SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Assignment 3: Capstone Research Project
Due Week 10 and worth 440 points
Assume you are the partner in an accounting firm hired to
perform the audit on a fortune 1000 company. Assume also that
the initial public offering (IPO) of the company was
approximately five (5) years ago and the company is concerned
that, in less than five (5) years after the IPO, a restatement may
be necessary. During your initial evaluation of the client, you
discover the following information:
· The client is currently undergoing a three (3) year income tax
examination by the Internal Revenue Service (IRS). A
significant issue involved in the IRS audit encompasses
inventory write-downs on the tax returns that are not included
in the financial statements. Because of the concealment of the
transaction, the IRS is labeling the treatment of the write-down
as fraud.
· The company has a share-based compensation plan for top-
level executives consisting of stock options. The value of the
options exercised during the year was not expensed or disclosed
in the financial statements.
· The company has several operating and capital leases in place,
and the CFO is considering leasing a substantial portion of the
assets for future use. The current leases in place are arranged
using special purpose entities (SPEs) and operating leases.
· The company seeks to acquire a global partner, which will
require IFRS reporting.
· The company received correspondence from the Securities and
Exchange Commission (SEC) requesting additional
supplemental information regarding the financial statements
submitted with the IPO.
Write an eight to ten (8-10) page paper in which you:
1. Evaluate any damaging financial and ethical repercussions of
failure to include the inventory write-downs in the financial
statements. Prepare a recommendation to the CFO, evaluating
the negative impact of a civil fraud penalty on the corporation
as a result of the IRS audit. In the recommendation, include
essential internal control procedures to prevent fraudulent
financial reporting from occurring, as well as the major
obligation of the CEO and CFO to ensure compliance.
2. Examine the negative results on stakeholders and the
financial statements of an IRS audit which generates additional
tax and penalties or subsequent audits. Assume that the
subsequent audit and / or additional tax and penalties result
from the taxpayer’s use of an inventory reserve account,
applying a 10 percent reduction to inventory over three (3)
years.
3. Discuss the applicable federal tax laws, regulations, rulings,
and court cases related to the inventory write-downs, and
explain the specific relevance of each to the write-down.
4. Research the current generally accepted accounting principles
(GAAP) regarding stock option accounting. Evaluate the current
treatment of the company’s share-based compensation plan
based on GAAP reporting. Contrast the financial benefits and
risks of the share-based compensation stock option plan with
the financial benefits and risks of a share-based stock-
appreciation rights plan (SARS). Recommend to the CFO which
plan the company should use, and provide the correct
accounting treatment for each.
5. Research the reporting requirements for lease reporting under
GAAP and International Financial Reporting Standards (IFRS).
Based on your research, create a proposal for future lease
transactions to the CFO. Within the proposal, discuss the use of
off-the-balance sheet financing arrangements, capital leases,
and operating leases, and indicate the related business and
financial risks of each.
6. Create an argument for or against a single set of international
accounting standards related to lease accounting based on the
global market and cross border leases of assets. Examine the
benefits and risks of your chosen position.
7. Examine the major implications of SAS 99 based on the
factors you discovered during the initial evaluation of the
company. Provide support for your rationale.
8. Analyze the potential for a material misstatement in the
financial statements based on the issues identified in your
initial evaluation. Make a recommendation to the CFO for the
issuance of restated financial statement restatement.
Identify at least three (3) significant issues that can result from
the failure to issue restated financial statements.
9. Examine the economic effect of restatement of the financial
statements on investors, employees, customers, and creditors.
10. Use five (5) quality academic resources in this assignment.
Note: Wikipedia and other Websites do not qualify as academic
resources.
Your assignment must follow these formatting requirements:
· Be typed, double spaced, using Times New Roman font (size
12), with one-inch margins on all sides; citations and references
must follow APA or school-specific format. Check with your
professor for any additional instructions.
· Include a cover page containing the title of the assignment, the
student’s name, the professor’s name, the course title, and the
date. The cover page and the reference page are not included in
the required assignment page length.
The specific course learning outcomes associated with this
assignment are:
· Analyze accounting situations to apply the proper accounting
rules and make recommendations to ensure compliance with
generally accepted accounting principles.
· Analyze business situations to determine the appropriateness
of decision making in terms of professional standards and ethics
· Analyze business situations and apply advanced federal
taxation concepts.
· Use technology and information resources to research issues in
accounting.
· Write clearly and concisely about accounting using proper
writing mechanics.
Grading for this assignment will be based on answer quality,
logic / organization of the paper, and language and writing
skills, using the following rubric.
Click here to view the grading rubric.
NIST Technical Note 1619
Modeling Human Behavior during
Building Fires
Erica D. Kuligowski
NIST Technical Note 1619
Modeling Human Behavior during
Building Fires
Erica D. Kuligowski
Fire Research Division
Building and Fire Research Laboratory
December 2008
U.S. Department of Commerce
Carlos M. Gutierrez, Secretary
National Institute of Standards and Technology
James M. Turner, Deputy Director
Certain commercial entities, equipment, or materials may be
identified in this
document in order to describe an experimental procedure or
concept adequately. Such
identification is not intended to imply recommendation or
endorsement by the
National Institute of Standards and Technology, nor is it
intended to imply that the
entities, materials, or equipment are necessarily the best
available for the purpose.
National Institute of Standards and Technology Technical Note
1619
Natl. Inst. Stand. Technol. Tech. Note 1619, 21 pages
(December 2008)
CODEN: NSPUE2
Abstract
Evacuation models, including engineering hand calculations and
computational tools, are used to
evaluate the level of safety provided by buildings during
evacuation. Building designs and
occupant procedures are based on the results produced from
these models, including evacuation
time results (i.e., how long building occupants will take to
evacuate a building). However, most
evacuation models focus primarily on calculating and predicting
evacuation movement (i.e., how
long will it take an occupant to move from his/her initial
position to safety), almost ignoring the
prediction of behaviors that occupants perform before and
during evacuation movement that can
delay their safety (e.g., searching for information, fighting the
fire, and helping others). Instead of
modeling and predicting behavior of simulated occupants,
evacuation models and users often
make assumptions and simplifications about occupant behavior
(i.e., what people do during
evacuations) that can be unrealistic and are likely to produce
inaccurate results.
A solution to this problem is to generate a robust,
comprehensive, and validated theory on human
behavior during evacuation from building fires. The social
scientific literature can be gleaned to
develop these theories, which can then be incorporated into the
current evacuation models to
accurately simulate occupant behavior during fire evacuations.
These models can then achieve
more realistic results which will lead to safer, more efficient
building design.
The purpose of this paper is to reevaluate our current egress
modeling techniques and advocate
for the inclusion of a comprehensive conceptual model of
occupant behavior during building
fires. The paper begins by describing the current state of
evacuation modeling of human behavior
in fires and identifying gaps in current behavioral techniques.
The second part of the paper
outlines a general process model for occupant response to
physical and social cues in a building
fire event.
Keywords
fire, evacuation, egress, evacuation models, building fires
Acknowledgements
Thanks to Kathleen Tierney, Liam Downey, William
Grosshandler, Dennis Mileti, and Ross
Corotis for their efforts. My appreciation to Richard Peacock,
Jason Averill, Anthony Hamins,
and Steve Gwynne for providing detailed and insightful
suggestions.
Modeling Human Behavior during Building Fires
Introduction
In 2006, structure fires injured over 14 000 people and killed
over 2700 people in the United
States (USFA 2007). In many fires, there may be occupants who
are not able to self-evacuate,
such as disabled or intoxicated occupants. However, research on
fire injuries and deaths shows
that over two-thirds of the injured and over half of the dead in
building fires could have
evacuated; these people were performing activities that delayed
their safety, including fighting
the fire, attempting to rescue others in the building, and moving
to safety under untenable
conditions inside the building (Hall 2004).
Evacuation models, including engineering hand calculations
(Nelson and Mowrer 2002) and
computational tools, can be used to evaluate the level of safety
provided by buildings during
evacuation. Building designs and occupant procedures are based
on the results produced from
these models, primarily evacuation time results (i.e., how long
building occupants require to
evacuate a building). However, most evacuation models focus
primarily on calculating and
predicting evacuation movement (i.e., how long will it take an
occupant to move from his/her
initial position to safety), almost ignoring the prediction of
behaviors that occupants perform
before and during evacuation movement that can delay their
exit.
Instead of modeling and predicting behavior of simulated
occupants, evacuation models and users
often make assumptions and simplifications about occupant
behavior that can be unrealistic and
can produce inaccurate results. For example, some evacuation
models allow for the user to
assume that building occupants immediately begin to move to
the stairs or exits upon hearing a
fire alarm or sensing a fire. This assumption and others like it
can represent a scenario that is
unlikely to occur in an actual fire event and thereby
inappropriately influence the evacuation time
calculated by the model. In cases in which assumptions lead to
an unrealistic underestimation of
evacuation results (i.e., shorter evacuation times), buildings or
procedures are designed according
to an inadequate life safety design; e.g., insufficient egress
routes and/or unsafe procedures for
staff and/or occupants. In cases in which assumptions can lead
to an unrealistic overestimation of
evacuation results (i.e., longer evacuation times), buildings or
procedures are designed based on
an overestimation of egress needs, which can raise the cost of
buildings unnecessarily.
