SlideShare a Scribd company logo
submit your work following the file naming convention
FirstInitial.LastName_M01.docx. For example,
J.Smith_M01.docx. Remember that it is not necessary to
manually type in the file extension; it will automatically
append.
Start by reading and following these instructions:
1. Quickly skim the questions or assignment below and the
assignment rubric to help you focus.
2. Read the required chapter(s) of the textbook and any
additional recommended resources. Some answers may require
you to do additional research on the Internet or in other
reference sources. Choose your sources carefully.
3. Consider the discussions and the any insights gained from it.
4. Create your Assignment submission and be sure to cite your
sources, use APA style as required, check your spelling.
Assignment:
Professional Development Assignment Description:
There are 4 parts to the assignment. Please place all four
assignments into one paper. The different parts should be
clearly marked so your professor knows which question you are
addressing.
1. Rewrite the following essays. Correct all errors in
capitalization, spelling, and punctuation. Divide the passage
into appropriate paragraphs.
· Essay #1:
When my husband Joe had cancer surgery five years ago, each
of his family members responded just as I knew they would.
John, his father, decided to organize the family’s calls. Because,
of course, everything would run so much more smoothly. Thus
Jane, Matt, and Jim received detailed sheets of instructions in
the mail. Telling them which days to telephone r.j. smith
hospital to talk to Joe and what presents to send. Jane, enraged,
promptly threw a tantrum. Calling Matt and me to complain
about her father’s overbearing behavior. “I,” she yelled, “am a
Psychiatrist who knows how to handle these situations, i am not
still a child.” Matt also responded predictably. By avoiding the
situation. He threw himself into his work. Normally a late
sleeper, Matt took to leaving at 5:00 a.m., driving on the
deserted expressway and arriving at work before six a.m. In
addition, he didn’t return until 11:00 p.m. When he would fall
into bed so exhausted that he couldn’t worry about
Joe. Jim, too, responded predictably. He fumed inside for
weeks, ignored John’s instructions, and sent cartons of books to
Joe. So that he would never be bored. The books were funny.
Because Jim had read Norman Cousins’ book about the healing
power of laughter. Within a few months, Joe recovered from
the surgery-inspite of his family
· Essay #2:
Treatment of atrial septal defect depends upon the size and
symptoms and therefore is individualized an atrial septal defect
of less than 3mm usually closes spontaneously (The Merck
Manual, 2006). When the defect is between 3mm and 8mm it
closes spontaneously in eighty percent of cases by the age of
eighteen months, however, atrial septal defects located in the
anteroinferior aspect of the septum (ostium primum) or in the
posterior aspect of the septum near the superior vena cava or
inferior vena cava (sinus venosus) don’t close spontaneously. If
the defect is very small does not close spontaneously and the
patient is asymptomatic. The treatment may be simply too
monitor via an annual echocardiogram. Of course their is a risk
of patients’ becoming symptomatic.
Moderate-sized atrial septal defects or larger or patients’ who
are symptomatic require closure of the shunt this is usually
done between the ages of 2 and 6 years. A catheter-delivered
closure device, such as Amplatzer Septal Occluder or Cardio-
Seal device. May be used for closure of atrial septal defects less
than 13mm in size, except than primum or
sinus venosus defects. If the defect is greater than
thirteen milimeters or located near important structures.
Surgical repair becomes necessary. If the atrial septal defect is
repaired during childhood there mortality rates approach 0 and
the patient’s life expectancy approaches that of the general
population prior to surgical repair, patients may need to be
treated with diuretics; digoxin; ACE inhibitor; or beta blockers
to prevent congestive heart failure (Moser & Riegel, 2007).
Following surgical repair patience will receive aspirin to
prevent clots, and be monitored closely for dysrhythmias and
pulmonary hypertension. Oxygen and nitric oxide therapy have
proven to be beneficial in treating postoperative pulmonary
hypertension. Also patients who have primum atrial septal
defect will need endocarditis prophylaxis.
2. Guided writing exercise:
· Think about a recent experience you have had that required
you to use critical thinking skills. Set a timer for five minutes.
Write about your experience. Do not worry about grammar,
punctuation, or spelling. Just write, but stop at five minutes.
Now go back and write your experience with correct grammar,
punctuation, and spelling. Submit both written pieces. Add a
concluding paragraph that answers the follows questions: How
was the formal writing experience different from the timed
writing experience? What did you do differently? Was there any
difference in the time it took you? How did you ensure your
spelling, grammar, and punctuation were correct?
3. Describe when the following words would be used in a
scholarly essay (define each word). Then, use those that are
appropriate to a scholarly essay in a sentence.
· Their, there, they’re
· Affect, effect
· Its, it’s
· your, you’re
· then, than
· accept, except
4. Describe the use of apostrophes in writing in general. When
should apostrophes be used in formal writing? When should
apostrophes be avoided in formal writing?
DOI: 10.1126/science.1198364
, 1447 (2011);331 Science
, et al.Gregory M. Walton
Outcomes of Minority Students
A Brief Social-Belonging Intervention Improves Academic and
Health
This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only.
clicking here.colleagues, clients, or customers by
, you can order high-quality copies for yourIf you wish to
distribute this article to others
here.following the guidelines
can be obtained byPermission to republish or repurpose articles
or portions of articles
): April 14, 2011 www.sciencemag.org (this infomation is
current as of
The following resources related to this article are available
online at
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html
version of this article at:
including high-resolution figures, can be found in the
onlineUpdated information and services,
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2011/03/15/331.6023.
1447.DC1.html
can be found at: Supporting Online Material
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html#ref
-list-1
, 8 of which can be accessed free:cites 23 articlesThis article
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/psychology
Psychology
subject collections:This article appears in the following
registered trademark of AAAS.
is aScience2011 by the American Association for the
Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title
CopyrightAmerican Association for the Advancement of
Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005.
(print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published
weekly, except the last week in December, by theScience
o
n
A
p
ri
l 1
4
,
2
0
1
1
w
w
w
.s
ci
e
n
ce
m
a
g
.o
rg
D
o
w
n
lo
a
d
e
d
f
ro
m
http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl
http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html
http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html#ref
-list-1
http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/psychology
http://www.sciencemag.org/
units (fig. S9) [paired t test, P < 0.02, n = 19 pairs;
median distances from the granular layer were 0.45
mm (source units) and 1.05 mm (target units)].
The present study demonstrated that canoni-
cal feed-forward signal flow across cortical layers
during sensory coding reverse to the feed-back
direction during memory retrieval phase, which sug-
gests flexible recruitment of interlaminar connectivity
depending on the cognitive demands in the monkey
association cortices (Fig. 4C). We used CSD anal-
ysis to estimate cortical layers (Fig. 1, C to E, and
fig. S1), and the observed stimulus-evoked CSD
profiles were quite similar to those in the primary
sensory cortices (17, 27). For some penetrations,
we observed that the current sink positioned super-
ficially next to the earliest-sink contact exhibited
larger peak amplitudes and much longer durations
thanthatoftheearliestcurrentsink.Thisobservation
might reflect the cytoarchitectural nature of A36 as
a dysgranular cortex (28) as well as the direct inputs
to the deepest part of the superficial layer, which is
consistent with anatomical observations (29).
A recent study in the rat primary auditory cor-
tex demonstrated that the direction of interlam-
inar signal flow depends on the cortical “state”:
Sensory-evoked responses were initiated in the
thalamorecipient layers and then propagated to the
superficial and deep layers, whereas in spontane-
ously active “up-states,” neuronal activity was
initiated in the deep layers and then propagated to
the superficial layers (27). These state-dependent
changes in the interlaminar signal flows in rats
are consistent with our results obtained in mon-
keys performing a memory task. Together, these
findings highlight the flexibility of cortical lam-
inar circuits. Further experiments will be needed
to determine whether such flexible interlaminar
connectivity is also implemented and used in
other cortical areas for other cognitive demands.
References and Notes
1. L. R. Squire, J. T. Wixted, R. E. Clark, Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
8, 872 (2007).
2. W. A. Suzuki, Neuron 61, 657 (2009).
3. E. A. Murray, T. J. Bussey, L. M. Saksida, Annu. Rev.
Neurosci. 30, 99 (2007).
4. Y. Miyashita, Science 306, 435 (2004).
5. Y. Naya, M. Yoshida, Y. Miyashita, J. Neurosci. 23, 2861
(2003).
6. N. K. Logothetis, D. L. Sheinberg, Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
19, 577 (1996).
7. G. Buzsáki, Neuron 68, 362 (2010).
8. S. H. Wang, R. G. Morris, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 49, C1
(2010).
9. M. P. Witter, E. I. Moser, Trends Neurosci. 29, 671 (2006).
10. A. Bollimunta, Y. Chen, C. E. Schroeder, M. Ding, J.
Neurosci.
28, 9976 (2008).
11. A. K. Engel, P. Fries, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 156
(2010).
12. I. E. Ohiorhenuan et al., Nature 466, 617 (2010).
13. R. J. Douglas, K. A. Martin, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 419
(2004).
14. J. J. Nassi, E. M. Callaway, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 360
(2009).
15. R. C. Reid, J. M. Alonso, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 475
(1996).
16. J. Csicsvari, B. Jamieson, K. D. Wise, G. Buzsáki, Neuron
37, 311 (2003).
17. C. E. Schroeder, P. Lakatos, Trends Neurosci. 32, 9 (2009).
18. A. K. Engel, W. Singer, Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 16 (2001).
19. T. Hirabayashi, Y. Miyashita, J. Neurosci. 25, 10299
(2005).
20. M. Tomita, J. J. Eggermont, J. Neurophysiol. 93, 378
(2005).
21. C. A. Atencio, C. E. Schreiner, PLoS ONE 5, e9521 (2010).
22. T. Hirabayashi, D. Takeuchi, K. Tamura, Y. Miyashita,
J. Neurosci. 30, 10407 (2010).
23. J. M. Alonso, L. M. Martinez, Nat. Neurosci. 1, 395 (1998).
24. Materials and methods are available as supporting
material on Science Online.
25. S. Lefort, C. Tomm, J. C. Floyd Sarria, C. C. H. Petersen,
Neuron 61, 301 (2009).
26. N. Weiler, L. Wood, J. I. Yu, S. A. Solla, G. M. G.
Shepherd, Nat. Neurosci. 11, 360 (2008).
27. S. Sakata, K. D. Harris, Neuron 64, 404 (2009).
28. W. A. Suzuki, D. G. Amaral, J. Comp. Neurol. 463, 67
(2003).
29. K. S. Saleem, K. Tanaka, J. Neurosci. 16, 4757 (1996).
30. This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Specially
Promoted Research from the Ministry of Education,
Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) to Y.M.
(19002010) and Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists from
MEXT to T.H. (18700378); a grant from Takeda Science
Foundation to Y.M.; and Japan Society for the Promotion
of Science (JSPS) Research Fellowships for Young Scientists
to D.T. (1811234) and K.T. (211438). This work was also
supported in part by Global Center of Excellence Program
from MEXT. We thank M. Takeda for discussions and
comments and H. Kasahara, R. Fujimichi, T. Matsui, and
K. W. Koyano for advice and help with experiments.
Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/331/6023/1443/DC1
Materials and Methods
SOM Text
Figs. S1 to S9
Tables S1 and S2
References
3 November 2010; accepted 8 February 2011
10.1126/science.1199967
A Brief Social-Belonging Intervention
Improves Academic and Health
Outcomes of Minority Students
Gregory M. Walton1* and Geoffrey L. Cohen1,2
A brief intervention aimed at buttressing college freshmen’s
sense of social belonging in school was
tested in a randomized controlled trial (N = 92), and its
academic and health-related consequences over
3 years are reported. The intervention aimed to lessen
psychological perceptions of threat on campus
by framing social adversity as common and transient. It used
subtle attitude-change strategies to lead
participants to self-generate the intervention message. The
intervention was expected to be particularly
beneficial to African-American students (N = 49), a stereotyped
and socially marginalized group in
academics, and less so to European-American students (N = 43).
