SlideShare a Scribd company logo
 
By Stuart Warren, MSc(Optom) (Hons)
 The majority of consensus is that reading problems such as dyslexia are due to
difficulties converting the visual components of words into their sound components.
Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, Scanlon (2004)
 Consider the word “cat”…
c / a / t > “c” / “a” / “t”
 The visual components are called “graphemes” & the sound components are called
“phonemes”.
 There are about 44 phonemes in the English language.
 Difficulty making this conversion underlies the “Phonological Model” of reading.
 This can be helped by teaching phonemic awareness (word sounds) and phonics
(mapping sounds to letters or parts of words).
 Research by Professor Sally Shaywitz at Yale University using fMRI brain scans on
students with dyslexia has shown numerous areas of the brain are involved in reading
but that there are 3 areas in the left hemisphere that dominate. Shaywitz et al (1998).
These include a Visual Word Form Area (yellow), a Word Analysis Area (red) and a
Speech Area (green).
(Front) (Back)
 The Word Analysis Area (WAA) is located in the temporal parietal cortex.
 The Visual Word Form Area (VWFA) is located in the occipital cortex.
 The speech area, also known as Broca’s Area, is located in the frontal cortex.
 The WAA is important for phonological processing and works together with the speech
area for saying words. It is the slower “sub-lexical” route important for decoding words.
 The VWFA is important for storing words that have been added to our sight word
vocabulary and also works together with the speech area for saying (or subvocalizing)
words. It is the faster “lexical” route for normal reading.
 These areas comprise the key neural circuits for reading.
 Other areas in the brain important for
reading include the Auditory Cortex
for processing auditory information
and Wernicke’s Area for language
comprehension.
 Although the evidence shows that phonological skills correlate the most highly with
reading disabilities this does not prove it is the cause. Castles & Coltheart (2004)
 Phonics training helps with non-word reading skills, word accuracy and letter-sound
knowledge but may not be helpful for reading fluency, spelling, phonological output
and reading comprehension. McArthur et al (2012)
 Not all students with dyslexia have a phonological problem.
 Not all students who require learning support have a reading problem.
 If other factors are shown to be deficient further upstream (such as vision) this
weakens the phonological model as being a cause for learning disabilities.
If a school phonics programme is successful is this because students have learned
better phonological skills or because they have developed better word identification
skills associated with the phonics programme?
 Vision is the dominant sense
 The symptoms suggest it
 Reading starts with vision
 The science shows a problem
 Visual interventions are effective
 It is estimated that at least 80% of learning is derived from vision.
 Try closing your eyes and think about all you couldn’t do in the classroom!
 Vision is the dominant sense
 The symptoms suggest it
 Reading starts with vision
 The science shows a problem
 Visual interventions are effective
 When most children start school they are fine with mapping sounds to pictures &
objects. The same part of the brain however is also believed to be used for written
language (referred to as “neuronal recycling”) which suggests that the problem of
written language may be more to do with processing of visual symbols (ie. letters and
numbers).
 Young (or impaired) readers need bigger text and wider spacing.
 Losing place, skipping words and lines is common.
 Words appear to move on the page for some readers.
 Missing the start or ends of words is common.
 Persistent letter reversals.
 Vision is the dominant sense
 The symptoms suggest it
 Reading starts with vision
 The science shows a problem
 Visual interventions are effective
Consider the visual skills required for reading. A reader needs to:
 Estimate the number of letters in a word (eg. a 2 or 5 letter word?)
 Position their eyes in the correct position on the word (about a third of the way along
the word).
 Keep the words stable on the page.
 Extract multiple letters per look (eg. 2 letters, 4 letters etc)
 Ensure the correct orientation and order of the letters.
 Identify the exact shape, colour and detail of the letters.
These visual skills can be describes as follows:
 Estimate the number of letters in a word – “visual counting”
 Position eyes in the correct position on the word – “saccades” or “voluntary eye
movement control”
 Keep the words stable on the page – “fixation”
 Extract multiple letters per look – “visual span”
 Ensure the correct orientation and order of the letters – “spatial attention”
 Identify the exact shape, colour and detail of the letters – “visual acuity”
But there’s a problem….
 Estimate the number of letters in a word – “visual counting”
 Position eyes in the correct position on the word – “saccades” or “voluntary eye
movement control”
 Keep the words stable on the page – “fixation”
 Extract multiple letters per look – “visual span”
 Ensure the correct orientation and order of the letters – “spatial attention”
 Identify the exact shape, colour and detail of the letters – “visual acuity”
When children’s eyes are tested at school only the skills at the
bottom in grey (“visual acuity” and “colour vision”) are tested.
The skills shown above in blue are NOT included however these
are the ones thought to be a problem with reading! They also
happen to be supported by the same neural pathways – the
“Magnocellular Pathway”.
 Vision is the dominant sense
 The symptoms suggest it
 Reading starts with vision
 The science shows a problem
 Visual interventions are effective
 The science shows there are 2 main visual systems referred to as the “Magnocellular
Pathway” (M-Pathway) and the “Parvocellular Pathway” (P-Pathway).
 Both of these pathways originate from the eyeball and extend all the way to the back
of the brain called the “visual cortex” & remain anatomically separated.
What Do these Pathways Do?
 M-Pathways: make up only 10% of the visual
pathway but are much bigger in size. They
conduct information more quickly and are
important for motion, spatial coding and
temporal (rapid) processing.
 P-Pathways: make up much of the remaining
visual pathway and are smaller in size. They
conduct information more slowly and are
important for high contrast, colour vision and
fine detailed (static) processing.
 Only one system however, the M-Pathway, has been found to be a problem in dyslexia.
 The M-Pathway has been studied by scientists for over 30 years. It is much easier to
study BEFORE it reaches the visual cortex (at a “subcortical level”) as it is anatomically
distinct from the P-Pathway. Livingstone, Rosen, Drislane, Galaburda (1991)
 Examples of tests used to look for an M-Pathway deficit are:
 coherent motion sensitivity
 critical fusion frequency
 contrast sensitivity
 anatomical appearance
There are over 300 studies on the Magnocellular Pathways with 90% of them showing
a problem in dyslexia. This has become known as the “Magnocellular Deficit Theory” of
dyslexia.
 Once the M-Pathways & P-Pathways reach the primary visual cortex (V1) they diverge
in two separate directions.
 The P-Pathways track down to follow the “Ventral Pathway”. This has an input into the
VWFA.
 The M-Pathways track up to follow the “Dorsal Pathway”. This has a large input into
the WAA in the Parietal Cortex and continues through to the Frontal Cortex - the
region of the brain that controls voluntary eye movements for reading.
(Front) (Back)
 The M-Pathway helps us to know “where” we are on the page and captures critical
information rapidly, co-ordinates this with voluntary eye movements, and determines
early characteristics of the letters including their spatial arrangement. This information
is combined with information from the P-Pathway in order to identify “what” the letters
are.
 In addition to feedforward pathways (blue) there are also many feedback pathways
(red) which makes the M-Pathways harder to study at the cortical level.