A solution to this problem is to generate a robust,
comprehensive, and validated theory on human
behavior during evacuation from building fires. The social
scientific literature and case studies
from disasters and building fires could be employed to develop
this theory, which could then be
incorporated into the current evacuation models to more
accurately simulate occupant behavior
during fire evacuations. These models would produce more
accurate results and benefit building
design. Until a comprehensive theory on occupant behavior is
developed for inclusion into
evacuation models, costly or ineffective egress or procedural
designs may be developed based on
the unquantified needs of the evacuating population.
Scope
The purpose of this paper is to reevaluate our current egress
modeling techniques and advocate
for the inclusion of a robust, comprehensive, and validated
conceptual model of occupant
behavior during building fires. This paper begins by describing
the current state of evacuation
modeling of human behavior in fires and identifying gaps in
current behavioral prediction
6
techniques. The second part of the paper outlines a model that
can predict occupant behavior in
response to the interpretations and decisions made regarding
physical and social cues in a
building fire. Although similar work has identified the lack of
behavioral simulation in evacuation
models (Santos and Aguirre 2005), no work has been completed
to systematically examine
research from the evacuation of both buildings and communities
to develop behavioral theory for
building evacuations.
Building Evacuation Models
How do building evacuation models work?
Evacuation models quantify evacuation performance by
calculating how long it takes for
occupants to evacuate a building. In order to make this
calculation, the model attempts to
simulate two things: 1) the actions that people take and 2) how
long it takes to perform each
action. In addition to total building evacuation times,
evacuation models can provide floor
clearing times and the location of the congestion points
throughout the building. However, due to
the lack of data and theory on occupant behavior/actions,
evacuation models significantly
simplify the evacuation process and many focus primarily on
how long it takes to perform one
kind of action: the movement of occupants from their initial
positions to the outside of the
building. In other words, the current evacuation models
primarily focus on the purposive
evacuation movement of the occupants and do not simulate
additional behaviors that may delay
evacuation to safety*.
In addition to purposive evacuation movement, occupants are
likely to engage in a variety of
other activities throughout their evacuation from the building
that can delay their movement to
safety. Such activities can include information gathering,
preparing for the evacuation by
gathering their personal belongings, assisting or even rescuing
others, alerting others in the
building, changing stairs, and fighting the fire. These actions
can take place during either period
of a building evacuation, either during the pre-evacuation
period or the evacuation period. The
pre-evacuation period is labeled as the period from the point
when the occupant is notified that
there is something wrong until s/he begins to travel an
evacuation route out of the building. The
evacuation period then ends when the occupant has reached a
point of safety or outside of the
building.
In the models that can account for occupant actions†, there are
two main methods used to
simulate occupant behavior during a building evacuation. One
method is for the user to assig
time period of delay/waiting (e.g., a specific period of time, a
distribution of times, etc.) to
individuals or groups in the simulated building to account for
any actions that they might perform
during the evacuation (e.g., Simulex
n a
‡ (Thompson, Wu and Marchant 1996; Spearpoint 2004),
EXIT89 (Fahy 2000; 1996), GridFlow (Bensilum and Purser
2002)). Using this method,
* Model exceptions to this include buildingEXODUS (Filippidis
et al. 2006; Gwynne et al. 1999a) and
CRISP (Fraser-Mitchell 1999). These models begin to address
behaviors performed by occupants during
building fires.
† There are a number of models that do not simulate occupant
behavior. These are labeled as movement
models (Kuligowski and Peacock 2005).
‡ Certain commercial entities, equipment, or materials may be
identified in this document in order to
describe an experimental procedure or concept adequately.
Such identification is not intended to imply
recommendation or endorsement by the National Institute of
Standards and Technology, nor is it intended
to imply that the entities, materials, or equipment are
necessarily the best available for the purpose.
7
simulated occupants remain stationary in their initial position
for a set period of time and th
begin purposive evacuation movement once this time period is
over. The other method is for the
user to assign a specific behavioral itinerary (i.e., a sequence of
actions) or a specific action to a
individual or group (e.g., CRISP (Fraser-Mitchell 1999),
buildingEXODUS (Filippidis et al.
2006; Gwynne et al. 1999a)). Action sequences can be used to
simulate behaviors that may
interrupt continuous movement, such as searching for
information, leaving the stairs, assisting
other occupants, and returning to initial locations to retrieve
personal or work items. Each action
performed is assigned a specific time for each occupant. An
example of a behavioral itine
the following: Occupant A is assigned a “search and rescue”
behavioral itinerary. To perform t
search and rescue mission, the model simulates that the
occupant moves from Point A (Occ
A’s original position) to Point B (another room in the building
where the rescue takes place),
waits for an assigned period of time at Point B, and then begins
purposive evacuation movem
from Point B to the st
en
n
rary is
he
upant
ent
airs.
Both of these methods of simulating behavior during evacuation
significantly simplify the
behavioral processes that take place during the evacuation. In
the first method, assigning a time
delay, an emphasis is placed on the time delay itself rather than
the decisions, actions, and
interactions made by the occupants in response to conditions
inside the building. The second
method, assigning a behavioral itinerary, begins to simulate
decisions and actions made in
response to certain conditions during the evacuation, however,
the entire behavior or behavioral
itinerary is defined by the user before the simulation begins
(rather than predicted by the model)
and interactions among other simulated occupants is simplified
or nonexistent.
There are problems with the approaches used by models to
simulate behavior during evacuation.
First, no behavior is actually predicted by the evacuation
models because behavioral information
is provided as prior, pre-programmed assumptions. Behavior is
determined by the user or
probabilistically by the model based on prescribed information.
More importantly, the user
prescribes the actions that will occur, or that s/he assumes may
occur, in each fire scenario. There
is no consistency associated with the prescription of behaviors;
this method relies entirely on the
user’s expertise in understanding occupant behavior in building
fires. This is an unrealistic
expectation of the user since there is no guidance,
comprehensive data set, or theory provided to
users about what people actually do during building
evacuations.
What are building evacuation models used for?
Currently, there are over 40 different evacuation models
(Kuligowski and Peacock 2005; Gwynne
et al. 1999b) available for use in three main types of projects.
These projects are safety
assessment evaluations (SAE), experimental work, and incident
re-creation (Gwynne 2000). Each
project type is unique to the purpose of the project, the use of
the evacuation model, and the
approval process used to evaluate the accuracy of the results, all
of which are described below.
During SAE projects, an evacuation model is used to assess the
safety of a particular building
design and/or egress procedure for the occupants in a building.
For these types of projects, the
model user is most likely an engineer or a life safety consultant
evaluating a new building design
or a design from an existing building undergoing a change, such
as a use or physical layout. For
SAE projects, the user typically runs various evacuation
simulations for the building (e.g.,
occupants travel via different building routes, occupants move
at various speeds, etc.) and
compares the evacuation simulation results with results from
fire modeling simulations. A
building is deemed to provide a sufficient level of life safety for
occupants if the amount of time
needed for evacuation of the building (evacuation modeling
results) is less than the time when
conditions become untenable for occupants inside the building
(fire modeling results). As a final
8
step in a SAE project, an authority having jurisdiction must
approve the safety analysis made by
the engineer, which is sometimes completed through a third-
party review process.
Evacuation models are also used in experimental projects. For
these projects, the evacuation
model is used to explore and investigate conditions that cannot
be easily examined otherwise. The
model user is often a consultant/engineer evaluating a variety of
different designs for the same
building or researchers and academics testing hypotheses on the
impact of building conditions on
results. For experimental projects, the user produces a variety of
egress results from the same
model from various input conditions. For example, the user can
simulate a variety of different
egress scenarios using the same model to test different aspects
of the building design, such as the
size of the stairwell(s), the number of stairs in the building, the
width of the corridors, the width,
location, and number of exits, etc. In this example, the user may
be interested in which designs
provide sufficiently fast evacuation times for building
occupants. Other examples include the
testing of hypotheses related to the impact of fire conditions on
people movement through the
building.
Evacuation models can also be used in incident re-creation
projects. The purpose of these projects
is often to determine the cause(s) of actual incident outcomes
(e.g., why so many people perished
in a particular fire) and/or answer particular questions about the
incident itself (e.g., what would
have happened if the building was more densely populated
during the fire event, if the building
had more exits, etc?). Model users are likely to be fire
investigators, researchers, engineers,
consultants and others charged with answering questions about
an actual event. In order to
determine causes behind actual incident results, users will
attempt to model the actual incident,
which may include a series of modeling runs, as close to the
actual conditions as possible by
using all known conditions from the event. If additional
questions are asked (e.g., what-if
questions), the user can develop a base-line case and then alter
specific conditions in the building
to answer the appropriate questions (e.g., adding more people to
the building and rerunning the
simulation). Evacuation models were used in the investigations
of the collapse of the World
Trade Center Towers in 2001 (Galea et al. 2008; Averill et al.
2005) and the Rhode Island
Nightclub fire (Galea et al. 2008; Grosshandler et al. 2005) to
answer specific questions.
A comprehensive theory of human behavior in fire can improve
building evacuation models for
all three types of projects. However, the generation of a
comprehensive theory is most important
for SAE and experimental projects. Whereas the user attempts
to model behaviors that are already
known in incident re-creation project, the user relies on the
model for accuracy in simulating an
event that has not yet occurred for SAE and experimental
projects. Data and theory on human
behavior during evacuations is necessary for accurate
evacuation modeling results.