Consistent with these expectations,
over the 3-year observation period the intervention raised
African Americans’ grade-point average (GPA)
relative to multiple control groups and halved the minority
achievement gap. This performance boost
was mediated by the effect of the intervention on subjective
construal: It prevented students from
seeing adversity on campus as an indictment of their belonging.
Additionally, the intervention
improved African Americans’ self-reported health and well-
being and reduced their reported number
of doctor visits 3 years postintervention. Senior-year surveys
indicated no awareness among participants
of the intervention's impact. The results suggest that social
belonging is a psychological lever where
targeted intervention can have broad consequences that lessen
inequalities in achievement and health.
A
n important question facing society con-
cerns the origins of inequalities between
socially marginalized and nonmarginal-
ized groups. Among the most consequential of
inequalities is the poorer school and health out-
comes experienced by African Americans, Latino
Americans, and other non-Asian ethnic minor-
ities relative to European Americans. These dif-
ferences occur at all levels of socioeconomic
status (1–3).
Although many structural factors contribute
to these inequalities, the present research exam-
ines a psychological factor: concern about social
belonging. Social belonging—a sense of having
positive relationships with others—is a fundamen-
tal human need (4, 5). Social isolation, loneliness,
and low social status harm not only subjective
well-being (6) but also intellectual achievement
(7) and immune function and health (8–11). Even
a single instance of exclusion can undermine well-
being (12, 13), intelligence quotient (IQ) test per-
formance, and self-control (14).
Members of socially stigmatized groups, such
as African Americans, may be relatively more
uncertain about their social belonging in main-
stream institutions like school and work (7). Be-
cause their ethnic group is often negatively
stereotyped and marginalized, they may be un-
sure of whether they will be fully included in
positive social relationships in these settings (2).
As the sociologist Erving Goffman wrote, “The
central feature of the stigmatized individual’s sit-
uation in life…is a question of…‘acceptance’”
(15). Uncertainty about belonging, especially when
chronic, can undermine minorities’ performance
(7, 16) and health (3, 17, 18). Social belonging
may thus constitute a psychological lever where
targeted intervention could yield broad benefits.
1Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA
94305, USA. 2School of Education and Graduate School of
Business, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
[email protected]
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 18 MARCH 2011
1447
REPORTS
o
n
A
p
ri
l 1
4
,
2
0
1
1
w
w
w
.s
ci
e
n
ce
m
a
g
.o
rg
D
o
w
n
lo
a
d
e
d
f
ro
m
http://www.sciencemag.org/
Such an intervention is reported here. Critical
to its rationale is the insight that it is people’s
subjective interpretations of the quality of their
relationships, more so than the objective number
or attributes of those relationships, that strongly
affects well-being (5, 19). The present interven-
tion, delivered to students during the challenging
transition to college, was designed to encourage
nonthreatening interpretations of adversity. During
the transition to a new school, students can face
frequent social setbacks and feelings of isolation.
Their well-being and performance may depend, to
a great extent, on whether they construe such
experiences as evidence that they do not belong.
Because African-American students experi-
ence relatively greater uncertainty about their
belonging in school, they were expected to ben-
efit from the intervention more than nonminority
students (7). Further if, as we intended, the in-
tervention triggered an enduring perceptual change
in the encoding of social experience, its effects
might persist over time. Short-term effects might
compound into long-term effects through a re-
cursive virtuous cycle, in which early perform-
ance gains assure students of their belonging in
school, which in turn improves their performance,
in a repeating feedback loop (20). Students who
feel more assured of their belonging may also
initiate more social interactions and form better
relationships on campus, facilitating their social
integration and further benefiting their well-
being, performance, and health (21).
The intervention was delivered to two cohorts
of African-American (N = 49) and European-
American (N = 43) students in the second semes-
ter of their first year at a selective college (22, 23).
To assess psychological responses to adversity,
we asked participants to complete daily surveys
in the first week after the intervention. To assess
their long-term sense of belonging, health, and
well-being, we asked them to complete an end-
of-college survey 3 years later (completion rate
78.26%) (23). At the end of this survey, partici-
pants were asked to authorize the release of their
complete college academic transcript (authoriza-
tion rate 97.22%) (23).
Participating students were randomly as-
signed to either the belonging-treatment condi-
tion or a control condition. In cohort 1, participants
were recruited through convenience sampling; in
cohort 2, through random sampling (23). An
additional campus-wide control group was ob-
tained by collecting the anonymized official
grade-point averages (GPAs) of all European
Americans (N = 1362) and African Americans
(N = 194) in the same class years as participants
but who did not participate in the study (23).
This group was included in secondary analyses
of GPA.
The intervention provided students with a
narrative that framed social adversity in school as
shared and short-lived (24). This message en-
couraged students to attribute adversity not to
fixed deficits unique to themselves or their ethnic
group but to common and transient aspects of the
college-adjustment process. Upon arrival in a
research lab, participants read a report of the
ostensible results of a survey of more senior
students at their school. Most students, the report
indicated, had worried about whether they be-
longed in college during the difficult first year but
grew confident in their belonging with time. The
survey results were said to be consistent across
ethnic and gender groups. For instance, one sur-
vey respondent was quoted as saying, “Freshman
year even though I met large numbers of people, I
didn’t have a small group of close friends…I was
pretty homesick, and I had to remind myself that
making close friends takes time. Since then…I
have met people some of whom are now just as
close as my friends in high school were” (23).
Concerns about belonging were thus represented
as common at first, as temporary, and as due to
the challenging nature of the college transition.
To encourage participants to internalize the
message, several steps exploited the “saying-
is-believing effect”—the tendency to endorse
messages that one has freely advocated (25). Par-
ticipants were asked to write an essay describing
how their own experiences in college echoed the
experiences summarized in the survey. They
then turned their essay into a speech, which they
delivered to a video camera. These materials,
participants were told, would be shown to future
students to help ease their transition to college.
Beyond facilitating internalization, this procedure
averted the potential stigma of receiving an
intervention, because it encouraged participants
to see themselves as benefactors and not as
beneficiaries (24, 26). In the control condition,
the procedure was the same but the survey ad-
dressed topics unrelated to belonging (e.g., change
in social-political attitudes) (23). The intervention
lasted about 1 hour.
Few analyses were moderated by cohort (i.e.,
no more than would be expected by chance alone
and none involving the primary outcomes of
GPA, health, or well-being). Thus, data from the
two cohorts were combined to increase statistical
power. First, analyses examined the trajectory of
students’ official GPA over time. In contrast to all
other groups, African Americans in the control
group showed no improvement in GPA from the
fall of their freshman year, the semester before
the intervention, through their sophomore, junior,
and senior years [linear trend F < 1]. By contrast,
the GPAs of intervention-treated African Amer-
icans rose over time [for linear trend, F(1,34) =
13.79, P= 0.0007; for time × condition, F(1,34) =
4.16, P= 0.049]. The GPAs of European-American
students also rose over time [F(1,29) = 6.88, P =
0.014] with no difference by condition [F < 1].
As Fig. 1 shows, the intervention set African
Americans on an upward trajectory such that the
gap between them and their European Americans
classmates closed over time. By students’ senior
year, the gap was cut by 79% (23).
Multiple regression, with student gender
controlled, tested the effect of student race and
condition (randomized control versus social-
belonging treatment) on change in GPA—mean
postintervention GPA (sophomore through senior
years) minus mean preintervention GPA (fall
term, first year) (23). There was no condition
effect on preintervention GPA for either racial
group [t values < 1] (table S1) (23). However, a
significant condition effect on change in GPA
emerged for African Americans [B = 0.30, t(65) =
2.54, P = 0.014] with no effect for European
Americans[t<1][race×conditionB=–0.43, t(65)=
–2.41, P = 0.019]. Virtually identical results were
obtained when preintervention GPA was used as
a covariate in an analysis of postintervention
GPA [treatment effect for African Americans, B =
0.24, t(64) = 2.65, P = 0.010; treatment effect for
European Americans, t < 1; race × condition: B =
–0.31, t(64) = –2.27, P = 0.027]. The intervention
closed the minority gap in 3-year GPA (SD =
0.36) from 0.29 points in the control condition to
0.14 points in the treatment condition, a 52%
reduction.
Adding the campus-wide sample further sup-
ports treatment efficacy. An agreement with uni-
Fig. 1. Raw GPA by
student race, experimen-
tal condition, and aca-
demic term. Means are
noncumulative and were
combined across cohorts.
Ranges in sample sizes
and standard errors for
European Americans are
N = 25 to 33 and SE =
0.08 to 0.14; for African
Americans, N = 30 to 37
and SE = 0.09 to 0.12.
3.00
3.10
3.20
3.30
3.40
3.50
3.60
3.70
3.80
3.90
4.00
Fall Term Fall Term Spring Term Fall Term Spring Term Fall
Term Spring Term
First Year Sophomore Year Junior Year Senior Year
Treatment
European Americans, Social-Belonging Treatment
African Americans, Social-Belonging Treatment
African Americans, Randomized Control Group
European Americans, Randomized Control Group
Social-Belonging
18 MARCH 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1448
REPORTS
o
n
A
p
ri
l 1
4
,
2
0
1
1
w
w
w
.s
ci
e
n
ce
m
a
g
.o
rg
D
o
w
n
lo
a
d
e
d
f
ro
m
http://www.sciencemag.org/
versity officials precludes the reporting of raw or
adjusted means in this sample. To honor this
agreement but present the results graphically, we
performed analyses on residual postintervention
GPA with preintervention GPA and gender con-
trolled. Multiple regression on change in GPA
and on raw postintervention GPA with preinter-
vention GPA included as a covariate yield vir-
tually identical results (23). As shown in Fig. 2A,
treated African Americans had higher residual
GPA scores than did African Americans campus-
wide [B = 0.28, t(1620) = 3.97, P = 0.00008] and
African Americans in therandomized controlgroup
[B = 0.24, t(1620) = 2.62, P = 0.009]. The latter
two groups did not differ [t < 1]. (Fig. 2A) (23).
Illustrating its broad impact, the interven-
tion tripled the percentage of African Americans
earning postintervention GPAs in the top 25%
of their class, as measured by both residual and
raw postintervention GPA, and tended to reduce
the percentage of African Americans performing
in the bottom 25% of their class on both indices
(Fig. 2, B and C) (23).
What accounts for these treatment effects?
Daily surveys, collected the week after the in-
tervention, suggest that the intervention buffered
African Americans against adversity (23). Among
untreated African Americans, feelings of be-
longing in school rose and fell with the degree
of adversity students reported having experienced
earlier that day and the day before. As adversity
rose, belonging fell (mean within-subjects R =
–0.45, derived from the average of individual
participants’ within-subjects correlations, after
each was subjected to a Fisher r-to-z trans-
formation) (23). For treated African Americans,
this relationship was reduced to nil [mean within-
subjects R = 0.01], a significant reduction [t(59) =
2.99, P = 0.004]. In summary, the intervention
robbed adversity of its symbolic meaning for
African Americans, untethering their sense of
belonging from daily hardship (27). Like treated
African Americans, European Americans showed
little relationship between adversity and belonging
[for both conditions, mean within-subjects R =
–0.09; condition difference, t < 1] [race × con-
dition: t(59) = –2.04, P = 0.046].
These results provide a window into the shift
in African-American students’ psychology caused
by the intervention. This shift benefited their
long-term performance. African Americans whose
belonging was more robust to daily adversity—
whose sense of belonging was relatively inde-
pendent of their day-to-day adversity—showed
greater improvement in their 3-year postinter-
vention GPA [R = 0.51, P = 0.001] (23). The
effect of the intervention in protecting African-
Americans students’sense of belonging from daily
adversity mediated its effect on their GPA (23).
The intervention thus planted a change in social
perception that, it appears, accompanied students
long after the intervention ended to affect their
performance in college.