(Front) (Back)
 In order to test for an M-Pathway deficit at the cortical level we need to consider the
type of skill being tested and the region of the brain that’s involved. The use of fMRI
brain scans can help with this. Demb (1998)
 Skills supported by the M-Pathway include:
 Voluntary eye tracking (saccades)
 Spatial coding of letters
 Temporal processing tasks (eg. visual counting & visual span)
 Motion stability
 A high M-Pathway input to the Cerebellum means that a deficit could potentially also
affect visual motor co-ordination. Stoodley & Stein (2013)
 There is evidence an equivalent M-Pathway may exist for auditory processing as well.
 This creates the possibility of a pansensory deficit across multiple domains or there
may be a predilection for just one domain only (eg. eye tracking). Stein (2001)
A standardized battery of tests that target M-Pathway functions essential for learning
would be helpful for clinicians and educators.
 It is well known that students with dyslexia have an eye tracking problem (ie. they make
more backwards eye movements and pauses) but this has often been attributed to a
language problem rather than a primary eye disorder.
 This is because early studies found no problem with eye tracking known as “saccades”.
 The reason was they only measured saccade reaction times for students ages 10 to 13
years old.
 Research by Professor Burkhart Fischer at the Freiburg University shows that
saccade reaction times are only significant for younger and older students for the two
main types of saccades, “pro-saccades” and “anti-saccades” (see below).
 Furthermore, when testing using an anti-saccade strategy (ie. one that requires the
student to look the other way from the test stimulus) it can be shown that students
with dyslexia have a significant problem with eye movement control independently of
language. Fischer & Hartnegg (2000), Fukushima (2005), Bucci (2008)
Graphs: Fischer & Hartnegg (2000)
 Studies show that “pursuit” eye movements are also associated with learning difficulties
including dyslexia. Callu et al (2005), Eden, Stein, Wood (1994)
 This is often overlooked because we do not use pursuit eye movements to read.
 It is known however that pursuit eye movements can share similar (and sometimes the
same) neural pathways as saccades! Krauzlis (2005), Rosano et al (2002)
 Above: Pursuit eye movements in non-dyslexics compared to dyslexics using a Tobii
eye tracker. Presentation from Dr. Anikar Haseloff (2009), University of Hohenheim.
 This includes a skill called “subitizing” (the ability to know the number of items present
in a look) and also the ability to count larger numbers of items from memory.
 It requires students to say how many spots were flashed on a small central screen.
 This visual perceptual capacity tests our concept of “number” and can be deficient in
students with dyscalculia as well as in dyslexia.
 A problem with this task correlates strongly with students struggling to acquire basic
arithmetic skills but it can also affect reading since we estimate the number of letters
in a word when we read (see next slide).
 It has been shown by Fischer and Hartnegg that students with dyslexia & dyscalculia
have a problem with visual counting – both in terms of accuracy and reaction times.
Fischer & Hartnegg (2008)
 The “visual span” is our window of visual attention.
 The further we can attend to visual information per look the greater the visual span.
 There are a number of different methods used to test visual span (eg. Form Resolving
Field, the Trigram Method, Letter Strings).
 If only one item is presented to the side of fixation this places a relatively low demand
on visual attention and so may not be deficient in dyslexia.
 If more than one item is presented to the side (or if items are presented on both
sides) of fixation this places a higher demand on visual attention (as the information
must be processed in parallel) and is more likely to be associated with dyslexia.
Trigram Method Letter String Method
 Bosse et al (2007) found the number of dyslexic students with a visual span disorder
was at least as high as the number with a phonological disorder and that around 23%
had a visual span disorder alone.
 A reduced visual span in dyslexia is not due to phonological processing, short term
memory (using letter strings) or lack of reading experience. Bosse, Valdois, Tainturier
(2007), Lobier, Zoubrinetzky, Valdois (2012)
 There is a relationship between the visual span and reading speed. A smaller visual
span leads to slower reading speeds (see graph below). Kwon, Legge, Dubbels (2007)

 Slow reading speed is a hallmark of dyslexia. Tressoldi, Stella, Faggella (2001)
 Dyslexics often exhibit a reduced visual span and slow reading. Dubois et al (2010)
 Early studies of spatial awareness focused on “lateral preference” (handedness and
eye/hand dominance) as a basis for learning problems. Belmont & Birch (1965) found
that lateral preference was not significant but confusion of left & right side was.
 Studies show that poor readers make more letter and word reversals which may be
linked to visual spatial confusion. Boone (1986), Jordan & Jordan (1990), Badian
(2005)
 As a child gets older they can use various linguistic mechanisms (eg. phonological,
semantic and syntactic) as well as other external cues to compensate for a visual
spatial deficit that may be contributing towards letter reversals. Hence the reversals
may no longer be obvious but the underlying spatial problem may still exist and
continue to affect academic performance. McMonnies (1992)
 The Florida Longitudinal Project found perceptual-motor factors are more important at
predicting reading performance for primary school aged children whereas cognitive
factors become more important for older students. Fletcher, Satz (1980)
 To understand the role of visual spatial development on reading we can turn to brain
imaging techniques which have become a useful tool for studying the effect of spatial
encoding on letter features. Pammer et al (2006)
 It has been found that the more disrupted the spatial qualities of the text becomes the
more active the spatial region of the brain as shown by MEG neuroimaging. This
places a higher demand on the M-Pathway. A deficiency would therefore make it
more difficult to encode letter features.
Eg. 1: “hAvInG iNcOsisTeNt FoNt can severely disrupt reading speed”.
Eg. 2: The ‘Cambridge Effect’ shows us that “lteters can be a taotl mses and you
can still raed wothiut a porbelm”.
 The above statement is not exactly true since reading a page of text like this would
severely decrease reading speed, even for a good reader. It would also be very
difficult for someone learning to read!
Consider the difficulty that a dyslexic student (or beginner reader) may have with
phonological processing (converting graphemes to phonemes) if they have trouble
encoding the letters!
 In addition, poor readers and students with dyslexia (over the age of 8) are more likely
to fail on the “Clock Drawing Test” - a medically recognized test of spatial awareness.
Eden, Wood & Stein (2003)
 Although dyslexics are often thought to have superior spatial abilities, a study by
Winner (2001) involving multiple spatial tasks found that dyslexics performed the
same or worse on all tasks except for one when compared to non-dyslexics.
 Dyslexics do appear to have better “global visual spatial ability”. Karolyi (2003)
 It has been argued that dyslexia may be a problem of visuo-spatial attention caused
by a deficiency in the visual dorsal stream. Vidyasagar (2010)
 This is the ability to perceive differences in sound. It has been shown to be a problem
in dyslexia by numerous investigators and has been linked to problems with decoding
(phonological processing). Tallal (1980), Heiervang et al (2002), Wang et al (2010)
 Fischer and Hartnegg (2004) show a systematic difference for dyslexics compared to
controls in auditory discrimination, especially for “frequency” and “time order” tasks
(see next slide).
 This may have a greater affect on skills such as spelling (converting sounds to letters)
and following instructions rather than on reading (converting letters to sounds).
Fischer and Hartnegg (2004)
 Vision is the dominant sense
 The symptoms suggest it
 Reading starts with vision
 The science shows a problem
 Visual interventions are effective
 Interventions to enhance the visual skills presented in this discussion usually involve
training in the form of daily exercises or the use of tinted lenses.