What is needed to improve building evacuation models?
A comprehensive theory of occupant behavior in evacuations
from building fires is needed to
improve the current building evacuation models. The theory
should be able to predict individual
behavior and group dynamics that are likely to occur in a
building fire, rather than relying on ad-
hoc user-prescription. This would take the burden away from
the user to prescribe actions, which
can lead to inconsistency and inaccuracy, and actually allow the
model to predict behaviors that
emerge from situational conditions.
More specifically, a theory should simulate the variety of
behaviors performed by occupants in a
building fire (e.g., seek information, warn, rescue, and prepare).
In mass crowd events, where
occupants are densely located in the same area and receive cues
together as a group, occupants
are likely to respond in similar ways to the cues presented
(Purser and Gwynne 2007; Santos and
9
Aguirre 2005). In these types of events, most occupants are
likely to be influenced by the group
throughout the entire evacuation. However, in most building
fires, occupants are located in
different places throughout the building, many times receiving
different instructions or cues from
the event. Occupants respond in a variety of ways based on the
different cues that are presented to
them; and even occupants presented with the same cues are
likely to act in different ways (Mileti
and Sorensen 1990). This is because occupants’ actions vary
based on the cues that they perceive,
their interpretations of the event and risk, and the decisions that
they make about next steps. With
this in mind, it is crucial to develop a theory of occupant
behavior in building fires based on
social/behavior processes.
Theory of Occupant Behavior during Building Fires
Social scientific theory has acknowledged for over 70 years
(Mead 1938) that human action or
response is the result of a process. Instead of actions based on
random chance or even actions
resulting directly from a change in the environment, an
individual’s actions are frequently the
result of a decision-making process. Research in disasters,
based on social scientific theory, has
led to the development of social-psychological process models
for public warning response
(Mileti and Sorensen 1990; Perry, Lindell and Greene 1981;
Mileti and Beck 1975). These
models specify that people go through a process of specific
phases, including hearing,
understanding, believing, and personalizing the warning, in
which they consider aspects of their
response before performing an act (Mileti and Sorensen 1990).
Additionally, researchers of fire
evacuations (Bryan 2002; Feinberg and Johnson 1995; Tong and
Canter 1985; Edelman, Herz and
Bickman 1980; Breaux, Canter and Sime 1976) have shown that
a process involving the phases of
recognition and interpretation of the environment influence
occupant actions. In these process
models, there are specific cue- and occupant-related factors that
influence the outcome of each
phase of the process (e.g., whether the person hears the warning
or interprets the situation
correctly). Cue-related factors are described later in this paper
and occupant-related factors
include demographics (e.g., gender, age, income, education,
race, and marital status), previous
experiences, and knowledge. An understanding of the
behavioral process and the influential
factors of each phase can be developed into a conceptual model
to predict the types of individual
behaviors that are likely to occur in building fires.
The behavioral process for the pre-evacuation or evacuation
period of building fires is shown in
Figure 1. This process suggests that an occupant’s actions are a
result of his/her perceptions,
interpretations and decisions made based upon the external and
internal (occupant-based) cues
presented in the fire situation. During a building fire, occupants
or groups will begin a behavioral
process only when presented with event-related information that
interrupts their daily routine. A
new behavioral process begins each time an occupant/group
receives new information relating to
the fire event, and a specific action is likely to occur based on
whether the information is
perceived, the interpretations of the cue, the situation, and the
risk are developed, and the
decisions are made on what to do next.
Disaster and fire theory (Mileti and Sorensen 1990; Perry,
Lindell and Greene 1981; Edelman,
Herz and Bickman 1980; Breaux, Canter and Sime 1976; Mileti
and Beck 1975) suggests that
individuals engage in a sequence of phases during each
behavioral process. In other words, in
building fires, interpretation of the cue is possible only if the
cue is perceived; an accurate
interpretation of the situation is more likely if cues are
interpreted accurately; the interpretation of
the risk to themselves and others is more likely if the situation
is interpreted accurately; and the
occupants are more likely to decide on a certain type of action
if they perceive cues and formulate
10
accurate interpretations of the event and risk. Each phase will
be described in further detail in the
following section.
Phase 3: Decision-making
Phase 2: Interpretation of
the cue, situation, and risk
Cue- and
occupant-
related factors
Phase 4: Actions
Phase 1: Perception
of the cue(s)
Figure 1: A conceptual model of the behavioral process for
building fires
Phase 1 of the behavioral process involves occupants perceiving
or receiving external physical
and social cues from their environment. In a building fire,
occupants are constantly presented
with external cues (Brennan 1999). These cues can be physical
or social in nature, meaning that
they arise from the physical environment or the social
environment, respectively. Examples of
physical cues in a building fire include flames, smoke, breaking
glass, debris, tone alarms, and
automatic warnings. Examples of social cues in a building fire
include attempted communication
from others inside or outside of the building, unofficial or
authority-given warnings, and actions
taken by the building population. These cues can be presented
one by one or several at a time,
depending upon the event. The perception phase involves an
occupant receiving or noticing cues
that makes him/her aware that something in his/her environment
has changed (Weick 1995;
Starbuck and Milliken 1988; Canter, Breaux and Sime 1980).
Physical and social cues produced
in a building fire can be perceived by occupants through hearing
(e.g., an alarm or authority
warning), smelling (e.g., smoke), seeing (e.g., others running),
tasting (e.g., sulfur dioxide or
hydrogen chloride), and/or touching (e.g., heat).
In the interpretation phase, Phase 2, the occupant or group
attempts to interpret the information
provided by the cues perceived during the perception phase
(Weick 1995; Canter, Donald and
Chalk 1992; Turner and Killian 1987). Interpretation can be
seen as the process of organizing
perceived cues into a framework (Weick 1995); constructing a
meaningful story based on an
outcome (that is, the cue itself) (Weick 1993); and/or making
sense of a situation by imagining
what is going on (Rudolph and Morrison 2007; Klein 1999).
Interpretation methods include the
recall of previously developed behavioral scripts (or a sequence
of expected behaviors based
upon a situation) (Gioia and Poole 1984), mental simulation
(Klein 1999), the use of mental
models (Burns 2005), sensemaking (Rudolph and Raemer 2004;
Weick 1995; Weick 1993), and
collective behavior processes such as milling or intensified
interaction (Dynes and Tierney 1994;
11
Marx and McAdam 1994; Goode 1992; McPhail 1991; Miller
1985; Berk 1974; Smelser 1962;
Turner and Killian 1957). Occupants engage in such methods
during fires and other emergencies,
because these events create the need for interpretation by
interrupting and disrupting normal
interaction patterns and creating uncertainty. Behavioral scripts,
mental simulation and modeling,
and individual sensemaking are interpretation processes
performed internally by an occupant to
mentally formulate an interpretation. Occupants use behavioral
scripts to interpret an event when
the cues evoke memories of a previous situation in which a
previous interpretation was formed
(Gioia and Poole 1984). Mental simulation and modeling allows
the occupant to develop
cognitive images of what is going on in his/her environment
based on the cues that s/he has
received. Essentially, the occupant begins to paint a mental
picture or story of the event based on
the outcome (e.g., the cues).
Group sensemaking and collective behavior involve interaction
among occupants to collectively
develop an understanding of the emergent situation. In new
and/or ambiguous situations (Turner
and Killian 1987) and times of urgency (Aguirre, Wenger and
Vigo 1998), occupants are likely to
interact with others around them. This type of interaction,
which is intensified in densely
populated buildings, has been documented in a variety of
different incidents (e.g., Averill et al.
2005; Bryan 1983) as a means to establish what is going on,
define the new situation at hand, and
propose and adopt new appropriate norms for behavior (Aguirre,
Wenger and Vigo 1998; Turner
and Killian 1987). It is through individual and group-based
reasoning strategies that occupants
can begin to construct meaning from the cues that they perceive
in fire situations. In these types
of situations, leaders can emerge that suggest interpretations of
the event, which can then be
incorporated into the occupant’s interpretation.
In fires and other extreme events, there are three main
interpretation stages: interpreting the cues
received, interpreting the situation (i.e. as a fire), and
interpreting or defining the risk to the self
and/or others. This process is shown in Figure 2. These three
interpretation stages do not follow a
linear, ordered pattern; instead, interpretive stages can overlap
and inform one another in various
ways. In fire events, however, it is likely that if occupants
interpret a cue correctly (e.g., as a fire
alarm, smoke, or an explosion), they are likely to interpret the
situation correctly (e.g., as a fire
situation), which in turn makes it more likely that they will
interpret the situation as risky to
themselves and/or to others (Wiegman et al. 1992; Perry and
Greene 1983).
Risk Situation Cue
Interpretation
Figure 2: The interpretation phase involves interpreting the cue,
the situation, and the risk
If the occupant recognizes the cue, defines the situation
correctly, and understands the risk, s/he is
likely to perform protective actions in order to begin the
evacuation process. Initially, however,
occupants are predisposed to interpret the situation as if nothing
is wrong, known as normalcy
bias (Okabe and Mikami 1982), and that they are not at risk. In
a state of normalcy, inaction or
waiting is likely to occur. The interpretation of the risk phase is
essential to understand, because
in order for people to act, they must interpret a situation as
dangerous (Aguirre 2005).