Three years after the intervention, at the end
of their college tenure, participants completed a
survey to assess long-term effects on psychology,
well-being, and health. Also, to assess whether
the intervention operated beneath conscious aware-
ness, we asked participants whether they remem-
bered the intervention from 3 years previously,
whether they thought it had affected them, and
whether they agreed with its message. On no
outcome did European Americans differ by con-
dition [t values < 1.35, P values > 0.18]. African
Americans, however, showed consistent treat-
ment effects. The race × condition interaction
was not always significant, indicating that the
treatment effect was not always larger for African
Americans than for European Americans. Degrees
of freedom vary because some measures were
completed only by participants in cohort 2 (23).
If the intervention lessened how much Afri-
can Americans’ belonging fluctuated with adver-
sity, and if it did so by lessening how much they
viewed campus life through the lens of race, then
intervention-treated African Americans should
(i) report greater stability and less uncertainty
about their belonging in school {less agreement
with items like, “When something bad happens, I
feel that maybe I don’t belong at [school name]”}
(7) and (ii) exhibit less cognitive accessibility of
negative racial stereotypes and self-doubt (23).
They did [self-reported belonging uncertainty,
t(36) = –2.01, P = 0.052; accessibility of negative
racial stereotypes, t(66) = –2.01, P = 0.049;
accessibility of self-doubt, t(64) = –2.64, P =
0.010] (Fig. 3) (23).
Given the importance of social belonging for
reducing stress and improving immune function
and physical health (5, 8–11, 19) and the rel-
atively poorer health experienced by African
Americans, even those high in socioeconomic
status (3), we examined effects on health. We
assessed self-reported health, an important pre-
dictor of morbidity and mortality (28), using the
five-item general health component of the Med-
ical Outcomes Study Short-Form Health Survey
(23). We also asked participants how many times
they had visited the doctor in the previous 3
months (cohort 1) or 1 month (cohort 2). African
Americans reported being healthier and visit-
ing the doctor less frequently in the treatment
condition than in the control condition [t(32) =
2.48, P = 0.019 and t(63) = –2.23, P = 0.030,
Fig. 2. Cumulative academic
performance from sophomore
through senior year. Data were
combined across cohorts. (A) Resid-
ual sophomore-through-senior-
year GPAs adjusted for student
gender and preintervention (fall
term, first year) GPA. Error bars
represent T1 SE. Means repre-
sent the degree to which stu-
dents performed better (positive
values) or worse (negative val-
ues) than expected after the in-
tervention in GPA units based on
their gender and preintervention
performance. (B) Percentage of
students in the top and bottom
25% of their college class in
residual postintervention GPA
(i.e., postintervention GPA ad-
justed for student gender and
preintervention GPA). (C) Percent-
age of students in the top and
bottom 25% of their college class
in raw postintervention GPA. For analytic details, see (23).
Sample sizes for European Americans areNcampus-wide control
group = 1362and Nexperimental groups = 33; for African
Americans, Ncampus-wide control group = 194 and
Nexperimental groups = 37.
-0.25
-0.20
-0.15
-0.10
-0.05
0.00
0.05
0.10
0.15
0.20
0.25
European
American
Students
African
American
Students
A Mean Residual
Postintervention GPA
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
In Top of
Class
In Bottom
of Class
In Top of
Class
In Bottom
of Class
European American
Students
African American
Students
B In Residual Postintervention GPA
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
In Top of
Class
In Bottom
of Class
In Top of
Class
In Bottom
of Class
European American
Students
African American
Students
C In Raw Postintervention GPA
Campus-Wide Control Group
Randomized Control Group
Social-Belonging Treatment
Percentage of Students in Top and Bottom 25%
of College Class
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 18 MARCH 2011
1449
REPORTS
o
n
A
p
ri
l 1
4
,
2
0
1
1
w
w
w
.s
ci
e
n
ce
m
a
g
.o
rg
D
o
w
n
lo
a
d
e
d
f
ro
m
http://www.sciencemag.org/
respectively] (Fig. 4, A and B). Whereas 60% of
untreated African Americans had seen a doctor
recently, only 28% of treated African Americans
had [c2(1, N = 38) = 3.98, P = 0.046]. The race
gap in self-reported health was eliminated in the
treatment condition; interestingly, there was no
gap for doctor visits (23). Future research should
examine whether these patterns generalize to
physiological and physical indicators of health
(9) to assess the robustness of the effect beyond
self-report outcomes and to identify biological
pathways (11).
As further evidence of improved well-being,
African Americans also scored higher on the
Subjective Happiness Scale (23) [t(35) = 2.61, P =
0.013] (Fig. 4C). The happiness gap with Euro-
pean Americans higher than African Americans
disappeared in the treatment condition (23). The
finding of a lasting positive impact on subjective
happiness is noteworthy in light of research
showing that individual happiness is relatively
stable (6).
Participants were unaware of the interven-
tion’s effect, suggesting that its efficacy did not
depend on conscious awareness. Most students
reported that they remembered participating in
the study 3 years earlier (79% did). But when
asked to describe “the most memorable and
important” information they had learned in the
study, few recalled the key content of the survey
they had read (8% did), and few reported that the
study had had “any” effect on their college ex-
perience (14% did) (table S3). There was no
condition difference on any of these outcomes for
African Americans [c2(1, N = 37 to 38) values <
1.40, P values > 0.20]; treated African Americans
ascribed no more effect to the study than un-
treated African Americans. However, indirect mea-
sures of recall and beliefs did show treatment
effects. When asked to “guess” the process of
change described in the survey they had read,
more treated than untreated African Americans
wrote that it concerned how students’ social ex-
periences in college improve over time (50%
versus 20%) [c2(1, N = 38) = 3.79, P = 0.052].
Additionally, treated students endorsed this mes-
sage. When asked to describe their own experi-
ences,moretreatedthanuntreatedAfricanAmericans
volunteered that their own social experiences in
college had improved over time (50% versus 20%)
[c2(1, N = 38) = 3.79, P= 0.052]. The subtle nature
of this intervention, with its influence occurring
outside conscious awareness (29), may contribute
to its efficacy. In some cases, conscious aware-
ness may undo the effects of an intervention (30).
More overt interventions risk sending the stig-
matizing message that the beneficiaries are seen
as in need of help. They may also cause resist-
ance and reactance and undermine the sense of
accomplishment people take in their success (26).
This study provides an experimental, longi-
tudinal demonstration that a brief intervention to
buttress feelings of social belonging can have
significant effects on a wide range of important
outcomes. The social-belonging intervention im-
proved the academic performance, self-reported
health, and well-being of ethnic minority students
over 3 years. The results suggest that inequality
between marginalized and nonmarginalized groups
arises not only from structural factors but also
from concern about social belonging.
This concern can be mitigated by using a
psychological remedy. The intervention provided
students a nonthreatening frame for interpreting
the daily challenges of school. By encouraging
students to adopt this message as their own, the
intervention made this message stick psycholog-
ically. Along with other recent research, this study
highlights how the impact of adversity depends
on its perceived meaning—how it is subjective-
ly construed (24–26, 31–33). Changing subjec-
tive construal is a fruitful avenue for intervention
because many events are ambiguous and ame-
nable to multiple interpretations. Moreover, a
change in construal can become self-reinforcing.
Students who feel confident in their belonging
may experience the social world in a way that re-
inforces this feeling. They may initiate more rela-
tionships and thus obtain more opportunities for
belonging and growth. Brief interventions that
shore up belonging can thus promote performance
0.0
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
2.5
3.0
3.5
4.0
European
American
Students
African
American
Students
B Accessibility of
Negative Racial
Stereotypes
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
European
American
Students
African
American
Students
A Self-Reported
Belonging Uncertainty
0.0
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
2.5
3.0
3.5
European
American
Students
African
American
Students
C Accessibility of
Self-Doubt
RasdgasgRandomized Control Group Social-Belonging
Treatment
Fig. 3. Self-reported belonging uncertainty and the cognitive
accessibility of negative racial stereotypes
and of self-doubt 3 years postintervention. Error bars represent
T1 SE. Data were combined across cohorts
where measures were completed by both cohorts. (A) Self-
reported belonging uncertainty (cohort 2). (B)
Accessibility of negative racial stereotypes (cohorts 1 and 2).
(C) Accessibility of self-doubt (cohorts 1 and
2). The y axis in (A) represents the full range of the scale. The
y axes in (B) and (C) represent about 3.00
standard deviations. Sample sizes in cohort 2 only are
NEuropean Americans = 20 and NAfrican Americans = 23.
Sample sizes in cohorts 1 and 2 are NEuropean Americans = 31
and NAfrican Americans = 38.
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
European
American
Students
African
American
Students
A
Self-Assessed
General Health
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
European
American
Students
African
American
Students
C
Subjective
Happiness
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1.25
1.50
European
American
Students
African
American
Students
B Number of Reported
Doctor Visits in the
Previous Month
RasdgasgRandomized Control Group Social-Belonging
Treatment
Fig. 4. Self-reported health and happiness 3 years
postintervention. Error bars represent T1 SE. Data
were combined across cohorts where measures were completed
by both cohorts. (A) Self-assessed general
health (cohort 2). (B) Reported doctor visits in the previous
month (cohorts 1 and 2). (C) Subjective
happiness (cohort 2). The y axes in (A) and (C) represent the
full range of each scale. The y axis in (B)
represents about 2.00 standard deviations. Sample sizes in
cohort 2 only are NEuropean Americans = 20 and
NAfrican Americans = 23. Sample sizes in cohorts 1 and 2 are
NEuropean Americans = 31 and NAfrican Americans = 38.
18 MARCH 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1450
REPORTS
o
n
A
p
ri
l 1
4
,
2
0
1
1
w
w
w
.s
ci
e
n
ce
m
a
g
.o
rg
D
o
w
n
lo
a
d
e
d
f
ro
m
http://www.sciencemag.org/
and well-being even long after their delivery
(7, 20, 25, 31, 34).
Importantly, the effectiveness of social-
psychological interventions depends on factors
in the context. Such interventions are unlikely to
be effective in contexts without opportunities for
learning. Also, because the present intervention
works by changing people’s subjective interpre-
tation of ambiguous events, it may be ineffective
in openly hostile environments. Lastly, whether
this intervention would work among younger or
less-select students, or students from other mar-
ginalized groups, is an important question for
future research (20, 31, 34). These qualifica-
tions noted, the results underscore the impor-
tance of social belonging and subjective construal
in contributing to social inequality and show
how this insight can inform our collective efforts
to promote equality in performance, health, and
well-being.
References and Notes
1. K. Magnuson, J. Waldfogel, Steady Gains and Stalled
Progress: Inequality and the Black-White Test Score Gap
(Russell Sage Foundation, New York, 2008).
2. C. M. Steele, S. J. Spencer, J. Aronson, in Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed.
(Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2002), pp. 379–440.
3. D. R. Williams, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 896, 173 (1999).
4. R. F. Baumeister, M. R. Leary, Psychol. Bull. 117, 497
(1995).
5. J. T. Cacioppo, B. Patrick, Loneliness: Human Nature and
the Need for Social Connection (Norton, New York, 2008)
6. S. Lyubomirsky, K. M. Sheldon, D. Schkade, Rev. Gen.
Psychol. 9, 111 (2005).
7. G. M. Walton, G. L. Cohen, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92,
82 (2007).
8. L. F. Berkman, S. L. Syme, Am. J. Epidemiol. 109, 186
(1979).
9. S. Cohen, D. Janicki-Deverts, Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4,
375 (2009).
10. G. E. Miller, N. Rohleder, S. W. Cole, Psychosom. Med.
71, 57 (2009).
11. B. N. Uchino, J. Behav. Med. 29, 377 (2006).
12. N. I. Eisenberger, M. D. Lieberman, K. D. Williams,
Science 302, 290 (2003).