 The fact these skills undergo such a long development period (up until 16 to 17 years
of age) demonstrates significant plasticity.
 The following slides will consider some of the evidence…
 There are studies in recent literature to support that saccadic eye movements can be
trained. Dyckman & McDowell (2005), Kveraga (2002), Solan et al (2001)
 Fischer & Hartnegg (2000), shows that voluntary saccades (the type used for reading)
can be trained in dyslexics by over 10 times their normal rate of maturation so that
they are no longer significantly different from those of non-dyslexics. Training non-
reading saccades (ie. reflex saccades) for the same time period of time however does
not improve voluntary saccades (anti-saccades).
 Furthermore, training eye movements reduces the number of errors observed in
reading (green bars) compared to a control group that received reading intervention
alone (red bars). Training typically took 10 minutes a day over a 3 to 8 week period.
Fischer & Hartnegg (2008)
 Saccade training has also been shown to significantly improve reading fluency for
elementary school students in a randomized crossover trial. Training was done in
school for 20 minutes a day (3 days a week) for 6 weeks. Leong et al (2014)
 Patching one eye while reading can improve fixation by reducing binocular instability
(eye wobble) in younger children resulting in significant improvements in reading
compared to placebo controls or other types of remedial methods.
Stein, Richardson & Fowler (2000)
 A reduction in binocular instability with monocular occlusion has also been reported by
Fischer and Hartnegg (2008) as shown in the graph below.
 Visual counting and subitizing can be trained. Groffman 2008
 This can lead to improvements in basic arithmetic (on DEMAT) as shown in the graph
below, Fischer, Kongeter & Hartnegg (2008).
 Group 1 has 3 weeks of training (green) and then waits but continues to improve
(blue).
 Group 2 waits with no improvement (red) and is then trained showing significant
improvements in basic arithmetic (yellow).
 Studies show that the visual span can be trained with subsequent improvements in
reading speed of around 40 to 60%. Legge et al (2007), Kwon et al (2007), Chung et
al (2004)
 Training the visual span in dyslexia can also result in faster reading speed (confirmed
with fMRI scan). Valdois et al (2013), Geiger et al (1994)
 As reading appears to be dependent upon the development of visual spatial coding it
is not unreasonable to expect that visual spatial training (that draws on similar spatial
coding patterns required for reading) should translate into better reading skills.
 Studies that use sinusoidal gratings (where the student has to respond in which
direction the grating moves), have been shown to improve reading outcomes for
students with dyslexia compared to age matched controls. The treatment was given
twice a week for 15 minutes over a 16 week period. This demonstrates that targeting
the visuospatial system with regular training can help remediate learning problems.
Lawton (2004), Lawton (2007), Lawton (2011)
 Another approach that is used routinely in optometric vision training uses symbol
charts (eg. arrows and pdpq’s) and links them to kinesthetic awareness.
 This method differs from earlier methods of perceptual training described in the
literature (eg. identifying shapes, labelling body parts, matching the start and ends of
words etc) as it uses developmental principles of daily repetition in combination with
progressively increasing the task difficulty.
 This approach appears promising however there is currently a need for more studies.
Mandani (2009), Fredericks (2006), Pienaar (2011)
 Multiple studies show that auditory skills can be trained. Schaffler, Sonntag, Hartnegg,
Fischer (2004), Gaab et al, (2007), Murphy & Schocat (2011).
Schaffler, Sonntag, Hartnegg,
Fischer (2004)
 Studies are mixed when it comes to showing that auditory training can help with
reading (converting graphemes to phonemes). Temple et al (2003), Gaab et al (2007),
Hook (2001), Strong et al (2010)
 Auditory training however may be more helpful for assisting with spelling as shown by
the green bars below compared with the wait group and placebo controls. Schaffler,
Sonntag, Hartnegg, Fischer (2004)
 Research done by Professor Stein and colleagues at Oxford University show that
tinted lenses (blue and yellow) can improve reading in selected students with dyslexia
by around 2 months/month compared to placebos (grey). Normal progress is
considered to be 1 month/month with most dyslexics falling below this. Ray, Fowler,
Stein (2005), Hall, Ray, Harries, Stein (2013),
 Other controlled studies show a positive effect of tinted overlays on reading for
selected students. Noble et al (2004)
 Evidence such as this has led to the Visual Model of reading.
 This is summarized in the book Visual Aspects of Dyslexia by Stein & Kapoula.
 It does not necessarily replace the Phonological Model since BOTH may be true.
 Furthermore, students with learning difficulties still require educational assistance!
 This view is supported by the American Optometry Association who claim that
“unresolved visual deficits can impair the ability to respond fully to educational
instruction. Management may require optical correction, vision therapy, or a
combination of both.”
“Dyslexia affects about 10% of all children and is a potent cause
of loss of self-confidence, personal and family misery, and waste
of potential. Although the dominant view is that it is caused
specifically by linguistic/phonological weakness, recent research
within the field of neuroscience has shown that it is associated
with visual processing problems as well. These discoveries have
led to a resurgence in visual methods of treatment, which have
shown promising results.”
 Independent research from the University of Padua in Italy claims that not only does
developmental dyslexia have a visual perceptual basis but that we should consider
perceptual training for students with dyslexia as well as for young at risk students.
Gori & Facoetti (2013)
“Our aim is to review the literature supporting a possible role of perceptual learning (PL) in
helping to solve the puzzle called DD (Developmental Dyslexia). PL is defined as improvement
of perceptual skills with practice. Based on the previous literature showing how PL is able to
selectively change visual abilities, we here propose to use PL to improve the impaired visual
functions characterizing DD and, in particular, the visual deficits that could be developmentally
related to an early magnocellular-dorsal pathway and selective attention dysfunction. The
crucial visual attention deficits that are causally linked to DD could be, indeed, strongly
reduced by training the magnocellular-dorsal pathway with the PL, and learning to read for
children with DD would not be anymore such a difficult task. This new remediation approach –
not involving any phonological or orthographic training – could be also used to develop new
prevention programs for pre-reading children at DD risk”.
 Visual factors such as refractive error (eg. long-sight or hyperopia over +1.00DS)
and vergence facility (binocular co-ordination) whilst not considered to be a basis for
dyslexia, are significantly related to reading (see graph below) and so should not be
overlooked. Quaid & Simpson (2003)
 All students with a significant learning problem should have a thorough optometric
assessment before starting vision training.
 Havn’t we disproved the perceptual theory or shown that perceptual factors only play a
minor role? Vellutino et al (2004), Ramus (2002)
 There are numerous studies to suggest that vision is not a factor in reading problems
or dyslexia and one can use these to support an argument against the role of vision. In
the end, one must critically examine each of the studies to see if their conclusions hold.
 In the 80’s it was believed that erratic eye movements observed in dyslexia were due to
poor reading skills. The methods used to study eye movements however were not
adequate to draw such conclusions. Using current scientific methods it can be shown
that a problem exists with eye movement control in dyslexia independently of language.
 Another reason for finding a lack of significance can be due to “ceiling effects” (eg.
when the test is not sensitive enough to discriminate between two groups) This was
argued by Fletcher & Satz (1979) when we moved away from the perceptual model.
 A study may also show no significant difference because of the way a group is selected
for testing. Eg. giving all dyslexic students tinted lenses.