12
Phase 3 of the behavioral process, decision-making, involves
occupants or groups making
decisions on what to do next based on their interpretations of
the cues, situations, and risks. The
decision-making phase is a two-step process in which occupants
initially search for options of
what to do and then choose one of the options (Gigerenzer and
Selton 2001).
The first step in the decision-making phase is to search for
options of what to do based on
interpretations of the event. Research literature suggests that
occupants develop their options by
performing mental simulation (Gwynne et al. 1999a; Thompson
et al. 1997), similar to the
methods of developing interpretations. Mental simulation (Klein
1999) allows an occupant to
mentally structure scenarios on what s/he would do and how
s/he would do it in the current
situation. The search for options becomes the process of
mentally developing scenarios of action
before actually performing the act.
The search for options of what to do can also occur collectively
in groups (Turner and Killian
1987). In addition to interpreting an event, groups work
together to plan a coordinated action that
will solve the problem presented by the interpretation, if any.
Suggestions for actions can come
from any member of the group, although leaders are likely to
emerge with suggestions of next
actions (Connell 2001; Turner and Killian 1987). In the face of
uncertainty and time pressure,
people are likely to come together, share their interpretations,
and define plans for collective
action in an event.
Occupants or groups are unlikely to search for a large number
of options during the decision-
making phase. Research suggests that individuals and groups
are likely to develop a very small,
even narrow range of decision options due to the following
conditions: 1) perceived time pressure
(Karau and Kelly 1992; Zakay 1993; Janis 1982; Ben-Zur and
Breznitz 1981); 2) limited mental
resources (Simon 1956; Gigerenzer and Selten 2001; Vaughan
1999); and/or 3) training and
knowledge of procedures (Klein 1999; Thompson et al. 1997).
Time pressure, likely in a fire
event, causes occupants/groups to perceive a fewer number of
cues, process the information less
thoroughly and in turn, to consider a narrow set of options
(Karau and Kelly 1992). Also, people
do not expend large amounts of intellectual resources, but rather
are likely to envision only the
scenarios that they believe are necessary to reach a goal
(Gigerenzer and Selten 2001). Finally,
research suggests that occupants who are highly trained and/or
know of specific procedures will
be guided by training and will likely not develop more than one
option at a time (Klein 1999).
The second stage in the decision-making phase is to choose one
of the options to perform.
Rationally-based research claims that occupants will optimize
their decision-making by
considering all options developed and choosing the best one –
known as rational choice strategy
(Slovic, Fischhoff and Lichtenstein 1977; Peterson and Beach
1967). In a fire situation, weighing
of multiple options is unlikely to occur. Research on decision-
making under uncertainty indicates
that occupants use a variety of heuristics to make this choice
(Klein 1999; Kahneman, Slovic and
Tversky 1982). Heuristics are simple rules to explain how
individuals make decisions. Whereas
some research might view the use of heuristics as a source of
bias in decision-making (Tversky
and Kahneman 1982), other researchers see heuristics as
strengths based on the use of expertise
(Flin et al. 1997). Examples of heuristics that occupants employ
in choosing options include
anchoring or focusing on the first option developed (Kahneman,
Slovic and Tversky 1982),
choosing the most available option (the easiest to develop or
recall) (Kahneman, Slovic and
Tversky 1982), comparing all options with each other and
choosing one based on the evaluation
criteria (Orasanu and Fischer 1997; Janis and Mann 1977;
Hammond and Adelman 1976), and
satisficing (Simon 1956).
13
Satisficing (Slovic, Kunreuther and White 1974; Gigerenzer and
Selten 2001) is a method in
which an individual chooses the first option that seems to work,
though not necessarily the best
option overall (Klein 1999). The satisficing heuristic actually
combines the processes of option
development and option choice together in one step. As the
decision-maker develops options, s/he
evaluates each one as it is developed and stops developing
options when one is deemed to satisfy
the search criteria. Whereas the rational choice strategy is more
likely to be used when people
attempt to optimize a decision (Klein 1999), satificing is more
likely to be used in situations with
a greater time pressure, dynamic conditions, and ill-defined
goals (Klein 1999).
In Phase 4 of the behavioral process, occupants may perform the
action that they decided upon in
the decision-making phase. If new information/cues are
presented before an action is performed,
the occupant will discard the current action and begin the
behavioral process again. The action
involves performing some type of physical act, although the act
could be waiting or even
inaction, that takes some amount of time to complete. Both
summary research (e.g., Bryan 2002;
Proulx 2002; Tong and Canter 1985) and research on specific
incidents (e.g., Averill et al. 2005;
Isner and Klem 1993; Bryan 1982; Best 1977) highlight certain
actions in which occupants are
likely to engage. These actions, depending upon the situation,
can include seeking information,
waiting, investigating the incident, alerting others, preparing for
evacuation, assisting others,
fighting the fire, and searching for and rescuing others. For
general purposes, labeling these
activities as actions would be appropriate; however, when
developing a behavioral model and
eventually a computer model, it is important to distinguish
between the goals and actions that
occupants undertake (Ozel 1985). A goal is an overall objective
of the occupant (e.g., fighting the
fire) which translates into a series of actions that lead toward
achieving that objective (e.g.,
occupant will travel to the location of the fire extinguisher, then
the location of the fire, etc.).
Discussion
The current evacuation models, as shown in Table 1, either do
not simulate occupant behavior at
all (Table 1a) or simulate occupant behavior by relying on the
user to pre-determine the types of
response delays or occupant actions that are likely to occur
(Table 1b). In both instances,
occupant behavior is simplified in such a way that may
inappropriately influence the evacuation
time(s) calculated by the model. In one case, occupant behavior
is ignored (Table 1a) and in the
other case, occupant behavior is simulated either as a
distributed delay time or as an imposed
action or action sequence that involves isolated movement and
delays rather than occupant
interaction and group dynamics (Table 1b). In addition, the
current behavioral models (Table 1b)
require users to provide a large amount of input data on
occupant delays and/or action sequences.
Most of this information would be impossible to provide since it
is required before the evacuation
simulation begins, i.e., before the conditions of the scenario are
established by the model.
Therefore, this paper proposes the development of a conceptual
model (shown in Table 1c) that
relies on data and theory imbedded in the evacuation model to
predict occupant actions. In the
proposed conceptual model, occupant actions are a result of the
developing conditions of the
simulation (the fire, the building, and the actions of other
occupants) which become input into the
occupant decision-making process. There are many benefits to
the development of a
comprehensive conceptual model for the field of human
behavior in building fires. The inclusion
of a conceptual model into computer evacuation tools will
enable a comprehensive model that can
actually predict occupant behavior in a building fire and require
the user to provide only initial
input for the scenario (i.e., information about the fire scenario,
the building and the characteristics
of the people). A computer model that incorporates a complete
behavioral conceptual model
would be able to predict situations rather than engineer an
outcome based heavily on user input.
14
A conceptual model will reduce the burden placed on users of
evacuation models and rely on the
model to simulate behavior during an event. Additionally, a
comprehensive behavioral model of
building evacuations illustrates where more data needs to be
collected in order to truly understand
human behavior in future fire evacuations.
Table 1: Diagrams representing three different evacuation
model types
a. Current Non-
Behavioral Model
b. Current Behavioral Model c. Proposed Behavioral
Model
No behavior is simulated.
The population or the
individual begins to
evacuate via the defined
route as soon as the event
begins.
The user determines the types of
responses that occupants are likely
to take during the model
configuration stage and these
actions are carried out during the
simulation. A group or individual
in the simulation can either delay
and/or perform an action or series
of actions during the evacuation
based on initial, user-defined
inputs.
The model’s underlying data
and theory predict the
behavioral responses taken
by the occupants based on
their perceived cues and the
interpretations/decisions
made. An individual in the
simulation develops an
interpretation of the cue, the
situation and the risk and
then makes a decision on
what to do next. This can
occur for an individual each
time a cue is received.
Movement
to Safety
Event is
Initiated
Event is
Initiated
Event is
Initiated
Cue
Interpretations
Response
User
Action(s) Delay
Future Research – How Can We Use This Theory?
The next step in developing a comprehensive theory is to
analyze qualitative data from actual
events to identify the various cue-related and occupant-related
factors that influence each phase
of the behavioral process. An example is provided here: data
from an actual event could show
that certain cues, for example, a fire alarm, influence a “false
alarm interpretation” for certain
types of occupants and “only a low amount of perceived risk,”
which leads to the performance of
certain longer-delay activities: e.g., continued working,
continued sleeping, milling behavior;
whereas instructions to evacuate provided by a member of the
fire department produce a
15
completely different behavioral process and eventual set of
actions. Once these influential factors
are linked to specific interpretations, risk perceptions and
activities, a behavioral model can be
developed. Then, the behavioral theory could be translated into
programming language that can
be tested and used in current evacuation models. This model
will need to be validated, however,
with behavioral data from other fire evacuation events.
Conclusion
Evacuation models are incomplete and oversimplified – they do
not account for actual occupant
behavior during buildings fires. A solution to this problem is to
generate a comprehensive, robust,
and validated theory on human behavior during evacuation from
building fires.