13. K. D. Williams, in Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed. (Academic Press, San Diego,
CA, 2009), pp. 279–314.
14. R. F. Baumeister, J. M. Twenge, C. K. Nuss, J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 83, 817 (2002).
15. E. Goffman, Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled
Identity (Simon and Schuster, New York, 1963), p. 8.
16. R. Mendoza-Denton, G. Downey, V. J. Purdie, A. Davis,
J. Pietrzak, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 896 (2002).
17. J. Blascovich, S. J. Spencer, D. Quinn, C. Steele,
Psychol. Sci. 12, 225 (2001).
18. S. W. Cole, M. E. Kemeny, S. E. Taylor, J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 72, 320 (1997).
19. S. Cohen et al., Health Psychol. 27, 268 (2008).
20. G. L. Cohen, J. Garcia, V. Purdie-Vaughns, N. Apfel,
P. Brzustoski, Science 324, 400 (2009).
21. T. D. Wilson, Science 313, 1251 (2006).
22. A previous paper reports relatively short-term results of
this intervention from the first cohort of students (7).
23. Information on materials and methods is available as
supporting material on Science Online.
24. T. D. Wilson, M. Damiani, N. Shelton, in Improving
Academic Achievement: Impact of Psychological Factors
on Education, J. Aronson, Ed. (Academic Press, Oxford,
2002), pp. 91–110.
25. J. Aronson, C. B. Fried, C. Good, J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38,
113 (2002).
26. L. Ross, R. Nisbett, The Person and the Situation
(McGraw-Hill, New York, 1991).
27. J. Crocker, C. T. Wolfe, Psychol. Rev. 108, 593
(2001).
28. E. L. Idler, Y. Benyamini, J. Health Soc. Behav. 38,
21 (1997).
29. T. D. Wilson, Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the
Adaptive Unconscious (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge,
MA, 2002).
30. D. K. Sherman et al., J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 745
(2009).
31. L. S. Blackwell, K. H. Trzesniewski, C. S. Dweck, Child
Dev.
78, 246 (2007).
32. J. J. Gross, R. A. Thompson, in Handbook of Emotion
Regulation, J. J. Gross, Ed. (Guilford, New York, 2007),
pp. 3–24.
33. J. P. Jamieson, W. B. Mendes, E. Blackstock, T. Schmader,
J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 208 (2010).
34. G. L. Cohen, J. Garcia, N. Apfel, A. Master, Science 313,
1307 (2006).
35. We thank participating students and helpful
administrators at an anonymous university for their
involvement. We also thank P. Carr, E. Chen, C. Dweck,
C. Logel, G. Miller, D. Paunesku, D. Sherman,
S. Spencer, C. Steele, and D. Yeager for input and
L. Cai, K. Crockett, J. Darwall, L. Egan, A. Master,
M. Silverio, and R. Titcombe for assistance. This
research was supported by grants from the American
Psychological Association, the Society for the
Psychological Study of Social Issues, and the Spencer
Foundation awarded to G.M.W. and by a grant from
the Russell Sage Foundation awarded to G.L.C.
Supporting Online Material
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/331/6023/1447/DC1
Materials and Methods
Tables S1 to S3
References
28 September 2010; accepted 14 February 2011
10.1126/science.1198364
Direct Interaction of RNA
Polymerase II and Mediator
Required for Transcription in Vivo
Julie Soutourina,1* Sandra Wydau,1† Yves Ambroise,2 Claire
Boschiero,1 Michel Werner1*
Gene transcription is highly regulated. Altered transcription can
lead to cancer or developmental
diseases. Mediator, a multisubunit complex conserved among
eukaryotes, is generally required
for RNA polymerase II (Pol II) transcription. An interaction
between the two complexes is known, but its
molecular nature and physiological role are unclear. We identify
a direct physical interaction between
the Rpb3 Pol II subunit of Saccharomyces cerevisiae and the
essential Mediator subunit, Med17.
Furthermore, we demonstrate a functional element in the
Mediator–Pol II interface that is important
for genome-wide Pol II recruitment in vivo. Our findings
suggest that a direct interaction between
Mediator and Pol II is generally required for transcription of
class II genes in eukaryotes.
M
ediator is a large multisubunit complex
conserved in all eukaryotes (1). It acts
as a link between specific protein regu-
lators and the RNA polymerase II (Pol II) tran-
scription machinery (2). Mediator is required at
most Pol II–transcribed gene promoters for reg-
ulated gene expression (3–5). In Saccharomyces
cerevisiae, Mediator is composed of 25 subunits
and is organized in four structural modules: the
tail, middle, head, and Cdk8 modules (6). A di-
rect Mediator–Pol II interaction is indicated
by previous copurification, coimmunoprecipita-
tion (CoIP) experiments (7–9) and by in vivo form-
aldehyde cross-linking (10). A model of the
Mediator–Pol II complex determined by electron
microscopy (EM) at 35 Å resolution suggests
that several Pol II subunits (Rpb1, 2, 3, 6, and 11)
might contact the middle or the head of Mediator
(11). It was recently suggested that Rpb4 and
Rpb7 could also be implicated in interactions
with Mediator (12–14). However, the requirement
of a direct interaction between Mediator and Pol II
for transcription activation has not been demon-
strated. Moreover, the identity of the Mediator
subunits contacting Pol II is unknown because of
the low resolution of the Mediator structure. As a
consequence, the mechanism by which Mediator
recruits Pol II is poorly understood.
To identify the subunit(s) of Mediator that
directly contact Pol II and determine the role of
these interactions in transcription regulation, we
usedaninvivophoto–cross-linkingapproachbased
on the incorporation by the cell-translation sys-
tem of photo-activable analogs of methionine and
leucine in proteins [see supporting online material
(SOM) text and figs. S1 and S2] (15, 16).
Because EM results (11) suggested potential
interactions of 16 Mediator subunits belonging to
the head (Med6, 8, 11, 17, 18, 19, 20, 22) and
middle (Med1, 4, 5, 7, 9, 10, 21, 31) modules with
Rpb1, 2, 3, 6, or 11 Pol II subunits, we immu-
noprecipitated hemagglutinin (HA)–tagged pro-
teins after in vivo cross-linking. Among the 80
pairwise contacts that we tested, only Myc-tagged
Rpb3 and HA-tagged Med17 cross-linked (Fig. 1).
These results demonstrate that the Rpb3 Pol II
1Commissariat à l’Energie Atomique et aux Energies Al-
ternatives (CEA), iBiTec-S, Service de Biologie Intégrative et
Génétique Moléculaire, F-91191 Gif-sur-Yvette cedex, France.
2CEA, iBiTec-S, Service de Chimie Bio-organique et Marquage,
F-91191 Gif-sur-Yvette cedex, France.
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail:
[email protected] (J.S.); [email protected] (M.W.)
†Present address: Université Paris Descartes, EA 4065, Faculté
des Sciences Pharmaceutiques et Biologiques, 75006 Paris,
France.
www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 18 MARCH 2011
1451
REPORTS
o
n
A
p
ri
l 1
4
,
2
0
1
1
w
w
w
.s
ci
e
n
ce
m
a
g
.o
rg
D
o
w
n
lo
a
d
e
d
f
ro
m
http://www.sciencemag.org/
Questions:
1. The article attempts to better understand the origins of
inequality by looking at one specific issue faced by minorities
in work and school. What is that issue, and why does it make
sense to study it?
2. The authors suggest that subjective construal of hardships is
more important than the objective number or attributes of the
hardship. What does this mean, and why does this matter for
this study?
3. The authors argue that there may be not only short-term
effects to their intervention, but also long-term ones. Why do
they think this?
4. This study included two participant variables (each with two
conditions) for a total of four possible conditions. What were
two variables, and what were the two conditions for each
variable?
5. What were the short-term measures? What were the long-term
measures?
6. What was the purpose of the fictional report that the
participants read? How was the message of this report
internalized?
7. Describe the trend in GPA throughout the remaining three
years of college for participants in all four conditions. Who
benefitted most from this intervention?
8. The researchers were not allowed to report any “raw”
information on GPA, but their ability to communicate the
effectiveness of the study relied on reporting GPA somehow.
How did they get around this?
9. The authors argue that, even though the intervention focused
on belongingness, it worked by changing how individuals view
adversity and hardship. Explain their logic here.
10. Did the intervention have any effect on health? If so, what
was it, and among whom was this effect found?
11. How well did participants generally remember the
intervention? Why is this important to the effectiveness of the
intervention?
12. The researchers describe several limitations to their
intervention. What are two conditions they mention under which
they do not expect to get the same effects as they did in this
study?
019 10:05 AM
Module 3
This week, our focus will be on grammar. If you have any
questions regarding the assignments or anything else, please let
me know. I am attaching an APA outline of the paper to help
you get started.
This week’s paper has 4 parts. The first part, you are rewriting
the 2 passages for content and grammar. You can rearrange, and
delete words in the passage to make it work. I understand
everyone has learned English differently and might have learned
different rules.
Next, is the guided writing assignment. Make sure you include
the rough draft and corrected version and label both.
The third part is the list of words. First, describe how the word
should be used, then state if it should be used in APA writing.
Lastly, use only the words that you would use in APA writing in
a sentence.
The last part of this assignment is explaining the use of
apostrophes in APA writing.
For this assignment, you will have 4 level 1 headings (bold and
centered) for each section of the paper. You will have level 2
headings for the first 2 assignments to separate them. Remember
level 2 headings are bold and at the left margin

More Related Content

PPT
Writing A Research Paper
PPT
Writing a research_paper
PDF
Expository Essay Format Outline
PPTX
2014 271150 communication disorders writing workshop 1
PPTX
2013 communication disorders assignment 1 a
DOCX
Essential Biology E5 (HL) The Human Brain
PDF
Vanders Human Physiology The Mechanisms of Body Function 14th Edition Widmaie...
PDF
Vanders Human Physiology The Mechanisms of Body Function 14th Edition Widmaie...
Writing A Research Paper
Writing a research_paper
Expository Essay Format Outline
2014 271150 communication disorders writing workshop 1
2013 communication disorders assignment 1 a
Essential Biology E5 (HL) The Human Brain
Vanders Human Physiology The Mechanisms of Body Function 14th Edition Widmaie...
Vanders Human Physiology The Mechanisms of Body Function 14th Edition Widmaie...

Similar to submit your work following the file naming convention FirstInitial.docx (13)

PPT
Scientific writing and international publication
PDF
Medical Terminology A Programmed Approach 2nd Edition Bostwick Solutions Manual
DOCX
Allow enough time. Before you can write about the research, yo.docx
PDF
Scientific Report Writing Tips
PDF
Medical Terminology A Programmed Approach 2nd Edition Bostwick Solutions Manual
DOCX
Process analysis paragraph
PPTX
Emphasis: Sentence Stucture
PDF
Essay Nursing Career
DOCX
Writing a ParagraphWriting a ParagraphA paragraph should be ar
PPTX
SPEECH, PARAGRAPH easy English prepararme.pptx
PDF
English grammar in context
DOCX
Learning Objectives• Be able to conceptualize the information.docx
PDF
Test Bank for Neuroscience Fundamentals for Rehabilitation 4th Edition by Lun...
Scientific writing and international publication
Medical Terminology A Programmed Approach 2nd Edition Bostwick Solutions Manual
Allow enough time. Before you can write about the research, yo.docx
Scientific Report Writing Tips
Medical Terminology A Programmed Approach 2nd Edition Bostwick Solutions Manual
Process analysis paragraph
Emphasis: Sentence Stucture
Essay Nursing Career
Writing a ParagraphWriting a ParagraphA paragraph should be ar
SPEECH, PARAGRAPH easy English prepararme.pptx
English grammar in context
Learning Objectives• Be able to conceptualize the information.docx
Test Bank for Neuroscience Fundamentals for Rehabilitation 4th Edition by Lun...