 Finally, a study may show no difference because of the manner in which a treatment is
applied. An example might be the Perceptual Motor Programme (PMP) used in many
NZ schools. A study by Klomp (2012) found no effect on learning outcomes for young
primary school children. The study was done in just 10 weeks and targets a gross level
of perceptual motor development. In order to show a significant improvement in
academic learning however would require that the perceptual motor development be
improved to a level necessary to support academic skills.
 The position statement by the American Academy of Ophthalmology (2014) claims
that “children with dyslexia or related learning disabilities have the same visual
function and ocular health as children without such conditions” .
 The evidence clearly shows however that students with dyslexia or related learning
disabilities do NOT have the same visual function as those without such conditions!
Eden (1995), Kulp,Schmidt (1996), Kulp (1999), Maples (2003), Valdois (2004),
Goldstand (2005), Solan (2007), Shin (2009), Bosse (2009), Chen (2011), Franceshini
(2012), Pienaar (2013)
 They also argue that since children with dyslexia like to play computer games (a highly
visual task) that vision cannot be a factor.
 This is not supported by any evidence. The eye movements used for computer games
are NOT the same as those used for reading books. Also, the nature of the visual
tasks found in gaming are quite different from reading text.
 They claim there is inadequate scientific evidence to support visual interventions.
 In fact there is quite a lot of evidence to support visual interventions as shown by this
review but there are not many randomized placebo controlled trials (studies providing
the highest level of evidence). This does not mean that visual interventions are not
“evidence based” however as most medical treatments also fail to meet this standard.
The basis for speech therapy, occupational therapy and rehabilitative medicine is that
the brain is plastic and can be trained. The evidence showing that vision can be
trained is at least as good. If this is accepted in other fields, one must accept the
possibility that vision can also be trained.
 A study by Olulade et al (2013) was widely promoted in the media claiming to rule
out vision as a factor in dyslexia.
 The study showed that visual motion activity measured using fMRI brain scans to be
less in dyslexic students compared with non-dyslexic students of the same age.
 However when dyslexics were compared with younger non-dyslexic students of the
same reading age then the visual activity was about the same.
 Furthermore, when dyslexics are given reading intervention visual activity increased
suggesting that any failure in vision was due to inadequate reading exposure.
 These findings taken together are used to support the Phonological Model.
 This finding argues the point that the visual activity is normal relative to reading age
and that reduced visual activity is therefore a function of low reading exposure.
 Another explanation however is that the reduced reading age of dyslexics is a
function of their poor visual development! The lower the visual activity the lower the
reading age. This being the case we would expect that improving visual development
would improve the reading age by removing visual barriers to reading.
 The fact that reading practice improves visual activity is not too surprising since there
may be a level of “reciprocal effect” in much the same way there is with phonological
processing and reading practice.
 There exists a large body of evidence to show that visual skills are related to reading
ability and that visual problems are linked with dyslexia.
 Much of the evidence points towards a problem with the development of visual skills
supported by the Magnocellular Pathway, however general optometric findings should
also be considered for individual students.
 Although more studies exist to show a phonological link (due to the higher level of
attention it has received) there are limitations to the Phonological Model.
 The evidence that visual skills can be improved with training is substantial.
 More placebo controlled studies showing the efficacy of visual treatments for dyslexia
are needed but this does not negate the many controlled studies and clinical reports
currently available or preclude it as an evidence-based intervention.
 Without intervention the gap in visual skills will often persist despite maturation.
 The view that vision problems do not contribute to the symptoms observed in dyslexia
or general learning disabilities seems very unlikely.
 Given the currently available evidence a more balanced approach may be to use
visual (and auditory) interventions to complement traditional therapies.
Other Presentations:
The Visual Model of Reading
iCept – For Students with Learning Delays
iCept Screening - Information for Schools

More Related Content

PPT
Perimetry by dr.ricky
PPT
Cyclosporine Ophthalmic Emulsion for Dry Eye Disease
PPTX
HOW TO WRITE A RESEARCH PROPOSAL/PROTOCOL
PDF
Eyeglass prescribing in children 2024 SLMC.pdf
PPTX
Diagnosis & management of accomodative esotropia
PPT
Corneal topography...ppt
PDF
Measuring Success - Security KPIs
PPTX
Vision & Dyslexia: A Critical Review
Perimetry by dr.ricky
Cyclosporine Ophthalmic Emulsion for Dry Eye Disease
HOW TO WRITE A RESEARCH PROPOSAL/PROTOCOL
Eyeglass prescribing in children 2024 SLMC.pdf
Diagnosis & management of accomodative esotropia
Corneal topography...ppt
Measuring Success - Security KPIs
Vision & Dyslexia: A Critical Review

What's hot (20)

PPTX
Exotropia
PPTX
Crvo management 2019 doscon
PPTX
OPTICAL COHERENCE TOMOGRAPHY ANGIOGRAPHY
PPTX
How currently achieve an EDoF lens
PPT
Visual Diagnosis and Care of Patients with Special Needs: Syndromes
PPTX
case presentation on retinitis pigmentosa (low vision aid)
PPTX
FFA Dr Md Afzal Mahfuzullah
PPTX
Identifying and Managing Technical Debt
PDF
Microtropia - Definition, Types and Shot Note
PDF
Prescribing Eyeglasses in Children
PPT
Calculation of EVD and EVP
PDF
Infantile congenital esotropia
PPTX
Establishing a Business Process Management Center of Excellence - Impact 2012
PPT
Accommodating IOLs
PPTX
Contrast Senstivity
PDF
Discover The Future Of SAP BusinessObjects (BI 4.3 SP02)
PPTX
CASE PRESENTATION BV
PPTX
Wide field imaging in retinal pathology.pptx
PPT
Low vision aid
Exotropia
Crvo management 2019 doscon
OPTICAL COHERENCE TOMOGRAPHY ANGIOGRAPHY
How currently achieve an EDoF lens
Visual Diagnosis and Care of Patients with Special Needs: Syndromes
case presentation on retinitis pigmentosa (low vision aid)
FFA Dr Md Afzal Mahfuzullah
Identifying and Managing Technical Debt
Microtropia - Definition, Types and Shot Note
Prescribing Eyeglasses in Children
Calculation of EVD and EVP
Infantile congenital esotropia
Establishing a Business Process Management Center of Excellence - Impact 2012
Accommodating IOLs
Contrast Senstivity
Discover The Future Of SAP BusinessObjects (BI 4.3 SP02)
CASE PRESENTATION BV
Wide field imaging in retinal pathology.pptx
Low vision aid
Ad

Similar to The Visual Model of Reading (20)

PPTX
Learning related visual and auditory problems
PPT
ProfConf2016Developing VMSystem for Reading.
PPT
ProfConf2016Developing VMSystem for Reading.