Behavior during a building fire evacuation is the result of a
behavioral process. Each process
begins with new cues and information from the physical and
social environment. First, cues need
to be perceived, then they are interpreted, and then a decision is
made as to what action (including
inaction) is undertaken. During an evacuation, individuals
repeat this process several times as
they engage in a variety of different activities both before and
during purposive evacuation
movement.
The social scientific literature and case studies from fires and
disasters can be gleaned to develop
this theory, which can then be incorporated to update the
current evacuation models to more
accurately simulate occupant behavior during fire evacuations.
With more accurate and realistic
evacuation models and results, engineers and sociologists can
develop safer and more cost-
effective procedures and building designs in the future.
16
References
Aguirre, Benigno E. 2005. “Emergency Evacuations, Panic, and
Social Psychology: Commentary on
‘Understanding Mass Panic and Other Collective Responses to
Threat and Disaster.’” Article
#402. Newark, DE: University of Delaware, Disaster Research
Center.
Aguirre, B. E., Dennis Wenger, and Gabriela Vigo. 1998. “A
Test of the Emergent Norm Theory of
Collective Behavior.” Sociological Forum 13:301-320.
Averill, Jason D., Dennis S. Mileti, Richard D. Peacock, Erica
D. Kuligowski, Norman Groner, Guylene
Proulx, Paul A. Reneke and Harold E. Nelson. 2005. “Federal
Building and Fire Safety
Investigation of the World Trade Center Disaster: Occupant
Behavior, Egress, and Emergency
Communications.” Report NCSTAR 1-7. Gaithersburg, MD:
National Institute of Standards and
Technology. http://wtc.nist.gov/NISTNCSTAR1-7.pdf
Bensilum, M. and David A. Purser. 2002. “Gridflow: An object-
oriented building evacuation model
combining pre-movement and movement behaviours for
performance-based design.” In 7th
International Symposium on Fire Safety Science, edited by D.
Evans. London, England:
Interscience Communications Ltd.
Ben Zur, H. and S. J. Breznitz. 1981. “The Effect of Time
Pressure on Risky Choice Behavior.” Acta
Psychologica 47:89-104.
Berk, Richard A. 1974. Collective Behavior. Dubuque, IO: WM.
C. Brown Company Publishers.
Best, R. L. 1977. Reconstruction of a Tragedy: The Beverly
Hills Supper Club Fire. Quincy, MA: National
Fire Protection Association.
Breaux, J., David Canter, and Jonathan D. Sime. 1976.
“Psychological Aspects of Behaviour of People in
Fire Situations.” Pp. 39-50 in International Fire Protection
Seminar, 5th. Karlsruhe, West
Germany.
Brennan, Patricia. 1999. “Modelling Cue Recognition and Pre-
Evacuation Response.” Pp. 1029-1040 in
Proceedings – 6th International Symposium of Fire Safety
Science. London, England: International
Association for Fire Safety Science.
Bryan, John L. 2002. “Behavioral Response to Fire and Smoke.”
Pp. 3-315 – 3-341 in The SFPE Handbook
of Fire Protection Engineering Third Edition, edited by P.J.
DiNenno. Quincy, MA: National Fire
Protection Association.
Bryan, John L. 1983. An Examination and Analysis of the
Dynamics of the Human Behavior in the MGM
Grand Hotel Fire. Quincy, MA: National Fire Protection
Association.
Bryan, John L. 1982. “Human Behavior in the MGM Grand
Hotel Fire.” Fire Journal 76:37-48.
Burns, Kevin. 2005. “Mental Models and Normal Errors.” Pp.
15-28 in How Professionals Make
Decisions, edited by H. Montgomery, R. Lipshitz and B.
Brehmer. Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum
Associates.
Canter, David, John Breaux and Jonathan Sime. 1980.
“Domestic, Multiple Occupancy and Hospital Fires.”
Pp. 117-136 in Fires and Human Behaviour, edited by David
Canter. New York, NY: John Wiley
& Sons.
Canter, David, Ian Donald, and Judith Chalk. 1992. “Pedestrian
Behaviour during Emergencies
Underground: The psychology of crowd control under life
threatening circumstances.” Pp. 135-
150 in Safety in Road and Rail Tunnels, edited by A. Vardy.
Bedford: Independent Technical
Conferences Ltd.
17
http://wtc.nist.gov/NISTNCSTAR1-7.pdf
Connell, Rory. 2001. “Collective Behavior in the September 11,
2001 Evacuation of the World Trade
Center.” Preliminary Paper #313. Newark: DE: University of
Delaware Disaster Research Center.
Dynes, Russell R. and Kathleen J. Tierney. 1994. Disasters,
Collective Behavior, and Social Organizations.
Newark, DE: University of Delaware Press.
Edelman, P., E. Herz, and L. Bickman. 1980. “A Model of
Behaviour in Fires Applied to a Nursing Home
Fire.” Pp. 181-203 in Fires and Human Behaviour, edited by D.
Canter. New York, NY: John
Wiley & Sons.
Fahy, Rita F. 2000. “New Developments in EXIT89.” Pp. 153-
159 in Proceedings of the 15th Joint Panel
Meeting of the U.S./Japan Government Cooperative Program on
Natural Resources. Fire
Research and Safety, edited by S.L. Bryner. San Antonio, TX.
Fahy, Rita F. 1996. “EXIT89 -- High-rise Evacuation Model --
Recent Enhancements and Example
Applications.” Pp. 1001-1005 in Interflam '96, International
Interflam Conference -- 7th
Proceedings, edited by C. A. Franks and S. Grayson. London,
England: Interscience
Communications Ltd.
Feinberg, W. E. and Norris R. Johnson. 1995. “Firescap: A
Computer Simulation Model of Reaction to a
Fire Alarm.” Journal of Mathematical Sociology 20:247-269.
Filippidis, L., E. Galea, S. Gwynne, P. Lawrence. 2006.
“Representing the Influence of Signage on
Evacuation Behaviour within an Evacuation Model.” Journal of
Fire Protection Engineering
16:37-73.
Flin, Rhona, Eduardo Salas, Michael Strub, and Lynne Martin.
1997. Decision Making Under Stress.
England, UK: Ashgate Publishing Ltd.
Fraser-Mitchell, J. N. 1999. “Modelling Human Behavior within
the Fire Risk Assessment Tool “CRISP.”
Fire and Materials 23:349-355.
Galea, E. R., G. Sharp, P. J. Lawrence, and R. Holden. 2008.
“Approximating the Evacuation of the World
Trade Center North Tower using Computer Simulation.” Journal
of Fire Protection Engineering
18:85-115.
Galea, Edwin, Zhaozhi Wang, Anand Veeraswamy, Fuchen Jia,
Peter J. Lawrence, and John Ewer. 2008.
“Coupled Fire/Evacuation Analysis of the Station Nightclub
Fire.” In the 9th International
Symposium on Fire Safety Science.
Gigerenzer, G. and R. Selten. 2001. Bounded Rationality: The
Adaptive Toolbox. Cambridge, MA: The
MIT Press.
Gioia, Dennis A. and Peter P. Poole. 1984. “Scripts in
Organizational Behavior.” Academy of Management
Review 9:449-459.
Goode, Erich. 1992. Collective Behavior. New York, NY:
Magickal Childe Inc.
Grosshandler, William, Nelson Bryner, Daniel Madrzykowski,
and K. Kuntz. 2005. “Report of the
Technical Investigation of The Station Nightclub Fire” Report
NCSTAR 2: Vol. 1. Gaithersburg,
MD: National Institute of Standards and Technology.
Gwynne, Steve M. V. 2000. “The introduction of adaptive social
decision-making in the mathematical
modelling of egress behaviour.” Ph.D. Thesis, University of
Greenwich, London, UK.
18
Gwynne, S., E. R. Galea, P. J. Lawrence, M. Owen and L.
Filippidis. 1999a. “Adaptive Decision-Making in
buildingEXODUS.” Journal of Applied Fire Science 8:265-289.
Gwynne, S., E. R. Galea, P. J. Lawrence, M. Owen, and L.
Filippidis. 1999b. “A Review of the
Methodologies used in the Computer Simulation of Evacuation
from the Built Environment.”
Building and Environment 34:741-749.
Hall, John R. 2004. “How Many People Can Be Saved from
Home Fires if Given More Time to Escape?”
Fire Technology 40:117-126.
Hammond, K. R. and L. Adelman. 1976. “Science, Values, and
Human Judgment.” Science 194:389-396.
Isner, M. S. and T. J. Klem. 1993. World Trade Center
Explosion and Fire, New York, New York, February
26, 1993. Fire Investigation Report. Quincy, MA: National Fire
Protection Association.
Janis, I. L. 1982. “Decision Making Under Stress.” Pp. 69-87 in
Handbook of Stress: Theoretical and
clinical aspects, edited by L. Goldberger and S. Breznitz. New
York, NY: The Free Press.
Janis, I. L. and L. Mann. 1977. Decision Making: A
psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and
commitment. New York, NY: Free Press.
Kahneman, Daniel, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky. 1982.
Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and
Biases. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Karau, Steven J. and Janice R. Kelly. 1992. “The Effects of
Time Scarcity and Time Abundance on Group
Performance Quality and Interaction Process.” Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology
28:542-571.