More from james891 (20)

DOCX
Submit Milestone Two, which is the second of two milestones for .docx
DOCX
Submit to Assignment Folder.Purpose The purpose of this.docx
DOCX
SUBMIT ASSIGNMENT 2.1Assignment 2.1 Liberty Challenged in Ninet.docx
DOCX
Submit Milestone Three. This submission includes the methods, educat.docx
DOCX
Submit an outline of your Signature Assignment research paper.  .docx
DOCX
Submit the Marketing Mix section of the Final Marketing Plan (Sectio.docx
DOCX
Submit Memo to the President.Content and General Guidelines.docx
DOCX
Submit an individual essay, addressing the question in a critical .docx
DOCX
Submit Final Project.Your course objective states consideration.docx
DOCX
Submit an outline for your research paper. The outline should make c.docx
DOCX
Submit the first two, double-spaced pages of your advocacy paper rou.docx
DOCX
Submit final project.1) Submit a brief abstract describing.docx
DOCX
Submit Section II of the final project case study, in which you will.docx
DOCX
Submit paper with two pages of information, in APA format, including.docx
DOCX
Submit paper with two pages of information, in APA format, inclu.docx
DOCX
Submit one well-developed, thorough paragraph synopsis of Chapter 4 .docx
DOCX
Substance-Related and Addictive Disordershttpsdoi-org.ezp.wal.docx
DOCX
SubstanceIndividual issues, dynamics, and behaviors associated .docx
DOCX
Submitting Files - Lab UtilitiesObjective of the activity.docx
DOCX
Substantive response, Please further discussion.APA formatAt.docx
Submit Milestone Two, which is the second of two milestones for .docx
Submit to Assignment Folder.Purpose The purpose of this.docx
SUBMIT ASSIGNMENT 2.1Assignment 2.1 Liberty Challenged in Ninet.docx
Submit Milestone Three. This submission includes the methods, educat.docx
Submit an outline of your Signature Assignment research paper.  .docx
Submit the Marketing Mix section of the Final Marketing Plan (Sectio.docx
Submit Memo to the President.Content and General Guidelines.docx
Submit an individual essay, addressing the question in a critical .docx
Submit Final Project.Your course objective states consideration.docx
Submit an outline for your research paper. The outline should make c.docx
Submit the first two, double-spaced pages of your advocacy paper rou.docx
Submit final project.1) Submit a brief abstract describing.docx
Submit Section II of the final project case study, in which you will.docx
Submit paper with two pages of information, in APA format, including.docx
Submit paper with two pages of information, in APA format, inclu.docx
Submit one well-developed, thorough paragraph synopsis of Chapter 4 .docx
Substance-Related and Addictive Disordershttpsdoi-org.ezp.wal.docx
SubstanceIndividual issues, dynamics, and behaviors associated .docx
Submitting Files - Lab UtilitiesObjective of the activity.docx
Substantive response, Please further discussion.APA formatAt.docx

Recently uploaded (20)

PDF
Business Ethics Teaching Materials for college
PDF
VCE English Exam - Section C Student Revision Booklet
PDF
O5-L3 Freight Transport Ops (International) V1.pdf
PDF
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ 4 KỸ NĂNG TIẾNG ANH 9 GLOBAL SUCCESS - CẢ NĂM - BÁM SÁT FORM Đ...
PDF
RMMM.pdf make it easy to upload and study
PDF
Saundersa Comprehensive Review for the NCLEX-RN Examination.pdf
PPTX
Week 4 Term 3 Study Techniques revisited.pptx
PDF
TR - Agricultural Crops Production NC III.pdf
PPTX
Pharmacology of Heart Failure /Pharmacotherapy of CHF
PPTX
Introduction to Child Health Nursing – Unit I | Child Health Nursing I | B.Sc...
PPTX
PPT- ENG7_QUARTER1_LESSON1_WEEK1. IMAGERY -DESCRIPTIONS pptx.pptx
PPTX
The Healthy Child – Unit II | Child Health Nursing I | B.Sc Nursing 5th Semester
PDF
Abdominal Access Techniques with Prof. Dr. R K Mishra
PDF
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
PDF
Origin of periodic table-Mendeleev’s Periodic-Modern Periodic table
PDF
Anesthesia in Laparoscopic Surgery in India
PDF
O7-L3 Supply Chain Operations - ICLT Program
PPTX
human mycosis Human fungal infections are called human mycosis..pptx
PPTX
Cell Types and Its function , kingdom of life
PPTX
Introduction_to_Human_Anatomy_and_Physiology_for_B.Pharm.pptx
Business Ethics Teaching Materials for college
VCE English Exam - Section C Student Revision Booklet
O5-L3 Freight Transport Ops (International) V1.pdf
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ 4 KỸ NĂNG TIẾNG ANH 9 GLOBAL SUCCESS - CẢ NĂM - BÁM SÁT FORM Đ...
RMMM.pdf make it easy to upload and study
Saundersa Comprehensive Review for the NCLEX-RN Examination.pdf
Week 4 Term 3 Study Techniques revisited.pptx
TR - Agricultural Crops Production NC III.pdf
Pharmacology of Heart Failure /Pharmacotherapy of CHF
Introduction to Child Health Nursing – Unit I | Child Health Nursing I | B.Sc...
PPT- ENG7_QUARTER1_LESSON1_WEEK1. IMAGERY -DESCRIPTIONS pptx.pptx
The Healthy Child – Unit II | Child Health Nursing I | B.Sc Nursing 5th Semester
Abdominal Access Techniques with Prof. Dr. R K Mishra
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
Origin of periodic table-Mendeleev’s Periodic-Modern Periodic table
Anesthesia in Laparoscopic Surgery in India
O7-L3 Supply Chain Operations - ICLT Program
human mycosis Human fungal infections are called human mycosis..pptx
Cell Types and Its function , kingdom of life
Introduction_to_Human_Anatomy_and_Physiology_for_B.Pharm.pptx

submit your work following the file naming convention FirstInitial.docx

  • 1. submit your work following the file naming convention FirstInitial.LastName_M01.docx. For example, J.Smith_M01.docx. Remember that it is not necessary to manually type in the file extension; it will automatically append. Start by reading and following these instructions: 1. Quickly skim the questions or assignment below and the assignment rubric to help you focus. 2. Read the required chapter(s) of the textbook and any additional recommended resources. Some answers may require you to do additional research on the Internet or in other reference sources. Choose your sources carefully. 3. Consider the discussions and the any insights gained from it. 4. Create your Assignment submission and be sure to cite your sources, use APA style as required, check your spelling. Assignment: Professional Development Assignment Description: There are 4 parts to the assignment. Please place all four assignments into one paper. The different parts should be clearly marked so your professor knows which question you are addressing. 1. Rewrite the following essays. Correct all errors in capitalization, spelling, and punctuation. Divide the passage into appropriate paragraphs. · Essay #1: When my husband Joe had cancer surgery five years ago, each of his family members responded just as I knew they would. John, his father, decided to organize the family’s calls. Because, of course, everything would run so much more smoothly. Thus Jane, Matt, and Jim received detailed sheets of instructions in the mail. Telling them which days to telephone r.j. smith hospital to talk to Joe and what presents to send. Jane, enraged, promptly threw a tantrum. Calling Matt and me to complain about her father’s overbearing behavior. “I,” she yelled, “am a
  • 2. Psychiatrist who knows how to handle these situations, i am not still a child.” Matt also responded predictably. By avoiding the situation. He threw himself into his work. Normally a late sleeper, Matt took to leaving at 5:00 a.m., driving on the deserted expressway and arriving at work before six a.m. In addition, he didn’t return until 11:00 p.m. When he would fall into bed so exhausted that he couldn’t worry about Joe. Jim, too, responded predictably. He fumed inside for weeks, ignored John’s instructions, and sent cartons of books to Joe. So that he would never be bored. The books were funny. Because Jim had read Norman Cousins’ book about the healing power of laughter. Within a few months, Joe recovered from the surgery-inspite of his family · Essay #2: Treatment of atrial septal defect depends upon the size and symptoms and therefore is individualized an atrial septal defect of less than 3mm usually closes spontaneously (The Merck Manual, 2006). When the defect is between 3mm and 8mm it closes spontaneously in eighty percent of cases by the age of eighteen months, however, atrial septal defects located in the anteroinferior aspect of the septum (ostium primum) or in the posterior aspect of the septum near the superior vena cava or inferior vena cava (sinus venosus) don’t close spontaneously. If the defect is very small does not close spontaneously and the patient is asymptomatic. The treatment may be simply too monitor via an annual echocardiogram. Of course their is a risk of patients’ becoming symptomatic. Moderate-sized atrial septal defects or larger or patients’ who are symptomatic require closure of the shunt this is usually done between the ages of 2 and 6 years. A catheter-delivered closure device, such as Amplatzer Septal Occluder or Cardio- Seal device. May be used for closure of atrial septal defects less than 13mm in size, except than primum or sinus venosus defects. If the defect is greater than thirteen milimeters or located near important structures. Surgical repair becomes necessary. If the atrial septal defect is
  • 3. repaired during childhood there mortality rates approach 0 and the patient’s life expectancy approaches that of the general population prior to surgical repair, patients may need to be treated with diuretics; digoxin; ACE inhibitor; or beta blockers to prevent congestive heart failure (Moser & Riegel, 2007). Following surgical repair patience will receive aspirin to prevent clots, and be monitored closely for dysrhythmias and pulmonary hypertension. Oxygen and nitric oxide therapy have proven to be beneficial in treating postoperative pulmonary hypertension. Also patients who have primum atrial septal defect will need endocarditis prophylaxis. 2. Guided writing exercise: · Think about a recent experience you have had that required you to use critical thinking skills. Set a timer for five minutes. Write about your experience. Do not worry about grammar, punctuation, or spelling. Just write, but stop at five minutes. Now go back and write your experience with correct grammar, punctuation, and spelling. Submit both written pieces. Add a concluding paragraph that answers the follows questions: How was the formal writing experience different from the timed writing experience? What did you do differently? Was there any difference in the time it took you? How did you ensure your spelling, grammar, and punctuation were correct? 3. Describe when the following words would be used in a scholarly essay (define each word). Then, use those that are appropriate to a scholarly essay in a sentence. · Their, there, they’re · Affect, effect · Its, it’s · your, you’re · then, than · accept, except 4. Describe the use of apostrophes in writing in general. When should apostrophes be used in formal writing? When should apostrophes be avoided in formal writing?