PPTX
dyslexia a specific language and phonology disorder
PDF
Reading difficulties
PPTX
Evidence base for the treatment of problems found on iCept
PDF
How mind learn to read
PPTX
Neurological Basis of Dyslexia
PDF
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and Hope
PPTX
Slides on how to diagnose dyslexia from by Sally Shaywitz
PDF
Wobble, Warbles and Fish: the brain basis of dyslexia
PPT
Dyslexia and Cognitive Psychology
PPT
Dyslexia and Dysgraphia
PPTX
Learning disabilities
PPTX
learningdisabilities-160223035008ll.pptx
PPTX
Visual perceptual skills
PPT
Dare2 read parent information evening
PPTX
LEARNING DISORDERS
PDF
Dyslexia: a case study of everyday neurobiology
PDF
Dyslexia: From Symptoms to Solutions
Learning related visual and auditory problems
ProfConf2016Developing VMSystem for Reading.
ProfConf2016Developing VMSystem for Reading.
dyslexia a specific language and phonology disorder
Reading difficulties
Evidence base for the treatment of problems found on iCept
How mind learn to read
Neurological Basis of Dyslexia
Report – Oxford-Kobe Symposium Progress and Hope
Slides on how to diagnose dyslexia from by Sally Shaywitz
Wobble, Warbles and Fish: the brain basis of dyslexia
Dyslexia and Cognitive Psychology
Dyslexia and Dysgraphia
Learning disabilities
learningdisabilities-160223035008ll.pptx
Visual perceptual skills
Dare2 read parent information evening
LEARNING DISORDERS
Dyslexia: a case study of everyday neurobiology
Dyslexia: From Symptoms to Solutions
Ad

Recently uploaded (20)

PPTX
master seminar digital applications in india
PDF
Trump Administration's workforce development strategy
PDF
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
PDF
Practical Manual AGRO-233 Principles and Practices of Natural Farming
PDF
Supply Chain Operations Speaking Notes -ICLT Program
PDF
LDMMIA Reiki Yoga Finals Review Spring Summer
PDF
GENETICS IN BIOLOGY IN SECONDARY LEVEL FORM 3
PDF
Black Hat USA 2025 - Micro ICS Summit - ICS/OT Threat Landscape
PPTX
school management -TNTEU- B.Ed., Semester II Unit 1.pptx
PDF
ChatGPT for Dummies - Pam Baker Ccesa007.pdf
PPTX
Lesson notes of climatology university.
PDF
STATICS OF THE RIGID BODIES Hibbelers.pdf
PDF
A systematic review of self-coping strategies used by university students to ...
PDF
Chinmaya Tiranga quiz Grand Finale.pdf
PDF
OBE - B.A.(HON'S) IN INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE -Ar.MOHIUDDIN.pdf
PDF
Yogi Goddess Pres Conference Studio Updates
DOC
Soft-furnishing-By-Architect-A.F.M.Mohiuddin-Akhand.doc
PDF
Computing-Curriculum for Schools in Ghana
PDF
Chapter 2 Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth.pdf
PDF
Module 4: Burden of Disease Tutorial Slides S2 2025
master seminar digital applications in india
Trump Administration's workforce development strategy
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
Practical Manual AGRO-233 Principles and Practices of Natural Farming
Supply Chain Operations Speaking Notes -ICLT Program
LDMMIA Reiki Yoga Finals Review Spring Summer
GENETICS IN BIOLOGY IN SECONDARY LEVEL FORM 3
Black Hat USA 2025 - Micro ICS Summit - ICS/OT Threat Landscape
school management -TNTEU- B.Ed., Semester II Unit 1.pptx
ChatGPT for Dummies - Pam Baker Ccesa007.pdf
Lesson notes of climatology university.
STATICS OF THE RIGID BODIES Hibbelers.pdf
A systematic review of self-coping strategies used by university students to ...
Chinmaya Tiranga quiz Grand Finale.pdf
OBE - B.A.(HON'S) IN INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE -Ar.MOHIUDDIN.pdf
Yogi Goddess Pres Conference Studio Updates
Soft-furnishing-By-Architect-A.F.M.Mohiuddin-Akhand.doc
Computing-Curriculum for Schools in Ghana
Chapter 2 Heredity, Prenatal Development, and Birth.pdf
Module 4: Burden of Disease Tutorial Slides S2 2025

The Visual Model of Reading

  • 1.   By Stuart Warren, MSc(Optom) (Hons)
  • 2.  The majority of consensus is that reading problems such as dyslexia are due to difficulties converting the visual components of words into their sound components. Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, Scanlon (2004)  Consider the word “cat”… c / a / t > “c” / “a” / “t”  The visual components are called “graphemes” & the sound components are called “phonemes”.  There are about 44 phonemes in the English language.  Difficulty making this conversion underlies the “Phonological Model” of reading.  This can be helped by teaching phonemic awareness (word sounds) and phonics (mapping sounds to letters or parts of words).
  • 3.  Research by Professor Sally Shaywitz at Yale University using fMRI brain scans on students with dyslexia has shown numerous areas of the brain are involved in reading but that there are 3 areas in the left hemisphere that dominate. Shaywitz et al (1998). These include a Visual Word Form Area (yellow), a Word Analysis Area (red) and a Speech Area (green). (Front) (Back)
  • 4.  The Word Analysis Area (WAA) is located in the temporal parietal cortex.  The Visual Word Form Area (VWFA) is located in the occipital cortex.  The speech area, also known as Broca’s Area, is located in the frontal cortex.  The WAA is important for phonological processing and works together with the speech area for saying words. It is the slower “sub-lexical” route important for decoding words.  The VWFA is important for storing words that have been added to our sight word vocabulary and also works together with the speech area for saying (or subvocalizing) words. It is the faster “lexical” route for normal reading.  These areas comprise the key neural circuits for reading.  Other areas in the brain important for reading include the Auditory Cortex for processing auditory information and Wernicke’s Area for language comprehension.
  • 5.  Although the evidence shows that phonological skills correlate the most highly with reading disabilities this does not prove it is the cause. Castles & Coltheart (2004)  Phonics training helps with non-word reading skills, word accuracy and letter-sound knowledge but may not be helpful for reading fluency, spelling, phonological output and reading comprehension. McArthur et al (2012)  Not all students with dyslexia have a phonological problem.  Not all students who require learning support have a reading problem.  If other factors are shown to be deficient further upstream (such as vision) this weakens the phonological model as being a cause for learning disabilities. If a school phonics programme is successful is this because students have learned better phonological skills or because they have developed better word identification skills associated with the phonics programme?
  • 6.  Vision is the dominant sense  The symptoms suggest it  Reading starts with vision  The science shows a problem  Visual interventions are effective
  • 7.  It is estimated that at least 80% of learning is derived from vision.  Try closing your eyes and think about all you couldn’t do in the classroom!
  • 8.  Vision is the dominant sense  The symptoms suggest it  Reading starts with vision  The science shows a problem  Visual interventions are effective
  • 9.  When most children start school they are fine with mapping sounds to pictures & objects. The same part of the brain however is also believed to be used for written language (referred to as “neuronal recycling”) which suggests that the problem of written language may be more to do with processing of visual symbols (ie. letters and numbers).  Young (or impaired) readers need bigger text and wider spacing.  Losing place, skipping words and lines is common.  Words appear to move on the page for some readers.  Missing the start or ends of words is common.  Persistent letter reversals.