Klein, Gary. 1999. Sources of Power: How People Make
Decisions. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Kuligowski, Erica D. and Richard D. Peacock. 2005. “Review of
Building Evacuation Models.” Report
NIST TN 1471. Gaithersburg, MD: National Institute of
Standards and Technology.
Marx, Gary T. and Douglas McAdam. 1994. Collective Behavior
and Social Movements. Upper Saddle
River, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
McPhail, Clark. 1991. The Myth of the Madding Crowd. New
York, NY: Walter de Gruyter, Inc.
Mead. George Herbert. 1938. The Philosophy of the Act. Edited
by C.W. Morris with J.M. Brewster, A.M.
Dunham, & D. Miller. Chicago: University of Chicago.
Mileti, Dennis S. and E. M. Beck. 1975. "Communication in
Crisis: Explaining Evacuation Symbolically."
Communication Research 2(1):24-49.
Mileti, Dennis S. and John H. Sorensen. 1990. “Communication
of Emergency Public Warnings: A Social
Science Perspective and State-of-the-Art Assessment.” Oak
Ridge, TN: Oak Ridge National
Laboratory, U.S. Department of Energy.
Miller, David L. 1985. Introduction to Collective Behavior.
Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press, Inc.
Nelson, Harold E. and Frederick W. Mowrer. 2002. “Emergency
Movement.” Pp. 3-367 – 3-380 in The
SFPE Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering Third Edition,
edited by P.J. DiNenno. Quincy,
MA: National Fire Protection Association.
19
Okabe, Keizo, and Shunji Mikami. 1982. “A Study on the
Socio-Psychological Effect of a False Warning
of the Tokai Earthquake in Japan.” A Paper presented at the
Tenth World Congress of Sociology,
Mexico City, Mexico, August.
Orasanu, J. and U. Fischer. 1997. “Finding Decisions in Natural
Environments: The view from the
cockpit.” Pp. 343-358 in Naturalistic Decision Making, edited
by C. Zsambok and G. Klein.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ozel, F. 1985. “A Stochastic Computer Simulation of the
Behavior of People in Fires: An Environmental
Cognitive Approach.” In Proceedings of the International
Conference on Building Use and Safety
Technology. Washington, DC: National Institute of Building
Sciences.
Perry, Ronald W. and Marjorie R. Greene. 1983. Citizen
Response to Volcanic Eruptions: The Case of Mt.
St. Helens. New York, NY: Irvington Publishers.
Perry, Ronald W., Michael K. Lindell, and Marjorie R. Greene.
1981. Evacuation Planning in Emergency
Management. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Peterson, C. R. and L. R. Beach. 1967. “Man as an Intuitive
Statistician.” Psychological Bulletin 68:29-46.
Proulx, Guylene. 2002. “Movement of People: The Evacuation
Timing.” Pp. 3-342 – 3-365 in The SFPE
Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering Third Edition, edited
by P.J. DiNenno. Quincy, MA:
National Fire Protection Association.
Purser, David A. and Steven M. V. Gwynne. 2007 “Identifying
Critical Evacuation Factors and the
Application of Egress Models. Pp. 203-214 in Proceedings of
the 11th International Interflam
Conference, edited by C. A. Franks and S. Grayson. London,
England: Interscience
Communications Ltd.
Rudolph, Jenny W., J. Bradley Morrison. 2007. “Confidence,
Error, and Ingenuity in Diagnostic Problem
Solving: Clarifying the Role of Exploration and Exploitation.”
Best Paper Proceedings of the
Academy of Management Annual Meeting.
Rudolph, Jenny W. and Daniel B. Raemer. 2004. “Diagnostic
problem solving during simulated crises in
the OR. Anesthesia and Analgesia 98:S34.
Santos, Gabriel and Aguirre, Benigno E. 2005. “Critical Review
of Emergency Evacuation Simulation
Models.” Pp. 27-52 in Workshop on Building Occupant
Movement During Fire Emergencies,
edited by R. D. Peacock and E. D. Kuligowski. Gaithersburg,
MD: National Institute of Standards
and Technology.
Simon, H. A. 1956. “Rational Choice and the Structure of
Environments.” Psych. Rev. 63:129-138.
Slovic, Paul, Baruch Fischhoff and Sarah Lichtenstein. 1977.
“Behavioral Decision Theory.” Annual
Review of Psychology 28:1-39.
Slovic, Paul, Howard Kunreuther and Gilbert F. White. 1974.
“Decision Processes, Rationality, and
Adjustments to Natural Hazards.” Pp. 187-205 in Natural
Hazards, edited by Gilbert F. White.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Smelser, Neil J. 1962. Theory of Collective Behavior. New
York, NY: The Free Press.
Spearpoint, M. 2004. “Effect of Pre-evacuation Distributions on
Evacuation Times in the Simulex Model.”
Journal of Fire Protection Engineering 14:33-54.
20
21
Starbuck, W. H. and F. J. Milliken. 1988. “Executives’
perceptual filters: What they notice and how they
make sense. Pp. 35-65 in The executive effect: Concepts and
methods for studying top managers,
edited by D. C. Hambrick. Greenwich, CT: JAI.
Thompson, Catherine M., John A. Forester, Susan E. Cooper,
Dennis C. Bley, and John Wreathall. 1997.
Pp. 9-13-9-17 in Proceedings of the IEEE Sixth Annual Human
Factors Meeting.
Thompson, Peter A., J. Wu, and E. W. Marchant. 1996.
“Modelling Evacuation in Multi-storey Buildings
with Simulex.” Fire Engineering 56:7-11.
Tong, D. and David Canter. 1985. “The Decision to Evacuate: A
Study of the Motivations which
Contribute to Evacuation in the Event of a Fire.” Fire Safety
Journal 9:257-265.
Turner, Ralph H. and Lewis M. Killian. 1987. Collective
Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall,
Inc.
Turner, Ralph H. and Lewis M. Killian. 1957. Collective
Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall,
Inc.
Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. 1982. “Judgment under
Uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.” Pp. 3-
20 in Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases,
edited by D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, and
A. Tversky. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Vaughan, Diane. 1999. “The Dark Side of Organizations:
Mistake, Misconduct, and Disaster.” Annual
Review of Sociology 25:271-305.
Weick, Karl E. 1995. Sensemaking in Organizations. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Weick, Karl E. 1993. “The Collapse of Sensemaking in
Organizations: The Mann Gulch Disaster.”
Administrative Science Quarterly 38: 628-652.
Wiegman, Oene, Egli Komilis, Bernard Cadet, Henk Boer, and
Jan M. Gutteling. 1992. “The Response of
Local Residents to a Chemical Hazard Warning: Prediction of
Behavioral Intentions in Greece,
France and the Netherlands.” International Journal of Mass
Emergencies and Disasters 10:499-
515.
USFA. 2007.
http://www.usfa.dhs.gov/statistics/national/index.shtm
Zakay, Dan. 1993. “The Impact of Time Perception Processes
on Decision Making Under Time Stress.”
Pp. 59-72 in Time Pressure and Stress in Human Judgment and
Decision Making, edited by Ola
Svenson and A. John Maule. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
http://www.usfa.dhs.gov/statistics/national/index.shtm
FIR 4306, Human Behavior in Fire 1
Course Learning Outcomes for Unit VII
Upon completion of this unit, students should be able to:
1. Explain how building evacuation models work.
2. Explain the use of building evacuation models.
3. Explain how building evacuation models can be improved.
4. Discuss the theory of occupant behavior during building
fires.
Reading Assignment
Kuligowski, E.D., (2008). Modeling human behavior during
building fires (NIST Technical Note 1619).
Washington DC: United States of America Department of
Commerce. Retrieved from
http://fire.nist.gov/bfrlpubs/fire09/art018.html
In order to access the resources below, you must first log into
the myCSU Student Portal and access the
ABI/Inform Complete database within the CSU Online Library.
Kuligowski, E. (2013, 01). Predicting human behavior during
fires. Fire Technology, 49, 101-120.
Wales, D., & Thompson, O. F. (2013). Human behaviour in fire:
Should the fire service stop telling and start
listening? International Journal of Emergency Services, 2(2),
94-103.
Unit Lesson
Have you ever been in a public building like a hotel or office
building when a fire alarm goes off? What is your
first response? Do you immediately head to the exit, or do you
stop and gather your things? Maybe you
hesitate or talk to others to see if the alarm is really a drill or a
serious situation. What about the evacuation
maps located in hotels and public buildings? When you check
into a hotel, do you take the time to locate the
emergency exits? Do you calculate how long it would take you
and your family to evacuate a hotel if a fire
were to break out? Emergency personnel, researchers, and other
professionals use evacuation models to
determine escape routes in an emergency, but do they take all
the factors necessary into account for an
accurate prediction of evacuation procedures? This unit looks at
how evacuation models work, how they are
used, and how evacuation models can be improved by taking
occupant behavior into account during fires.
Evacuation models can be used to evaluate a level of safety
provided by buildings during evacuations. Most
evacuation models focus on calculating and predicting
evacuation movements. The calculations can be
conducted through engineering programs and in some case
through hand calculation. The problem in a lot of
situations is that these models typically ignore the predictions
of behaviors of occupants or make wrong
assumptions about occupant behavior. A solution to this
challenge is to generate a robust, comprehensive
and validated theory on human behavior during evacuation of
building fires. Research on fire injuries and
deaths shows that two-thirds of injured victims and fatalities
could have been evacuated (Kuligowski, 2008).