  • 4. DOI: 10.1126/science.1198364 , 1447 (2011);331 Science , et al.Gregory M. Walton Outcomes of Minority Students A Brief Social-Belonging Intervention Improves Academic and Health This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only. clicking here.colleagues, clients, or customers by , you can order high-quality copies for yourIf you wish to distribute this article to others here.following the guidelines can be obtained byPermission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles ): April 14, 2011 www.sciencemag.org (this infomation is current as of The following resources related to this article are available online at http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html version of this article at:
  • 5. including high-resolution figures, can be found in the onlineUpdated information and services, http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2011/03/15/331.6023. 1447.DC1.html can be found at: Supporting Online Material http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html#ref -list-1 , 8 of which can be accessed free:cites 23 articlesThis article http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/psychology Psychology subject collections:This article appears in the following registered trademark of AAAS. is aScience2011 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title CopyrightAmerican Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by theScience o n A p ri l 1 4 ,
  • 7. ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html http://www.sciencemag.org/content/331/6023/1447.full.html#ref -list-1 http://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/psychology http://www.sciencemag.org/ units (fig. S9) [paired t test, P < 0.02, n = 19 pairs; median distances from the granular layer were 0.45 mm (source units) and 1.05 mm (target units)]. The present study demonstrated that canoni- cal feed-forward signal flow across cortical layers during sensory coding reverse to the feed-back direction during memory retrieval phase, which sug- gests flexible recruitment of interlaminar connectivity depending on the cognitive demands in the monkey association cortices (Fig. 4C). We used CSD anal- ysis to estimate cortical layers (Fig. 1, C to E, and fig. S1), and the observed stimulus-evoked CSD profiles were quite similar to those in the primary sensory cortices (17, 27). For some penetrations, we observed that the current sink positioned super- ficially next to the earliest-sink contact exhibited larger peak amplitudes and much longer durations thanthatoftheearliestcurrentsink.Thisobservation might reflect the cytoarchitectural nature of A36 as a dysgranular cortex (28) as well as the direct inputs to the deepest part of the superficial layer, which is
  • 8. consistent with anatomical observations (29). A recent study in the rat primary auditory cor- tex demonstrated that the direction of interlam- inar signal flow depends on the cortical “state”: Sensory-evoked responses were initiated in the thalamorecipient layers and then propagated to the superficial and deep layers, whereas in spontane- ously active “up-states,” neuronal activity was initiated in the deep layers and then propagated to the superficial layers (27). These state-dependent changes in the interlaminar signal flows in rats are consistent with our results obtained in mon- keys performing a memory task. Together, these findings highlight the flexibility of cortical lam- inar circuits. Further experiments will be needed to determine whether such flexible interlaminar connectivity is also implemented and used in other cortical areas for other cognitive demands. References and Notes 1. L. R. Squire, J. T. Wixted, R. E. Clark, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 872 (2007). 2. W. A. Suzuki, Neuron 61, 657 (2009). 3. E. A. Murray, T. J. Bussey, L. M. Saksida, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 99 (2007). 4. Y. Miyashita, Science 306, 435 (2004). 5. Y. Naya, M. Yoshida, Y. Miyashita, J. Neurosci. 23, 2861 (2003). 6. N. K. Logothetis, D. L. Sheinberg, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 577 (1996).
  • 9. 7. G. Buzsáki, Neuron 68, 362 (2010). 8. S. H. Wang, R. G. Morris, Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 49, C1 (2010). 9. M. P. Witter, E. I. Moser, Trends Neurosci. 29, 671 (2006). 10. A. Bollimunta, Y. Chen, C. E. Schroeder, M. Ding, J. Neurosci. 28, 9976 (2008). 11. A. K. Engel, P. Fries, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 156 (2010). 12. I. E. Ohiorhenuan et al., Nature 466, 617 (2010). 13. R. J. Douglas, K. A. Martin, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 419 (2004). 14. J. J. Nassi, E. M. Callaway, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 360 (2009). 15. R. C. Reid, J. M. Alonso, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 475 (1996). 16. J. Csicsvari, B. Jamieson, K. D. Wise, G. Buzsáki, Neuron 37, 311 (2003). 17. C. E. Schroeder, P. Lakatos, Trends Neurosci. 32, 9 (2009). 18. A. K. Engel, W. Singer, Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 16 (2001). 19. T. Hirabayashi, Y. Miyashita, J. Neurosci. 25, 10299 (2005). 20. M. Tomita, J. J. Eggermont, J. Neurophysiol. 93, 378 (2005). 21. C. A. Atencio, C. E. Schreiner, PLoS ONE 5, e9521 (2010). 22. T. Hirabayashi, D. Takeuchi, K. Tamura, Y. Miyashita, J. Neurosci. 30, 10407 (2010). 23. J. M. Alonso, L. M. Martinez, Nat. Neurosci. 1, 395 (1998). 24. Materials and methods are available as supporting
  • 10. material on Science Online. 25. S. Lefort, C. Tomm, J. C. Floyd Sarria, C. C. H. Petersen, Neuron 61, 301 (2009). 26. N. Weiler, L. Wood, J. I. Yu, S. A. Solla, G. M. G. Shepherd, Nat. Neurosci. 11, 360 (2008). 27. S. Sakata, K. D. Harris, Neuron 64, 404 (2009). 28. W. A. Suzuki, D. G. Amaral, J. Comp. Neurol. 463, 67 (2003). 29. K. S. Saleem, K. Tanaka, J. Neurosci. 16, 4757 (1996). 30. This work was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Specially Promoted Research from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) to Y.M. (19002010) and Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists from MEXT to T.H. (18700378); a grant from Takeda Science Foundation to Y.M.; and Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Research Fellowships for Young Scientists to D.T. (1811234) and K.T. (211438). This work was also supported in part by Global Center of Excellence Program from MEXT. We thank M. Takeda for discussions and comments and H. Kasahara, R. Fujimichi, T. Matsui, and K. W. Koyano for advice and help with experiments. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/331/6023/1443/DC1 Materials and Methods SOM Text Figs. S1 to S9 Tables S1 and S2 References 3 November 2010; accepted 8 February 2011 10.1126/science.1199967
  • 11. A Brief Social-Belonging Intervention Improves Academic and Health Outcomes of Minority Students Gregory M. Walton1* and Geoffrey L. Cohen1,2 A brief intervention aimed at buttressing college freshmen’s sense of social belonging in school was tested in a randomized controlled trial (N = 92), and its academic and health-related consequences over 3 years are reported. The intervention aimed to lessen psychological perceptions of threat on campus by framing social adversity as common and transient. It used subtle attitude-change strategies to lead participants to self-generate the intervention message. The intervention was expected to be particularly beneficial to African-American students (N = 49), a stereotyped and socially marginalized group in academics, and less so to European-American students (N = 43). Consistent with these expectations, over the 3-year observation period the intervention raised African Americans’ grade-point average (GPA) relative to multiple control groups and halved the minority achievement gap. This performance boost was mediated by the effect of the intervention on subjective construal: It prevented students from seeing adversity on campus as an indictment of their belonging. Additionally, the intervention improved African Americans’ self-reported health and well- being and reduced their reported number of doctor visits 3 years postintervention. Senior-year surveys indicated no awareness among participants of the intervention's impact. The results suggest that social belonging is a psychological lever where targeted intervention can have broad consequences that lessen inequalities in achievement and health.
  • 12. A n important question facing society con- cerns the origins of inequalities between socially marginalized and nonmarginal- ized groups. Among the most consequential of inequalities is the poorer school and health out- comes experienced by African Americans, Latino Americans, and other non-Asian ethnic minor- ities relative to European Americans. These dif- ferences occur at all levels of socioeconomic status (1–3). Although many structural factors contribute to these inequalities, the present research exam- ines a psychological factor: concern about social belonging. Social belonging—a sense of having positive relationships with others—is a fundamen- tal human need (4, 5). Social isolation, loneliness, and low social status harm not only subjective well-being (6) but also intellectual achievement (7) and immune function and health (8–11). Even a single instance of exclusion can undermine well- being (12, 13), intelligence quotient (IQ) test per- formance, and self-control (14). Members of socially stigmatized groups, such as African Americans, may be relatively more uncertain about their social belonging in main- stream institutions like school and work (7). Be- cause their ethnic group is often negatively stereotyped and marginalized, they may be un- sure of whether they will be fully included in
  • 13. positive social relationships in these settings (2). As the sociologist Erving Goffman wrote, “The central feature of the stigmatized individual’s sit- uation in life…is a question of…‘acceptance’” (15). Uncertainty about belonging, especially when chronic, can undermine minorities’ performance (7, 16) and health (3, 17, 18). Social belonging may thus constitute a psychological lever where targeted intervention could yield broad benefits. 1Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA. 2School of Education and Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA. *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected] www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 18 MARCH 2011 1447 REPORTS o n A p ri l 1 4 , 2 0 1 1
  • 15. http://www.sciencemag.org/ Such an intervention is reported here. Critical to its rationale is the insight that it is people’s subjective interpretations of the quality of their relationships, more so than the objective number or attributes of those relationships, that strongly affects well-being (5, 19). The present interven- tion, delivered to students during the challenging transition to college, was designed to encourage nonthreatening interpretations of adversity. During the transition to a new school, students can face frequent social setbacks and feelings of isolation. Their well-being and performance may depend, to a great extent, on whether they construe such experiences as evidence that they do not belong. Because African-American students experi- ence relatively greater uncertainty about their belonging in school, they were expected to ben- efit from the intervention more than nonminority students (7). Further if, as we intended, the in- tervention triggered an enduring perceptual change in the encoding of social experience, its effects might persist over time. Short-term effects might compound into long-term effects through a re- cursive virtuous cycle, in which early perform- ance gains assure students of their belonging in school, which in turn improves their performance, in a repeating feedback loop (20). Students who feel more assured of their belonging may also initiate more social interactions and form better relationships on campus, facilitating their social integration and further benefiting their well- being, performance, and health (21).
  • 16. The intervention was delivered to two cohorts of African-American (N = 49) and European- American (N = 43) students in the second semes- ter of their first year at a selective college (22, 23). To assess psychological responses to adversity, we asked participants to complete daily surveys in the first week after the intervention. To assess their long-term sense of belonging, health, and well-being, we asked them to complete an end- of-college survey 3 years later (completion rate 78.26%) (23). At the end of this survey, partici- pants were asked to authorize the release of their complete college academic transcript (authoriza- tion rate 97.22%) (23). Participating students were randomly as- signed to either the belonging-treatment condi- tion or a control condition. In cohort 1, participants were recruited through convenience sampling; in cohort 2, through random sampling (23). An additional campus-wide control group was ob- tained by collecting the anonymized official grade-point averages (GPAs) of all European Americans (N = 1362) and African Americans (N = 194) in the same class years as participants but who did not participate in the study (23). This group was included in secondary analyses of GPA. The intervention provided students with a narrative that framed social adversity in school as shared and short-lived (24). This message en- couraged students to attribute adversity not to fixed deficits unique to themselves or their ethnic group but to common and transient aspects of the
  • 17. college-adjustment process. Upon arrival in a research lab, participants read a report of the ostensible results of a survey of more senior students at their school. Most students, the report indicated, had worried about whether they be- longed in college during the difficult first year but grew confident in their belonging with time. The survey results were said to be consistent across ethnic and gender groups. For instance, one sur- vey respondent was quoted as saying, “Freshman year even though I met large numbers of people, I didn’t have a small group of close friends…I was pretty homesick, and I had to remind myself that making close friends takes time. Since then…I have met people some of whom are now just as close as my friends in high school were” (23). Concerns about belonging were thus represented as common at first, as temporary, and as due to the challenging nature of the college transition. To encourage participants to internalize the message, several steps exploited the “saying- is-believing effect”—the tendency to endorse messages that one has freely advocated (25). Par- ticipants were asked to write an essay describing how their own experiences in college echoed the experiences summarized in the survey. They then turned their essay into a speech, which they delivered to a video camera. These materials, participants were told, would be shown to future students to help ease their transition to college. Beyond facilitating internalization, this procedure averted the potential stigma of receiving an intervention, because it encouraged participants to see themselves as benefactors and not as
  • 18. beneficiaries (24, 26). In the control condition, the procedure was the same but the survey ad- dressed topics unrelated to belonging (e.g., change in social-political attitudes) (23). The intervention lasted about 1 hour. Few analyses were moderated by cohort (i.e., no more than would be expected by chance alone and none involving the primary outcomes of GPA, health, or well-being). Thus, data from the two cohorts were combined to increase statistical power. First, analyses examined the trajectory of students’ official GPA over time. In contrast to all other groups, African Americans in the control group showed no improvement in GPA from the fall of their freshman year, the semester before the intervention, through their sophomore, junior, and senior years [linear trend F < 1]. By contrast, the GPAs of intervention-treated African Amer- icans rose over time [for linear trend, F(1,34) = 13.79, P= 0.0007; for time × condition, F(1,34) = 4.16, P= 0.049]. The GPAs of European-American students also rose over time [F(1,29) = 6.88, P = 0.014] with no difference by condition [F < 1]. As Fig. 1 shows, the intervention set African Americans on an upward trajectory such that the gap between them and their European Americans classmates closed over time. By students’ senior year, the gap was cut by 79% (23). Multiple regression, with student gender controlled, tested the effect of student race and condition (randomized control versus social- belonging treatment) on change in GPA—mean postintervention GPA (sophomore through senior
  • 19. years) minus mean preintervention GPA (fall term, first year) (23). There was no condition effect on preintervention GPA for either racial group [t values < 1] (table S1) (23). However, a significant condition effect on change in GPA emerged for African Americans [B = 0.30, t(65) = 2.54, P = 0.014] with no effect for European Americans[t<1][race×conditionB=–0.43, t(65)= –2.41, P = 0.019]. Virtually identical results were obtained when preintervention GPA was used as a covariate in an analysis of postintervention GPA [treatment effect for African Americans, B = 0.24, t(64) = 2.65, P = 0.010; treatment effect for European Americans, t < 1; race × condition: B = –0.31, t(64) = –2.27, P = 0.027]. The intervention closed the minority gap in 3-year GPA (SD = 0.36) from 0.29 points in the control condition to 0.14 points in the treatment condition, a 52% reduction. Adding the campus-wide sample further sup- ports treatment efficacy. An agreement with uni- Fig. 1. Raw GPA by student race, experimen- tal condition, and aca- demic term. Means are noncumulative and were combined across cohorts. Ranges in sample sizes and standard errors for European Americans are N = 25 to 33 and SE = 0.08 to 0.14; for African Americans, N = 30 to 37 and SE = 0.09 to 0.12.