  • 10.  Vision is the dominant sense  The symptoms suggest it  Reading starts with vision  The science shows a problem  Visual interventions are effective
  • 11. Consider the visual skills required for reading. A reader needs to:  Estimate the number of letters in a word (eg. a 2 or 5 letter word?)  Position their eyes in the correct position on the word (about a third of the way along the word).  Keep the words stable on the page.  Extract multiple letters per look (eg. 2 letters, 4 letters etc)  Ensure the correct orientation and order of the letters.  Identify the exact shape, colour and detail of the letters.
  • 12. These visual skills can be describes as follows:  Estimate the number of letters in a word – “visual counting”  Position eyes in the correct position on the word – “saccades” or “voluntary eye movement control”  Keep the words stable on the page – “fixation”  Extract multiple letters per look – “visual span”  Ensure the correct orientation and order of the letters – “spatial attention”  Identify the exact shape, colour and detail of the letters – “visual acuity”
  • 13. But there’s a problem….  Estimate the number of letters in a word – “visual counting”  Position eyes in the correct position on the word – “saccades” or “voluntary eye movement control”  Keep the words stable on the page – “fixation”  Extract multiple letters per look – “visual span”  Ensure the correct orientation and order of the letters – “spatial attention”  Identify the exact shape, colour and detail of the letters – “visual acuity” When children’s eyes are tested at school only the skills at the bottom in grey (“visual acuity” and “colour vision”) are tested. The skills shown above in blue are NOT included however these are the ones thought to be a problem with reading! They also happen to be supported by the same neural pathways – the “Magnocellular Pathway”.
  • 14.  Vision is the dominant sense  The symptoms suggest it  Reading starts with vision  The science shows a problem  Visual interventions are effective
  • 15.  The science shows there are 2 main visual systems referred to as the “Magnocellular Pathway” (M-Pathway) and the “Parvocellular Pathway” (P-Pathway).  Both of these pathways originate from the eyeball and extend all the way to the back of the brain called the “visual cortex” & remain anatomically separated. What Do these Pathways Do?  M-Pathways: make up only 10% of the visual pathway but are much bigger in size. They conduct information more quickly and are important for motion, spatial coding and temporal (rapid) processing.  P-Pathways: make up much of the remaining visual pathway and are smaller in size. They conduct information more slowly and are important for high contrast, colour vision and fine detailed (static) processing.
  • 16.  Only one system however, the M-Pathway, has been found to be a problem in dyslexia.  The M-Pathway has been studied by scientists for over 30 years. It is much easier to study BEFORE it reaches the visual cortex (at a “subcortical level”) as it is anatomically distinct from the P-Pathway. Livingstone, Rosen, Drislane, Galaburda (1991)  Examples of tests used to look for an M-Pathway deficit are:  coherent motion sensitivity  critical fusion frequency  contrast sensitivity  anatomical appearance There are over 300 studies on the Magnocellular Pathways with 90% of them showing a problem in dyslexia. This has become known as the “Magnocellular Deficit Theory” of dyslexia.
  • 17.  Once the M-Pathways & P-Pathways reach the primary visual cortex (V1) they diverge in two separate directions.  The P-Pathways track down to follow the “Ventral Pathway”. This has an input into the VWFA.  The M-Pathways track up to follow the “Dorsal Pathway”. This has a large input into the WAA in the Parietal Cortex and continues through to the Frontal Cortex - the region of the brain that controls voluntary eye movements for reading. (Front) (Back)
  • 18.  The M-Pathway helps us to know “where” we are on the page and captures critical information rapidly, co-ordinates this with voluntary eye movements, and determines early characteristics of the letters including their spatial arrangement. This information is combined with information from the P-Pathway in order to identify “what” the letters are.  In addition to feedforward pathways (blue) there are also many feedback pathways (red) which makes the M-Pathways harder to study at the cortical level. (Front) (Back)
  • 19.  In order to test for an M-Pathway deficit at the cortical level we need to consider the type of skill being tested and the region of the brain that’s involved. The use of fMRI brain scans can help with this. Demb (1998)  Skills supported by the M-Pathway include:  Voluntary eye tracking (saccades)  Spatial coding of letters  Temporal processing tasks (eg. visual counting & visual span)  Motion stability  A high M-Pathway input to the Cerebellum means that a deficit could potentially also affect visual motor co-ordination. Stoodley & Stein (2013)  There is evidence an equivalent M-Pathway may exist for auditory processing as well.  This creates the possibility of a pansensory deficit across multiple domains or there may be a predilection for just one domain only (eg. eye tracking). Stein (2001) A standardized battery of tests that target M-Pathway functions essential for learning would be helpful for clinicians and educators.
  • 20.  It is well known that students with dyslexia have an eye tracking problem (ie. they make more backwards eye movements and pauses) but this has often been attributed to a language problem rather than a primary eye disorder.  This is because early studies found no problem with eye tracking known as “saccades”.  The reason was they only measured saccade reaction times for students ages 10 to 13 years old.  Research by Professor Burkhart Fischer at the Freiburg University shows that saccade reaction times are only significant for younger and older students for the two main types of saccades, “pro-saccades” and “anti-saccades” (see below).
  • 21.  Furthermore, when testing using an anti-saccade strategy (ie. one that requires the student to look the other way from the test stimulus) it can be shown that students with dyslexia have a significant problem with eye movement control independently of language. Fischer & Hartnegg (2000), Fukushima (2005), Bucci (2008) Graphs: Fischer & Hartnegg (2000)
  • 22.  Studies show that “pursuit” eye movements are also associated with learning difficulties including dyslexia. Callu et al (2005), Eden, Stein, Wood (1994)  This is often overlooked because we do not use pursuit eye movements to read.  It is known however that pursuit eye movements can share similar (and sometimes the same) neural pathways as saccades! Krauzlis (2005), Rosano et al (2002)  Above: Pursuit eye movements in non-dyslexics compared to dyslexics using a Tobii eye tracker. Presentation from Dr. Anikar Haseloff (2009), University of Hohenheim.
  • 23.  This includes a skill called “subitizing” (the ability to know the number of items present in a look) and also the ability to count larger numbers of items from memory.  It requires students to say how many spots were flashed on a small central screen.  This visual perceptual capacity tests our concept of “number” and can be deficient in students with dyscalculia as well as in dyslexia.  A problem with this task correlates strongly with students struggling to acquire basic arithmetic skills but it can also affect reading since we estimate the number of letters in a word when we read (see next slide).
