstohabituation)
Age (Months)
Soc Bi
Soc Uni
Nonsoc Bi
Nonsoc Uni
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
2 3 4-5 6-8
AverageLengthofLook(insec)
Age (Months)
Soc Bi
Nonsoc Bi
Soc Uni
Nonsoc Uni
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
18
2 3 4-5 6-8
Disengagement(LooksAwayperMinute)
Age (Months)
Soc Bi
Nonsoc Bi
Soc Uni
Nonsoc Uni
Social Nonsocial
Figure 1: Static image of one example of the social and nonsocial events.
Figure 3 (a). Mean Looking Time
Figure 3 (b). Mean Length of Look
Figure 3 (c). Number of Looks Away
per Minute

More Related Content

PDF
Sample experiment-paper-1
PDF
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis
PDF
Ross Shegog - The Secret of Seven Stones: A Game to Impact Youth Skills and P...
PPT
Problem Solving
PDF
The Effect of Prolonged Attachment to Transitional Object on Anxiety Level an...
PDF
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...
PDF
PEC PA&Utilities 2016 | Razionalizzazione del public procurement
Sample experiment-paper-1
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis
Ross Shegog - The Secret of Seven Stones: A Game to Impact Youth Skills and P...
Problem Solving
The Effect of Prolonged Attachment to Transitional Object on Anxiety Level an...
The worsening trajectory of social impairment in preterm born young adults an...
PEC PA&Utilities 2016 | Razionalizzazione del public procurement

Viewers also liked (8)

PPTX
Problemas
DOCX
Bitácora de tecnología doc
PPTX
GANODERMA LUCIDUM
PDF
Lenguaje corporal
PPTX
DXN COLOMBIA
PPTX
What Ascom can do for you.....
PPTX
El lenguaje corporal
Problemas
Bitácora de tecnología doc
GANODERMA LUCIDUM
Lenguaje corporal
DXN COLOMBIA
What Ascom can do for you.....
El lenguaje corporal
Ad

Similar to SRCD 2011 Baseline Handout (20)

PDF
IMFAR 2010 ASD Intersensory Poster Layout
PPT
sensation perception and learning.ppt
PPT
Challengesto piaget
PPT
Infancy
PPT
PDF
An Introduction to Developmental Psychology-Wiley (2017)-170-239.pdf
DOCX
Developmental Psychology Copyright 1987 by the American Psycho
DOCX
Developmental psychology copyright 1987 by the american psycho
PPTX
Sensory and perceptual development
PDF
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...
PDF
5 # infancy &amp; pre childhood
PPT
Child development, chapter 6, Caprice Paduano
PPT
Child development, chapter 6, paduano
PPT
Capacités perceptives
PDF
Infants and Children Prenatal through Middle Childhood 8th Edition Berk Solut...
PPTX
Category Representation in Young Infants
PPT
Piaget
PPTX
Theories of child psychology
PDF
Childhood Voyages in Development 5th Edition Rathus Solutions Manual
PPTX
Santrock 16e ch05_accessible
IMFAR 2010 ASD Intersensory Poster Layout
sensation perception and learning.ppt
Challengesto piaget
Infancy
An Introduction to Developmental Psychology-Wiley (2017)-170-239.pdf
Developmental Psychology Copyright 1987 by the American Psycho
Developmental psychology copyright 1987 by the american psycho
Sensory and perceptual development
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...
5 # infancy &amp; pre childhood
Child development, chapter 6, Caprice Paduano
Child development, chapter 6, paduano
Capacités perceptives
Infants and Children Prenatal through Middle Childhood 8th Edition Berk Solut...
Category Representation in Young Infants
Piaget
Theories of child psychology
Childhood Voyages in Development 5th Edition Rathus Solutions Manual
Santrock 16e ch05_accessible
Ad