  • 20. 3.00 3.10 3.20 3.30 3.40 3.50 3.60 3.70 3.80 3.90 4.00 Fall Term Fall Term Spring Term Fall Term Spring Term Fall Term Spring Term First Year Sophomore Year Junior Year Senior Year Treatment European Americans, Social-Belonging Treatment African Americans, Social-Belonging Treatment African Americans, Randomized Control Group European Americans, Randomized Control Group
  • 21. Social-Belonging 18 MARCH 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1448 REPORTS o n A p ri l 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a
  • 22. g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ versity officials precludes the reporting of raw or adjusted means in this sample. To honor this agreement but present the results graphically, we performed analyses on residual postintervention GPA with preintervention GPA and gender con- trolled. Multiple regression on change in GPA and on raw postintervention GPA with preinter- vention GPA included as a covariate yield vir- tually identical results (23). As shown in Fig. 2A, treated African Americans had higher residual GPA scores than did African Americans campus-
  • 23. wide [B = 0.28, t(1620) = 3.97, P = 0.00008] and African Americans in therandomized controlgroup [B = 0.24, t(1620) = 2.62, P = 0.009]. The latter two groups did not differ [t < 1]. (Fig. 2A) (23). Illustrating its broad impact, the interven- tion tripled the percentage of African Americans earning postintervention GPAs in the top 25% of their class, as measured by both residual and raw postintervention GPA, and tended to reduce the percentage of African Americans performing in the bottom 25% of their class on both indices (Fig. 2, B and C) (23). What accounts for these treatment effects? Daily surveys, collected the week after the in- tervention, suggest that the intervention buffered African Americans against adversity (23). Among untreated African Americans, feelings of be- longing in school rose and fell with the degree of adversity students reported having experienced earlier that day and the day before. As adversity rose, belonging fell (mean within-subjects R = –0.45, derived from the average of individual participants’ within-subjects correlations, after each was subjected to a Fisher r-to-z trans- formation) (23). For treated African Americans, this relationship was reduced to nil [mean within- subjects R = 0.01], a significant reduction [t(59) = 2.99, P = 0.004]. In summary, the intervention robbed adversity of its symbolic meaning for African Americans, untethering their sense of belonging from daily hardship (27). Like treated African Americans, European Americans showed little relationship between adversity and belonging
  • 24. [for both conditions, mean within-subjects R = –0.09; condition difference, t < 1] [race × con- dition: t(59) = –2.04, P = 0.046]. These results provide a window into the shift in African-American students’ psychology caused by the intervention. This shift benefited their long-term performance. African Americans whose belonging was more robust to daily adversity— whose sense of belonging was relatively inde- pendent of their day-to-day adversity—showed greater improvement in their 3-year postinter- vention GPA [R = 0.51, P = 0.001] (23). The effect of the intervention in protecting African- Americans students’sense of belonging from daily adversity mediated its effect on their GPA (23). The intervention thus planted a change in social perception that, it appears, accompanied students long after the intervention ended to affect their performance in college. Three years after the intervention, at the end of their college tenure, participants completed a survey to assess long-term effects on psychology, well-being, and health. Also, to assess whether the intervention operated beneath conscious aware- ness, we asked participants whether they remem- bered the intervention from 3 years previously, whether they thought it had affected them, and whether they agreed with its message. On no outcome did European Americans differ by con- dition [t values < 1.35, P values > 0.18]. African Americans, however, showed consistent treat- ment effects. The race × condition interaction was not always significant, indicating that the
  • 25. treatment effect was not always larger for African Americans than for European Americans. Degrees of freedom vary because some measures were completed only by participants in cohort 2 (23). If the intervention lessened how much Afri- can Americans’ belonging fluctuated with adver- sity, and if it did so by lessening how much they viewed campus life through the lens of race, then intervention-treated African Americans should (i) report greater stability and less uncertainty about their belonging in school {less agreement with items like, “When something bad happens, I feel that maybe I don’t belong at [school name]”} (7) and (ii) exhibit less cognitive accessibility of negative racial stereotypes and self-doubt (23). They did [self-reported belonging uncertainty, t(36) = –2.01, P = 0.052; accessibility of negative racial stereotypes, t(66) = –2.01, P = 0.049; accessibility of self-doubt, t(64) = –2.64, P = 0.010] (Fig. 3) (23). Given the importance of social belonging for reducing stress and improving immune function and physical health (5, 8–11, 19) and the rel- atively poorer health experienced by African Americans, even those high in socioeconomic status (3), we examined effects on health. We assessed self-reported health, an important pre- dictor of morbidity and mortality (28), using the five-item general health component of the Med- ical Outcomes Study Short-Form Health Survey (23). We also asked participants how many times they had visited the doctor in the previous 3 months (cohort 1) or 1 month (cohort 2). African Americans reported being healthier and visit-
  • 26. ing the doctor less frequently in the treatment condition than in the control condition [t(32) = 2.48, P = 0.019 and t(63) = –2.23, P = 0.030, Fig. 2. Cumulative academic performance from sophomore through senior year. Data were combined across cohorts. (A) Resid- ual sophomore-through-senior- year GPAs adjusted for student gender and preintervention (fall term, first year) GPA. Error bars represent T1 SE. Means repre- sent the degree to which stu- dents performed better (positive values) or worse (negative val- ues) than expected after the in- tervention in GPA units based on their gender and preintervention performance. (B) Percentage of students in the top and bottom 25% of their college class in residual postintervention GPA (i.e., postintervention GPA ad- justed for student gender and preintervention GPA). (C) Percent- age of students in the top and bottom 25% of their college class in raw postintervention GPA. For analytic details, see (23). Sample sizes for European Americans areNcampus-wide control group = 1362and Nexperimental groups = 33; for African Americans, Ncampus-wide control group = 194 and Nexperimental groups = 37. -0.25
  • 28. 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% In Top of Class In Bottom of Class In Top of Class In Bottom of Class European American Students African American Students B In Residual Postintervention GPA 0% 10% 20%
  • 29. 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% In Top of Class In Bottom of Class In Top of Class In Bottom of Class European American Students African American Students C In Raw Postintervention GPA Campus-Wide Control Group Randomized Control Group Social-Belonging Treatment Percentage of Students in Top and Bottom 25%
  • 30. of College Class www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 18 MARCH 2011 1449 REPORTS o n A p ri l 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a
  • 31. g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ respectively] (Fig. 4, A and B). Whereas 60% of untreated African Americans had seen a doctor recently, only 28% of treated African Americans had [c2(1, N = 38) = 3.98, P = 0.046]. The race gap in self-reported health was eliminated in the treatment condition; interestingly, there was no gap for doctor visits (23). Future research should examine whether these patterns generalize to physiological and physical indicators of health (9) to assess the robustness of the effect beyond self-report outcomes and to identify biological
  • 32. pathways (11). As further evidence of improved well-being, African Americans also scored higher on the Subjective Happiness Scale (23) [t(35) = 2.61, P = 0.013] (Fig. 4C). The happiness gap with Euro- pean Americans higher than African Americans disappeared in the treatment condition (23). The finding of a lasting positive impact on subjective happiness is noteworthy in light of research showing that individual happiness is relatively stable (6). Participants were unaware of the interven- tion’s effect, suggesting that its efficacy did not depend on conscious awareness. Most students reported that they remembered participating in the study 3 years earlier (79% did). But when asked to describe “the most memorable and important” information they had learned in the study, few recalled the key content of the survey they had read (8% did), and few reported that the study had had “any” effect on their college ex- perience (14% did) (table S3). There was no condition difference on any of these outcomes for African Americans [c2(1, N = 37 to 38) values < 1.40, P values > 0.20]; treated African Americans ascribed no more effect to the study than un- treated African Americans. However, indirect mea- sures of recall and beliefs did show treatment effects. When asked to “guess” the process of change described in the survey they had read, more treated than untreated African Americans wrote that it concerned how students’ social ex- periences in college improve over time (50% versus 20%) [c2(1, N = 38) = 3.79, P = 0.052].
  • 33. Additionally, treated students endorsed this mes- sage. When asked to describe their own experi- ences,moretreatedthanuntreatedAfricanAmericans volunteered that their own social experiences in college had improved over time (50% versus 20%) [c2(1, N = 38) = 3.79, P= 0.052]. The subtle nature of this intervention, with its influence occurring outside conscious awareness (29), may contribute to its efficacy. In some cases, conscious aware- ness may undo the effects of an intervention (30). More overt interventions risk sending the stig- matizing message that the beneficiaries are seen as in need of help. They may also cause resist- ance and reactance and undermine the sense of accomplishment people take in their success (26). This study provides an experimental, longi- tudinal demonstration that a brief intervention to buttress feelings of social belonging can have significant effects on a wide range of important outcomes. The social-belonging intervention im- proved the academic performance, self-reported health, and well-being of ethnic minority students over 3 years. The results suggest that inequality between marginalized and nonmarginalized groups arises not only from structural factors but also from concern about social belonging. This concern can be mitigated by using a psychological remedy. The intervention provided students a nonthreatening frame for interpreting the daily challenges of school. By encouraging students to adopt this message as their own, the intervention made this message stick psycholog- ically. Along with other recent research, this study
  • 34. highlights how the impact of adversity depends on its perceived meaning—how it is subjective- ly construed (24–26, 31–33). Changing subjec- tive construal is a fruitful avenue for intervention because many events are ambiguous and ame- nable to multiple interpretations. Moreover, a change in construal can become self-reinforcing. Students who feel confident in their belonging may experience the social world in a way that re- inforces this feeling. They may initiate more rela- tionships and thus obtain more opportunities for belonging and growth. Brief interventions that shore up belonging can thus promote performance 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 European American Students
  • 35. African American Students B Accessibility of Negative Racial Stereotypes 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 European American Students African American Students A Self-Reported Belonging Uncertainty 0.0
  • 36. 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 European American Students African American Students C Accessibility of Self-Doubt RasdgasgRandomized Control Group Social-Belonging Treatment Fig. 3. Self-reported belonging uncertainty and the cognitive accessibility of negative racial stereotypes and of self-doubt 3 years postintervention. Error bars represent T1 SE. Data were combined across cohorts where measures were completed by both cohorts. (A) Self- reported belonging uncertainty (cohort 2). (B) Accessibility of negative racial stereotypes (cohorts 1 and 2).