  • 24.  It has been shown by Fischer and Hartnegg that students with dyslexia & dyscalculia have a problem with visual counting – both in terms of accuracy and reaction times. Fischer & Hartnegg (2008)
  • 25.  The “visual span” is our window of visual attention.  The further we can attend to visual information per look the greater the visual span.  There are a number of different methods used to test visual span (eg. Form Resolving Field, the Trigram Method, Letter Strings).  If only one item is presented to the side of fixation this places a relatively low demand on visual attention and so may not be deficient in dyslexia.  If more than one item is presented to the side (or if items are presented on both sides) of fixation this places a higher demand on visual attention (as the information must be processed in parallel) and is more likely to be associated with dyslexia. Trigram Method Letter String Method
  • 26.  Bosse et al (2007) found the number of dyslexic students with a visual span disorder was at least as high as the number with a phonological disorder and that around 23% had a visual span disorder alone.  A reduced visual span in dyslexia is not due to phonological processing, short term memory (using letter strings) or lack of reading experience. Bosse, Valdois, Tainturier (2007), Lobier, Zoubrinetzky, Valdois (2012)  There is a relationship between the visual span and reading speed. A smaller visual span leads to slower reading speeds (see graph below). Kwon, Legge, Dubbels (2007)   Slow reading speed is a hallmark of dyslexia. Tressoldi, Stella, Faggella (2001)  Dyslexics often exhibit a reduced visual span and slow reading. Dubois et al (2010)
  • 27.  Early studies of spatial awareness focused on “lateral preference” (handedness and eye/hand dominance) as a basis for learning problems. Belmont & Birch (1965) found that lateral preference was not significant but confusion of left & right side was.  Studies show that poor readers make more letter and word reversals which may be linked to visual spatial confusion. Boone (1986), Jordan & Jordan (1990), Badian (2005)  As a child gets older they can use various linguistic mechanisms (eg. phonological, semantic and syntactic) as well as other external cues to compensate for a visual spatial deficit that may be contributing towards letter reversals. Hence the reversals may no longer be obvious but the underlying spatial problem may still exist and continue to affect academic performance. McMonnies (1992)  The Florida Longitudinal Project found perceptual-motor factors are more important at predicting reading performance for primary school aged children whereas cognitive factors become more important for older students. Fletcher, Satz (1980)  To understand the role of visual spatial development on reading we can turn to brain imaging techniques which have become a useful tool for studying the effect of spatial encoding on letter features. Pammer et al (2006)
  • 28.  It has been found that the more disrupted the spatial qualities of the text becomes the more active the spatial region of the brain as shown by MEG neuroimaging. This places a higher demand on the M-Pathway. A deficiency would therefore make it more difficult to encode letter features. Eg. 1: “hAvInG iNcOsisTeNt FoNt can severely disrupt reading speed”. Eg. 2: The ‘Cambridge Effect’ shows us that “lteters can be a taotl mses and you can still raed wothiut a porbelm”.  The above statement is not exactly true since reading a page of text like this would severely decrease reading speed, even for a good reader. It would also be very difficult for someone learning to read! Consider the difficulty that a dyslexic student (or beginner reader) may have with phonological processing (converting graphemes to phonemes) if they have trouble encoding the letters!
  • 29.  In addition, poor readers and students with dyslexia (over the age of 8) are more likely to fail on the “Clock Drawing Test” - a medically recognized test of spatial awareness. Eden, Wood & Stein (2003)  Although dyslexics are often thought to have superior spatial abilities, a study by Winner (2001) involving multiple spatial tasks found that dyslexics performed the same or worse on all tasks except for one when compared to non-dyslexics.  Dyslexics do appear to have better “global visual spatial ability”. Karolyi (2003)  It has been argued that dyslexia may be a problem of visuo-spatial attention caused by a deficiency in the visual dorsal stream. Vidyasagar (2010)
  • 30.  This is the ability to perceive differences in sound. It has been shown to be a problem in dyslexia by numerous investigators and has been linked to problems with decoding (phonological processing). Tallal (1980), Heiervang et al (2002), Wang et al (2010)  Fischer and Hartnegg (2004) show a systematic difference for dyslexics compared to controls in auditory discrimination, especially for “frequency” and “time order” tasks (see next slide).  This may have a greater affect on skills such as spelling (converting sounds to letters) and following instructions rather than on reading (converting letters to sounds).
  • 32.  Vision is the dominant sense  The symptoms suggest it  Reading starts with vision  The science shows a problem  Visual interventions are effective
  • 33.  Interventions to enhance the visual skills presented in this discussion usually involve training in the form of daily exercises or the use of tinted lenses.  The fact these skills undergo such a long development period (up until 16 to 17 years of age) demonstrates significant plasticity.  The following slides will consider some of the evidence…
  • 34.  There are studies in recent literature to support that saccadic eye movements can be trained. Dyckman & McDowell (2005), Kveraga (2002), Solan et al (2001)  Fischer & Hartnegg (2000), shows that voluntary saccades (the type used for reading) can be trained in dyslexics by over 10 times their normal rate of maturation so that they are no longer significantly different from those of non-dyslexics. Training non- reading saccades (ie. reflex saccades) for the same time period of time however does not improve voluntary saccades (anti-saccades).
  • 35.  Furthermore, training eye movements reduces the number of errors observed in reading (green bars) compared to a control group that received reading intervention alone (red bars). Training typically took 10 minutes a day over a 3 to 8 week period. Fischer & Hartnegg (2008)  Saccade training has also been shown to significantly improve reading fluency for elementary school students in a randomized crossover trial. Training was done in school for 20 minutes a day (3 days a week) for 6 weeks. Leong et al (2014)
  • 36.  Patching one eye while reading can improve fixation by reducing binocular instability (eye wobble) in younger children resulting in significant improvements in reading compared to placebo controls or other types of remedial methods. Stein, Richardson & Fowler (2000)  A reduction in binocular instability with monocular occlusion has also been reported by Fischer and Hartnegg (2008) as shown in the graph below.
  • 37.  Visual counting and subitizing can be trained. Groffman 2008  This can lead to improvements in basic arithmetic (on DEMAT) as shown in the graph below, Fischer, Kongeter & Hartnegg (2008).  Group 1 has 3 weeks of training (green) and then waits but continues to improve (blue).  Group 2 waits with no improvement (red) and is then trained showing significant improvements in basic arithmetic (yellow).
  • 38.  Studies show that the visual span can be trained with subsequent improvements in reading speed of around 40 to 60%. Legge et al (2007), Kwon et al (2007), Chung et al (2004)  Training the visual span in dyslexia can also result in faster reading speed (confirmed with fMRI scan). Valdois et al (2013), Geiger et al (1994)
  • 39.  As reading appears to be dependent upon the development of visual spatial coding it is not unreasonable to expect that visual spatial training (that draws on similar spatial coding patterns required for reading) should translate into better reading skills.  Studies that use sinusoidal gratings (where the student has to respond in which direction the grating moves), have been shown to improve reading outcomes for students with dyslexia compared to age matched controls. The treatment was given twice a week for 15 minutes over a 16 week period. This demonstrates that targeting the visuospatial system with regular training can help remediate learning problems. Lawton (2004), Lawton (2007), Lawton (2011)  Another approach that is used routinely in optometric vision training uses symbol charts (eg. arrows and pdpq’s) and links them to kinesthetic awareness.  This method differs from earlier methods of perceptual training described in the literature (eg. identifying shapes, labelling body parts, matching the start and ends of words etc) as it uses developmental principles of daily repetition in combination with progressively increasing the task difficulty.  This approach appears promising however there is currently a need for more studies. Mandani (2009), Fredericks (2006), Pienaar (2011)
  • 40.  Multiple studies show that auditory skills can be trained. Schaffler, Sonntag, Hartnegg, Fischer (2004), Gaab et al, (2007), Murphy & Schocat (2011). Schaffler, Sonntag, Hartnegg, Fischer (2004)
  • 41.  Studies are mixed when it comes to showing that auditory training can help with reading (converting graphemes to phonemes). Temple et al (2003), Gaab et al (2007), Hook (2001), Strong et al (2010)  Auditory training however may be more helpful for assisting with spelling as shown by the green bars below compared with the wait group and placebo controls. Schaffler, Sonntag, Hartnegg, Fischer (2004)
  • 42.  Research done by Professor Stein and colleagues at Oxford University show that tinted lenses (blue and yellow) can improve reading in selected students with dyslexia by around 2 months/month compared to placebos (grey). Normal progress is considered to be 1 month/month with most dyslexics falling below this. Ray, Fowler, Stein (2005), Hall, Ray, Harries, Stein (2013),  Other controlled studies show a positive effect of tinted overlays on reading for selected students. Noble et al (2004)
  • 43.  Evidence such as this has led to the Visual Model of reading.  This is summarized in the book Visual Aspects of Dyslexia by Stein & Kapoula.  It does not necessarily replace the Phonological Model since BOTH may be true.  Furthermore, students with learning difficulties still require educational assistance!  This view is supported by the American Optometry Association who claim that “unresolved visual deficits can impair the ability to respond fully to educational instruction. Management may require optical correction, vision therapy, or a combination of both.” “Dyslexia affects about 10% of all children and is a potent cause of loss of self-confidence, personal and family misery, and waste of potential. Although the dominant view is that it is caused specifically by linguistic/phonological weakness, recent research within the field of neuroscience has shown that it is associated with visual processing problems as well. These discoveries have led to a resurgence in visual methods of treatment, which have shown promising results.”