SRCD 2011 Baseline Handout

  • 1. James T. Todd, Christoph H. Ronacher, and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Assessing the Development of Infant Attention to Dynamic Events: A Simple Procedure Presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, March 2011, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. This research was supported by grants from NICHD R01 HD053776, KO2 HDO64943, and R03 HD052602, NIMH R01 MH62226, NSF SLC SBE0350201, and Autism Speaks #1906. Requests for reprints should be sent to the first author at jtodd@fiu.edu. . Background Although it is widely agreed that infants prefer social over nonsocial events, there has been little systematic assessment of the basis for this preference or when it emerges in infancy. Our recent findings suggest that heightened attention to social events emerges, in part, as a result of the attentional salience of intersensory redundancy (Bahrick et al., 2009). According to the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2002, in press), infants show heightened attention to bimodal, audiovisual events as compared with unimodal (visual, auditory) events because they provide intersensory redundancy (e.g., synchrony, rhythm, and tempo invariant across the senses). Relative to nonsocial events, social events provide a great amount of intersensory redundancy, particularly across face, speech, and gesture. Bahrick et al. (2009) demonstrated that although infant processing time to events decreases across age, infants show greater attention (longer processing time, longer looks, fewer disengagements) to bimodal (audiovisual, redundant) than unimodal (visual, nonredundant) events, and to social than nonsocial events, with differences increasing across age. Thus, preferences for dynamic, multimodal social events emerge gradually across infancy with increasing experience in the social world. These findings have potential applications for identifying atypical developmental patterns, including infants at risk for autism. However, our previous results were based on data from infant control habituation sessions which are complex and lengthy to administer, particularly outside research lab settings. The present study explored a shorter, simpler alternative for measuring infant attention by using only the first two infant-controlled trials (baseline interest) of the habituation procedure. Method Data from Bahrick et al. (2009), which included a number of infant-control habituation studies conducted in our lab over the past decade, with 718 infants of 2, 3, 4-5, or 6-8 months (N = 167, 189, 205, 157, respectively) were rescored and analyzed. Infants had been habituated to one of four types of dynamic events, bimodal audiovisual or unimodal visual (silent) social events (women speaking using infant-directed speech), or bimodal audiovisual or unimodal visual nonsocial events (toy hammer tapping a rhythm; see Figure. 1). Three measures of attention were computed from the first two trials of habituation (defined by a 1.5 s look-away): mean total looking time (processing time), mean length of look, and mean number of looks away per minute (disengagement). Conclusions Results of the present study using two infant-controlled trials replicated those of our prior study based on complete habituation data for infants of 2-8 months of age. Consistent with predictions of the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis, infants showed greater attention (longer processing time, longer looks, fewer disengagements) to bimodal audiovisual social events than to all other event types (unimodal visual, nonsocial), with differences emerging gradually across infancy. They also showed greater attention to bimodal events (that provide intersensory redundancy) than to unimodal visual events (that provide no redundancy). These findings have important theoretical and methodological implications. They demonstrate the feasibility of assessing three fundamental indices of infant attention in a relatively simple procedure based on two infant- controlled looking trials (an average of 68 s), making assessments in applied settings possible. Further, they indicate that attentional differences as a function of infant age, event type, and intersensory redundancy are evident in early processing, when events are most novel. Results Results from the first two habituation trials are displayed in Figure 2a, b, c, alongside results from the overall habituation phase (Figure 3a, b, c). Age (2, 3, 4- 5, 6-8 months) by event type (social, nonsocial) by type of stimulation (bimodal, unimodal) ANOVAs were conducted (all factors between subjects) for each of the three indices of attention. ANOVAs revealed findings similar to those of our prior study, including decreasing looking times across age (ps < .001) and greater attention (longer processing time, longer looks, fewer disengagements) to bimodal than unimodal stimulation, and to social than nonsocial events (ps < .002). In addition, the effects of condition and event type were increasingly apparent across age (ps < .04), with greatest attention (processing time) to bimodal social events than all other event types (nonsocial or unimodal) emerging by 6-8 months of age (ps < .001). Figure 2 (a). Mean Looking Time References Bahrick, L.E. & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy guides early perceptual and cognitive development. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 30 (pp. 153-187). New York: Academic Press. Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (in press). The role of intersensory redundancy in early perceptual, cognitive, and social development. In A. Bremner, D. J. Lewkowicz, & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp. xx-xx). New York: Oxford University Press. Bahrick, L. E., Todd, J., Castellanos, I., Sorondo, B., Vaillant-Molina, M., & Argumosa, M. A. (2009, May). The role of intersensory redundancy in the typical development of socal orienting across infancy: A new hypothesis for autism. Poster presented at the International Meeting for Autism Research, Chicago, IL. Figure 2 (b). Mean Length of Look Figure 2 (c). Number of Looks Away per Minute 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 2 3 4-5 6-8 ProcessingTime(Secondstohabituation) Age (Months) Soc Bi Nonsoc Bi Soc Uni Non Uni 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 2 3 4-5 6-8 AverageLengthofLook(insec) Age (Months) Soc Bi Nonsoc Bi Soc Uni Nonsoc Uni 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 2 3 4-5 6-8 Disengagement(LooksAwayperMinute) Age (Months) Soc Bi Nonsoc Bi Nonsoc Uni Soc Uni First Two Trials of Habituation Overall Habituation (Bahrick et al., 2009) 0 50 100 150 200 250 300 2 3 4-5 6-8 ProcessingTime(Secondstohabituation) Age (Months) Soc Bi Soc Uni Nonsoc Bi Nonsoc Uni 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 2 3 4-5 6-8 AverageLengthofLook(insec) Age (Months) Soc Bi Nonsoc Bi Soc Uni Nonsoc Uni 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 2 3 4-5 6-8 Disengagement(LooksAwayperMinute) Age (Months) Soc Bi Nonsoc Bi Soc Uni Nonsoc Uni Social Nonsocial Figure 1: Static image of one example of the social and nonsocial events. Figure 3 (a). Mean Looking Time Figure 3 (b). Mean Length of Look Figure 3 (c). Number of Looks Away per Minute