  • 37. (C) Accessibility of self-doubt (cohorts 1 and 2). The y axis in (A) represents the full range of the scale. The y axes in (B) and (C) represent about 3.00 standard deviations. Sample sizes in cohort 2 only are NEuropean Americans = 20 and NAfrican Americans = 23. Sample sizes in cohorts 1 and 2 are NEuropean Americans = 31 and NAfrican Americans = 38. 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 European American Students African American
  • 39. 0.50 0.75 1.00 1.25 1.50 European American Students African American Students B Number of Reported Doctor Visits in the Previous Month RasdgasgRandomized Control Group Social-Belonging Treatment Fig. 4. Self-reported health and happiness 3 years postintervention. Error bars represent T1 SE. Data were combined across cohorts where measures were completed by both cohorts. (A) Self-assessed general health (cohort 2). (B) Reported doctor visits in the previous month (cohorts 1 and 2). (C) Subjective happiness (cohort 2). The y axes in (A) and (C) represent the full range of each scale. The y axis in (B) represents about 2.00 standard deviations. Sample sizes in cohort 2 only are NEuropean Americans = 20 and
  • 40. NAfrican Americans = 23. Sample sizes in cohorts 1 and 2 are NEuropean Americans = 31 and NAfrican Americans = 38. 18 MARCH 2011 VOL 331 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org1450 REPORTS o n A p ri l 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s ci e n ce m a
  • 41. g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ and well-being even long after their delivery (7, 20, 25, 31, 34). Importantly, the effectiveness of social- psychological interventions depends on factors in the context. Such interventions are unlikely to be effective in contexts without opportunities for learning. Also, because the present intervention works by changing people’s subjective interpre- tation of ambiguous events, it may be ineffective in openly hostile environments. Lastly, whether
  • 42. this intervention would work among younger or less-select students, or students from other mar- ginalized groups, is an important question for future research (20, 31, 34). These qualifica- tions noted, the results underscore the impor- tance of social belonging and subjective construal in contributing to social inequality and show how this insight can inform our collective efforts to promote equality in performance, health, and well-being. References and Notes 1. K. Magnuson, J. Waldfogel, Steady Gains and Stalled Progress: Inequality and the Black-White Test Score Gap (Russell Sage Foundation, New York, 2008). 2. C. M. Steele, S. J. Spencer, J. Aronson, in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed. (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2002), pp. 379–440. 3. D. R. Williams, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 896, 173 (1999). 4. R. F. Baumeister, M. R. Leary, Psychol. Bull. 117, 497 (1995). 5. J. T. Cacioppo, B. Patrick, Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection (Norton, New York, 2008) 6. S. Lyubomirsky, K. M. Sheldon, D. Schkade, Rev. Gen. Psychol. 9, 111 (2005). 7. G. M. Walton, G. L. Cohen, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 82 (2007). 8. L. F. Berkman, S. L. Syme, Am. J. Epidemiol. 109, 186 (1979).
  • 43. 9. S. Cohen, D. Janicki-Deverts, Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 375 (2009). 10. G. E. Miller, N. Rohleder, S. W. Cole, Psychosom. Med. 71, 57 (2009). 11. B. N. Uchino, J. Behav. Med. 29, 377 (2006). 12. N. I. Eisenberger, M. D. Lieberman, K. D. Williams, Science 302, 290 (2003). 13. K. D. Williams, in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed. (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2009), pp. 279–314. 14. R. F. Baumeister, J. M. Twenge, C. K. Nuss, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 817 (2002). 15. E. Goffman, Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity (Simon and Schuster, New York, 1963), p. 8. 16. R. Mendoza-Denton, G. Downey, V. J. Purdie, A. Davis, J. Pietrzak, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 896 (2002). 17. J. Blascovich, S. J. Spencer, D. Quinn, C. Steele, Psychol. Sci. 12, 225 (2001). 18. S. W. Cole, M. E. Kemeny, S. E. Taylor, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 320 (1997). 19. S. Cohen et al., Health Psychol. 27, 268 (2008). 20. G. L. Cohen, J. Garcia, V. Purdie-Vaughns, N. Apfel, P. Brzustoski, Science 324, 400 (2009). 21. T. D. Wilson, Science 313, 1251 (2006). 22. A previous paper reports relatively short-term results of
  • 44. this intervention from the first cohort of students (7). 23. Information on materials and methods is available as supporting material on Science Online. 24. T. D. Wilson, M. Damiani, N. Shelton, in Improving Academic Achievement: Impact of Psychological Factors on Education, J. Aronson, Ed. (Academic Press, Oxford, 2002), pp. 91–110. 25. J. Aronson, C. B. Fried, C. Good, J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 113 (2002). 26. L. Ross, R. Nisbett, The Person and the Situation (McGraw-Hill, New York, 1991). 27. J. Crocker, C. T. Wolfe, Psychol. Rev. 108, 593 (2001). 28. E. L. Idler, Y. Benyamini, J. Health Soc. Behav. 38, 21 (1997). 29. T. D. Wilson, Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 2002). 30. D. K. Sherman et al., J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 745 (2009). 31. L. S. Blackwell, K. H. Trzesniewski, C. S. Dweck, Child Dev. 78, 246 (2007). 32. J. J. Gross, R. A. Thompson, in Handbook of Emotion Regulation, J. J. Gross, Ed. (Guilford, New York, 2007),
  • 45. pp. 3–24. 33. J. P. Jamieson, W. B. Mendes, E. Blackstock, T. Schmader, J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 208 (2010). 34. G. L. Cohen, J. Garcia, N. Apfel, A. Master, Science 313, 1307 (2006). 35. We thank participating students and helpful administrators at an anonymous university for their involvement. We also thank P. Carr, E. Chen, C. Dweck, C. Logel, G. Miller, D. Paunesku, D. Sherman, S. Spencer, C. Steele, and D. Yeager for input and L. Cai, K. Crockett, J. Darwall, L. Egan, A. Master, M. Silverio, and R. Titcombe for assistance. This research was supported by grants from the American Psychological Association, the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues, and the Spencer Foundation awarded to G.M.W. and by a grant from the Russell Sage Foundation awarded to G.L.C. Supporting Online Material www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/331/6023/1447/DC1 Materials and Methods Tables S1 to S3 References 28 September 2010; accepted 14 February 2011 10.1126/science.1198364 Direct Interaction of RNA Polymerase II and Mediator Required for Transcription in Vivo Julie Soutourina,1* Sandra Wydau,1† Yves Ambroise,2 Claire Boschiero,1 Michel Werner1*
  • 46. Gene transcription is highly regulated. Altered transcription can lead to cancer or developmental diseases. Mediator, a multisubunit complex conserved among eukaryotes, is generally required for RNA polymerase II (Pol II) transcription. An interaction between the two complexes is known, but its molecular nature and physiological role are unclear. We identify a direct physical interaction between the Rpb3 Pol II subunit of Saccharomyces cerevisiae and the essential Mediator subunit, Med17. Furthermore, we demonstrate a functional element in the Mediator–Pol II interface that is important for genome-wide Pol II recruitment in vivo. Our findings suggest that a direct interaction between Mediator and Pol II is generally required for transcription of class II genes in eukaryotes. M ediator is a large multisubunit complex conserved in all eukaryotes (1). It acts as a link between specific protein regu- lators and the RNA polymerase II (Pol II) tran- scription machinery (2). Mediator is required at most Pol II–transcribed gene promoters for reg- ulated gene expression (3–5). In Saccharomyces cerevisiae, Mediator is composed of 25 subunits and is organized in four structural modules: the tail, middle, head, and Cdk8 modules (6). A di- rect Mediator–Pol II interaction is indicated by previous copurification, coimmunoprecipita- tion (CoIP) experiments (7–9) and by in vivo form- aldehyde cross-linking (10). A model of the Mediator–Pol II complex determined by electron microscopy (EM) at 35 Å resolution suggests
  • 47. that several Pol II subunits (Rpb1, 2, 3, 6, and 11) might contact the middle or the head of Mediator (11). It was recently suggested that Rpb4 and Rpb7 could also be implicated in interactions with Mediator (12–14). However, the requirement of a direct interaction between Mediator and Pol II for transcription activation has not been demon- strated. Moreover, the identity of the Mediator subunits contacting Pol II is unknown because of the low resolution of the Mediator structure. As a consequence, the mechanism by which Mediator recruits Pol II is poorly understood. To identify the subunit(s) of Mediator that directly contact Pol II and determine the role of these interactions in transcription regulation, we usedaninvivophoto–cross-linkingapproachbased on the incorporation by the cell-translation sys- tem of photo-activable analogs of methionine and leucine in proteins [see supporting online material (SOM) text and figs. S1 and S2] (15, 16). Because EM results (11) suggested potential interactions of 16 Mediator subunits belonging to the head (Med6, 8, 11, 17, 18, 19, 20, 22) and middle (Med1, 4, 5, 7, 9, 10, 21, 31) modules with Rpb1, 2, 3, 6, or 11 Pol II subunits, we immu- noprecipitated hemagglutinin (HA)–tagged pro- teins after in vivo cross-linking. Among the 80 pairwise contacts that we tested, only Myc-tagged Rpb3 and HA-tagged Med17 cross-linked (Fig. 1). These results demonstrate that the Rpb3 Pol II 1Commissariat à l’Energie Atomique et aux Energies Al- ternatives (CEA), iBiTec-S, Service de Biologie Intégrative et Génétique Moléculaire, F-91191 Gif-sur-Yvette cedex, France.
  • 48. 2CEA, iBiTec-S, Service de Chimie Bio-organique et Marquage, F-91191 Gif-sur-Yvette cedex, France. *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected] (J.S.); [email protected] (M.W.) †Present address: Université Paris Descartes, EA 4065, Faculté des Sciences Pharmaceutiques et Biologiques, 75006 Paris, France. www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 331 18 MARCH 2011 1451 REPORTS o n A p ri l 1 4 , 2 0 1 1 w w w .s
  • 49. ci e n ce m a g .o rg D o w n lo a d e d f ro m http://www.sciencemag.org/ Questions: 1. The article attempts to better understand the origins of inequality by looking at one specific issue faced by minorities in work and school. What is that issue, and why does it make sense to study it?
  • 50. 2. The authors suggest that subjective construal of hardships is more important than the objective number or attributes of the hardship. What does this mean, and why does this matter for this study? 3. The authors argue that there may be not only short-term effects to their intervention, but also long-term ones. Why do they think this? 4. This study included two participant variables (each with two conditions) for a total of four possible conditions. What were two variables, and what were the two conditions for each variable? 5. What were the short-term measures? What were the long-term measures? 6. What was the purpose of the fictional report that the participants read? How was the message of this report internalized? 7. Describe the trend in GPA throughout the remaining three years of college for participants in all four conditions. Who benefitted most from this intervention? 8. The researchers were not allowed to report any “raw” information on GPA, but their ability to communicate the effectiveness of the study relied on reporting GPA somehow. How did they get around this? 9. The authors argue that, even though the intervention focused on belongingness, it worked by changing how individuals view adversity and hardship. Explain their logic here.
  • 51. 10. Did the intervention have any effect on health? If so, what was it, and among whom was this effect found? 11. How well did participants generally remember the intervention? Why is this important to the effectiveness of the intervention? 12. The researchers describe several limitations to their intervention. What are two conditions they mention under which they do not expect to get the same effects as they did in this study? 019 10:05 AM Module 3 This week, our focus will be on grammar. If you have any questions regarding the assignments or anything else, please let me know. I am attaching an APA outline of the paper to help you get started. This week’s paper has 4 parts. The first part, you are rewriting the 2 passages for content and grammar. You can rearrange, and delete words in the passage to make it work. I understand everyone has learned English differently and might have learned different rules. Next, is the guided writing assignment. Make sure you include the rough draft and corrected version and label both. The third part is the list of words. First, describe how the word should be used, then state if it should be used in APA writing. Lastly, use only the words that you would use in APA writing in a sentence. The last part of this assignment is explaining the use of apostrophes in APA writing. For this assignment, you will have 4 level 1 headings (bold and
  • 52. centered) for each section of the paper. You will have level 2 headings for the first 2 assignments to separate them. Remember level 2 headings are bold and at the left margin