  • 44.  Independent research from the University of Padua in Italy claims that not only does developmental dyslexia have a visual perceptual basis but that we should consider perceptual training for students with dyslexia as well as for young at risk students. Gori & Facoetti (2013) “Our aim is to review the literature supporting a possible role of perceptual learning (PL) in helping to solve the puzzle called DD (Developmental Dyslexia). PL is defined as improvement of perceptual skills with practice. Based on the previous literature showing how PL is able to selectively change visual abilities, we here propose to use PL to improve the impaired visual functions characterizing DD and, in particular, the visual deficits that could be developmentally related to an early magnocellular-dorsal pathway and selective attention dysfunction. The crucial visual attention deficits that are causally linked to DD could be, indeed, strongly reduced by training the magnocellular-dorsal pathway with the PL, and learning to read for children with DD would not be anymore such a difficult task. This new remediation approach – not involving any phonological or orthographic training – could be also used to develop new prevention programs for pre-reading children at DD risk”.
  • 45.  Visual factors such as refractive error (eg. long-sight or hyperopia over +1.00DS) and vergence facility (binocular co-ordination) whilst not considered to be a basis for dyslexia, are significantly related to reading (see graph below) and so should not be overlooked. Quaid & Simpson (2003)  All students with a significant learning problem should have a thorough optometric assessment before starting vision training.
  • 46.  Havn’t we disproved the perceptual theory or shown that perceptual factors only play a minor role? Vellutino et al (2004), Ramus (2002)  There are numerous studies to suggest that vision is not a factor in reading problems or dyslexia and one can use these to support an argument against the role of vision. In the end, one must critically examine each of the studies to see if their conclusions hold.  In the 80’s it was believed that erratic eye movements observed in dyslexia were due to poor reading skills. The methods used to study eye movements however were not adequate to draw such conclusions. Using current scientific methods it can be shown that a problem exists with eye movement control in dyslexia independently of language.  Another reason for finding a lack of significance can be due to “ceiling effects” (eg. when the test is not sensitive enough to discriminate between two groups) This was argued by Fletcher & Satz (1979) when we moved away from the perceptual model.  A study may also show no significant difference because of the way a group is selected for testing. Eg. giving all dyslexic students tinted lenses.  Finally, a study may show no difference because of the manner in which a treatment is applied. An example might be the Perceptual Motor Programme (PMP) used in many NZ schools. A study by Klomp (2012) found no effect on learning outcomes for young primary school children. The study was done in just 10 weeks and targets a gross level of perceptual motor development. In order to show a significant improvement in academic learning however would require that the perceptual motor development be improved to a level necessary to support academic skills.
  • 47.  The position statement by the American Academy of Ophthalmology (2014) claims that “children with dyslexia or related learning disabilities have the same visual function and ocular health as children without such conditions” .  The evidence clearly shows however that students with dyslexia or related learning disabilities do NOT have the same visual function as those without such conditions! Eden (1995), Kulp,Schmidt (1996), Kulp (1999), Maples (2003), Valdois (2004), Goldstand (2005), Solan (2007), Shin (2009), Bosse (2009), Chen (2011), Franceshini (2012), Pienaar (2013)  They also argue that since children with dyslexia like to play computer games (a highly visual task) that vision cannot be a factor.  This is not supported by any evidence. The eye movements used for computer games are NOT the same as those used for reading books. Also, the nature of the visual tasks found in gaming are quite different from reading text.  They claim there is inadequate scientific evidence to support visual interventions.  In fact there is quite a lot of evidence to support visual interventions as shown by this review but there are not many randomized placebo controlled trials (studies providing the highest level of evidence). This does not mean that visual interventions are not “evidence based” however as most medical treatments also fail to meet this standard. The basis for speech therapy, occupational therapy and rehabilitative medicine is that the brain is plastic and can be trained. The evidence showing that vision can be trained is at least as good. If this is accepted in other fields, one must accept the possibility that vision can also be trained.
  • 48.  A study by Olulade et al (2013) was widely promoted in the media claiming to rule out vision as a factor in dyslexia.  The study showed that visual motion activity measured using fMRI brain scans to be less in dyslexic students compared with non-dyslexic students of the same age.  However when dyslexics were compared with younger non-dyslexic students of the same reading age then the visual activity was about the same.  Furthermore, when dyslexics are given reading intervention visual activity increased suggesting that any failure in vision was due to inadequate reading exposure.  These findings taken together are used to support the Phonological Model.  This finding argues the point that the visual activity is normal relative to reading age and that reduced visual activity is therefore a function of low reading exposure.  Another explanation however is that the reduced reading age of dyslexics is a function of their poor visual development! The lower the visual activity the lower the reading age. This being the case we would expect that improving visual development would improve the reading age by removing visual barriers to reading.  The fact that reading practice improves visual activity is not too surprising since there may be a level of “reciprocal effect” in much the same way there is with phonological processing and reading practice.
  • 49.  There exists a large body of evidence to show that visual skills are related to reading ability and that visual problems are linked with dyslexia.  Much of the evidence points towards a problem with the development of visual skills supported by the Magnocellular Pathway, however general optometric findings should also be considered for individual students.  Although more studies exist to show a phonological link (due to the higher level of attention it has received) there are limitations to the Phonological Model.  The evidence that visual skills can be improved with training is substantial.  More placebo controlled studies showing the efficacy of visual treatments for dyslexia are needed but this does not negate the many controlled studies and clinical reports currently available or preclude it as an evidence-based intervention.  Without intervention the gap in visual skills will often persist despite maturation.  The view that vision problems do not contribute to the symptoms observed in dyslexia or general learning disabilities seems very unlikely.  Given the currently available evidence a more balanced approach may be to use visual (and auditory) interventions to complement traditional therapies.
  • 50. Other Presentations: The Visual Model of Reading iCept – For Students with Learning Delays iCept Screening - Information for Schools