In what way do building evacuation models work? Evacuation
models quantify evacuation performance by
calculating how long it takes for occupants to evacuate a
building (Kuligowski, 2008). The model attempts to
simulate two things, the actions occupants participate in and
how long it takes to perform these actions. For
example, evacuation models can offer floor clearing times and
locate congestion points showing how people
may converge together and form cluster in halls or stairways.
Along with specific evacuation movements,
occupants may partake in a variety of other activities during
evacuation that can hinder arrival to a safe
location. Occupants may take the time to gather personal items,
assist fellow occupants, or help put out a fire.
UNIT VII STUDY GUIDE
Modeling Human Behavior During
Building Fires
FIR 4306, Human Behavior in Fire 2
There are typically two methods used to account for occupant
behavior during building evacuation. One
method is for the user to assign a time period of delay or
waiting to individuals or groups. Another method is
for the user to assign a specific behavior itinerary of actions or
specific action to an individual or group
(Kuligowski, 2008). There are problems with the approaches
that models use to simulate the behaviors during
evacuation. First, no behavior is actually predicted by the
models. Behavioral information is provided based
on prior, pre-programmed assumptions. Behavior is determined
by the user or the model based on prescribed
information and in turn, can have a design or user bias built in
(Kuligowski, 2008).
Experimental projects rely on evaluation, especially in
situations where real-time data is not available or
conditions cannot easily be investigated. Evacuation models can
also be used in incident recreation projects.
The models in these situations help investigators determine why
a situation happened, why a fire resulted in
so many fatalities, or why there was a problem with the
evacuation method. It may also answer questions
related to the specific incidents. Model users are likely to be
fire investigators, researchers, engineers, and/or
consultants (Kuligowski, 2008).
It is important that a theory takes into account the range of
behaviors of those involved in a building fire.
Occupants respond in a variety of ways based on the different
cues presented to them. Even when occupants
are presented with the same cues, they are likely to act
differently (Kuligowski, 2008). This is because
humans are individuals and will react differently based on their
past experience, what they perceive is
happening around them in relationship to the fire, and how they
view the risks involved during the situation. All
of these issues help determine the ultimate decisions that will
be made as to the next steps to take during the
fire situation. A conceptual model of behavior process for
building fires has four phases. These are:
perception of the cues, interpretation of the cues, situation, and
risk, decision-making, and actions.
Engineers, fire fighters, and even psychologists and sociologists
need to work together to create authentic
and true-to-life evacuation models. These accurate models
should allow emergency personnel to make more
informed decisions during a rescue attempt, and provide
occupants with a higher chance to reach safety
during a building fire.
Reference
Kuligowski, E. D., (2008). Modeling human behavior during
building fires (NIST Technical Note 1619).
Washington DC: United States of America Department of
Commerce.
Learning Activities (Non-Graded)
If you chose to create a blog in Unit III, take the time now to
update it one last time. Discuss the topics in this
course that you believe will help you in your professional
career. Describe a behavior change in yourself that
may have occurred based on what you have learned during this
course. If you choose, email your instructor,
reminding them of your blog site address and let them know you
have updated your blog.
Non-graded Learning Activities are provided to aid students in
their course of study. You do not have to
submit them. If you have questions, contact your instructor for
further guidance and information.
Assignment 3 Capstone Research ProjectDue Week 10 and worth 440.docx

More Related Content

DOCX
Acc 499 assignment 3 capstone research project
DOCX
Acc 499 assignment 3 capstone research project
PDF
499 week 10 assignment 3 capstone research project
DOCX
ACC 418 Exceptional Education / snaptutorial.com
DOC
Acc 418 Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
DOC
Acc 418 Education Specialist-snaptutorial.com
DOC
Acc 418 Education Redefined - snaptutorial.com
DOC
Acc 418 Education Organization / snaptutorial.com
Acc 499 assignment 3 capstone research project
Acc 499 assignment 3 capstone research project
499 week 10 assignment 3 capstone research project
ACC 418 Exceptional Education / snaptutorial.com
Acc 418 Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
Acc 418 Education Specialist-snaptutorial.com
Acc 418 Education Redefined - snaptutorial.com
Acc 418 Education Organization / snaptutorial.com

Similar to Assignment 3 Capstone Research ProjectDue Week 10 and worth 440.docx (17)

DOCX
The tasks You are assumed to be one of the software consultants .docx
PDF
Financial Reporting Financial Statement Analysis and Valuation 9th Edition Wa...
DOCX
Acc 418 Extraordinary Success/newtonhelp.com
PDF
Solution Manual for Information Technology Auditing 3rd Edition by Hall
PDF
Intermediate Accounting Kieso 13th Edition Solutions Manual
PDF
Accounting Solution Book.pdf
DOCX
Assume you are the partner in an accounting firm hired to perform th.docx
PDF
Financial Reporting Financial Statement Analysis and Valuation 9th Edition Wa...
DOC
Argosy university acc 418 module 4 assignment 2 constructive dividends new
PDF
Intermediate Accounting Kieso 13th Edition Solutions Manual
DOCX
Assignment 3 Capstone Research ProjectDue Week 10 and worth.docx
RTF
DOCX
Chapter 17 lin Yates for undergrads; Chapters 15 - 19 in Batholom.docx
PDF
Instant Download for Intermediate Accounting Kieso 13th Edition Solutions Man...
PDF
ACC 418 Expect Success/newtonhelp.com
PDF
Solution Manual for Managerial ACCT2 2nd Edition by Sawyers
DOCX
Acc 418 Entire Course NEW
The tasks You are assumed to be one of the software consultants .docx
Financial Reporting Financial Statement Analysis and Valuation 9th Edition Wa...
Acc 418 Extraordinary Success/newtonhelp.com
Solution Manual for Information Technology Auditing 3rd Edition by Hall
Intermediate Accounting Kieso 13th Edition Solutions Manual
Accounting Solution Book.pdf
Assume you are the partner in an accounting firm hired to perform th.docx
Financial Reporting Financial Statement Analysis and Valuation 9th Edition Wa...
Argosy university acc 418 module 4 assignment 2 constructive dividends new
Intermediate Accounting Kieso 13th Edition Solutions Manual
Assignment 3 Capstone Research ProjectDue Week 10 and worth.docx
Chapter 17 lin Yates for undergrads; Chapters 15 - 19 in Batholom.docx
Instant Download for Intermediate Accounting Kieso 13th Edition Solutions Man...
ACC 418 Expect Success/newtonhelp.com
Solution Manual for Managerial ACCT2 2nd Edition by Sawyers
Acc 418 Entire Course NEW
Ad

More from rock73 (20)

DOCX
In a two- to three-page paper (excluding the title and reference pag.docx
DOCX
In a substantial paragraph respond to either one of the following qu.docx
DOCX
In a study by Dr. Sandra Levitsky, she considers why the economic,.docx
DOCX
In a response of at least two paragraphs, provide an explanation o.docx
DOCX
in a minimum of 1000 words, describe why baseball is Americas past .docx
DOCX
In a minimum 200 word response, describe some ways how the public .docx
DOCX
In a weekly coordination meeting, several senior investigators from .docx
DOCX
In a memo, describe 1) the form and style of art as well as 2) the e.docx
DOCX
In a minimum 200 word response explain the problems that law enforce.docx
DOCX
In a minimum 200 word response explain some of the reasons why, in.docx
DOCX
In a maximum of 750 words, you are required to1. Summarize the ar.docx
DOCX
in a two- to- three page paper (not including the title and referenc.docx
DOCX
In a two- to three-page paper (not including the title and reference.docx
DOCX
In a group, take a look at the two student essays included in this f.docx
DOCX
BASEBALLRuns Scored (X)Wins (Y)70869875906547970480787957307166786.docx
DOCX
Based on Santa Clara University Ethics DialogueEthics .docx
DOCX
Barbara Corcoran Learns Her Heart’s True Desires In her.docx
DOCX
Bapsi Sidhwa’s Cracking India1947 PartitionDeepa Meh.docx
DOCX
Barriers of therapeutic relationshipThe therapeutic relations.docx
DOCX
Barada 2Mohamad BaradaProfessor Andrew DurdinReligions of .docx
In a two- to three-page paper (excluding the title and reference pag.docx
In a substantial paragraph respond to either one of the following qu.docx
In a study by Dr. Sandra Levitsky, she considers why the economic,.docx
In a response of at least two paragraphs, provide an explanation o.docx
in a minimum of 1000 words, describe why baseball is Americas past .docx
In a minimum 200 word response, describe some ways how the public .docx
In a weekly coordination meeting, several senior investigators from .docx
In a memo, describe 1) the form and style of art as well as 2) the e.docx
In a minimum 200 word response explain the problems that law enforce.docx
In a minimum 200 word response explain some of the reasons why, in.docx
In a maximum of 750 words, you are required to1. Summarize the ar.docx
in a two- to- three page paper (not including the title and referenc.docx
In a two- to three-page paper (not including the title and reference.docx
In a group, take a look at the two student essays included in this f.docx
BASEBALLRuns Scored (X)Wins (Y)70869875906547970480787957307166786.docx
Based on Santa Clara University Ethics DialogueEthics .docx
Barbara Corcoran Learns Her Heart’s True Desires In her.docx
Bapsi Sidhwa’s Cracking India1947 PartitionDeepa Meh.docx
Barriers of therapeutic relationshipThe therapeutic relations.docx
Barada 2Mohamad BaradaProfessor Andrew DurdinReligions of .docx
Ad

Recently uploaded (20)

PDF
احياء السادس العلمي - الفصل الثالث (التكاثر) منهج متميزين/كلية بغداد/موهوبين
PDF
Practical Manual AGRO-233 Principles and Practices of Natural Farming
PDF
ChatGPT for Dummies - Pam Baker Ccesa007.pdf
PDF
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
PDF
Computing-Curriculum for Schools in Ghana
PPTX
A powerpoint presentation on the Revised K-10 Science Shaping Paper
PPTX
TNA_Presentation-1-Final(SAVE)) (1).pptx
PDF
What if we spent less time fighting change, and more time building what’s rig...
PDF
1_English_Language_Set_2.pdf probationary
PDF
Indian roads congress 037 - 2012 Flexible pavement
PDF
MBA _Common_ 2nd year Syllabus _2021-22_.pdf
PPTX
Chinmaya Tiranga Azadi Quiz (Class 7-8 )
PDF
Τίμαιος είναι φιλοσοφικός διάλογος του Πλάτωνα
PDF
medical_surgical_nursing_10th_edition_ignatavicius_TEST_BANK_pdf.pdf
PDF
1.3 FINAL REVISED K-10 PE and Health CG 2023 Grades 4-10 (1).pdf
PPTX
Onco Emergencies - Spinal cord compression Superior vena cava syndrome Febr...
PPTX
20th Century Theater, Methods, History.pptx
PDF
Chinmaya Tiranga quiz Grand Finale.pdf
PDF
AI-driven educational solutions for real-life interventions in the Philippine...
PPTX
Introduction to pro and eukaryotes and differences.pptx
احياء السادس العلمي - الفصل الثالث (التكاثر) منهج متميزين/كلية بغداد/موهوبين
Practical Manual AGRO-233 Principles and Practices of Natural Farming
ChatGPT for Dummies - Pam Baker Ccesa007.pdf
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
Computing-Curriculum for Schools in Ghana
A powerpoint presentation on the Revised K-10 Science Shaping Paper
TNA_Presentation-1-Final(SAVE)) (1).pptx
What if we spent less time fighting change, and more time building what’s rig...
1_English_Language_Set_2.pdf probationary
Indian roads congress 037 - 2012 Flexible pavement
MBA _Common_ 2nd year Syllabus _2021-22_.pdf
Chinmaya Tiranga Azadi Quiz (Class 7-8 )
Τίμαιος είναι φιλοσοφικός διάλογος του Πλάτωνα
medical_surgical_nursing_10th_edition_ignatavicius_TEST_BANK_pdf.pdf
1.3 FINAL REVISED K-10 PE and Health CG 2023 Grades 4-10 (1).pdf
Onco Emergencies - Spinal cord compression Superior vena cava syndrome Febr...
20th Century Theater, Methods, History.pptx
Chinmaya Tiranga quiz Grand Finale.pdf
AI-driven educational solutions for real-life interventions in the Philippine...
Introduction to pro and eukaryotes and differences.pptx

Assignment 3 Capstone Research ProjectDue Week 10 and worth 440.docx

  • 1. Assignment 3: Capstone Research Project Due Week 10 and worth 440 points Assume you are the partner in an accounting firm hired to perform the audit on a fortune 1000 company. Assume also that the initial public offering (IPO) of the company was approximately five (5) years ago and the company is concerned that, in less than five (5) years after the IPO, a restatement may be necessary. During your initial evaluation of the client, you discover the following information: · The client is currently undergoing a three (3) year income tax examination by the Internal Revenue Service (IRS). A significant issue involved in the IRS audit encompasses inventory write-downs on the tax returns that are not included in the financial statements. Because of the concealment of the transaction, the IRS is labeling the treatment of the write-down as fraud. · The company has a share-based compensation plan for top- level executives consisting of stock options. The value of the options exercised during the year was not expensed or disclosed in the financial statements. · The company has several operating and capital leases in place, and the CFO is considering leasing a substantial portion of the assets for future use. The current leases in place are arranged using special purpose entities (SPEs) and operating leases. · The company seeks to acquire a global partner, which will require IFRS reporting. · The company received correspondence from the Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC) requesting additional supplemental information regarding the financial statements submitted with the IPO. Write an eight to ten (8-10) page paper in which you: 1. Evaluate any damaging financial and ethical repercussions of
  • 2. failure to include the inventory write-downs in the financial statements. Prepare a recommendation to the CFO, evaluating the negative impact of a civil fraud penalty on the corporation as a result of the IRS audit. In the recommendation, include essential internal control procedures to prevent fraudulent financial reporting from occurring, as well as the major obligation of the CEO and CFO to ensure compliance. 2. Examine the negative results on stakeholders and the financial statements of an IRS audit which generates additional tax and penalties or subsequent audits. Assume that the subsequent audit and / or additional tax and penalties result from the taxpayer’s use of an inventory reserve account, applying a 10 percent reduction to inventory over three (3) years. 3. Discuss the applicable federal tax laws, regulations, rulings, and court cases related to the inventory write-downs, and explain the specific relevance of each to the write-down. 4. Research the current generally accepted accounting principles (GAAP) regarding stock option accounting. Evaluate the current treatment of the company’s share-based compensation plan based on GAAP reporting. Contrast the financial benefits and risks of the share-based compensation stock option plan with the financial benefits and risks of a share-based stock- appreciation rights plan (SARS). Recommend to the CFO which plan the company should use, and provide the correct accounting treatment for each. 5. Research the reporting requirements for lease reporting under GAAP and International Financial Reporting Standards (IFRS). Based on your research, create a proposal for future lease transactions to the CFO. Within the proposal, discuss the use of off-the-balance sheet financing arrangements, capital leases, and operating leases, and indicate the related business and financial risks of each. 6. Create an argument for or against a single set of international accounting standards related to lease accounting based on the global market and cross border leases of assets. Examine the
  • 3. benefits and risks of your chosen position. 7. Examine the major implications of SAS 99 based on the factors you discovered during the initial evaluation of the company. Provide support for your rationale. 8. Analyze the potential for a material misstatement in the financial statements based on the issues identified in your initial evaluation. Make a recommendation to the CFO for the issuance of restated financial statement restatement. Identify at least three (3) significant issues that can result from the failure to issue restated financial statements. 9. Examine the economic effect of restatement of the financial statements on investors, employees, customers, and creditors. 10. Use five (5) quality academic resources in this assignment. Note: Wikipedia and other Websites do not qualify as academic resources. Your assignment must follow these formatting requirements: · Be typed, double spaced, using Times New Roman font (size 12), with one-inch margins on all sides; citations and references must follow APA or school-specific format. Check with your professor for any additional instructions. · Include a cover page containing the title of the assignment, the student’s name, the professor’s name, the course title, and the date. The cover page and the reference page are not included in the required assignment page length. The specific course learning outcomes associated with this assignment are: · Analyze accounting situations to apply the proper accounting rules and make recommendations to ensure compliance with generally accepted accounting principles. · Analyze business situations to determine the appropriateness of decision making in terms of professional standards and ethics · Analyze business situations and apply advanced federal taxation concepts. · Use technology and information resources to research issues in
  • 4. accounting. · Write clearly and concisely about accounting using proper writing mechanics. Grading for this assignment will be based on answer quality, logic / organization of the paper, and language and writing skills, using the following rubric. Click here to view the grading rubric. NIST Technical Note 1619 Modeling Human Behavior during Building Fires Erica D. Kuligowski NIST Technical Note 1619
  • 5. Modeling Human Behavior during Building Fires Erica D. Kuligowski Fire Research Division Building and Fire Research Laboratory December 2008 U.S. Department of Commerce Carlos M. Gutierrez, Secretary National Institute of Standards and Technology James M. Turner, Deputy Director Certain commercial entities, equipment, or materials may be identified in this document in order to describe an experimental procedure or concept adequately. Such identification is not intended to imply recommendation or endorsement by the National Institute of Standards and Technology, nor is it
  • 6. intended to imply that the entities, materials, or equipment are necessarily the best available for the purpose. National Institute of Standards and Technology Technical Note 1619 Natl. Inst. Stand. Technol. Tech. Note 1619, 21 pages (December 2008) CODEN: NSPUE2 Abstract Evacuation models, including engineering hand calculations and computational tools, are used to evaluate the level of safety provided by buildings during evacuation. Building designs and occupant procedures are based on the results produced from these models, including evacuation time results (i.e., how long building occupants will take to evacuate a building). However, most evacuation models focus primarily on calculating and predicting evacuation movement (i.e., how long will it take an occupant to move from his/her initial position to safety), almost ignoring the prediction of behaviors that occupants perform before and during evacuation movement that can delay their safety (e.g., searching for information, fighting the fire, and helping others). Instead of modeling and predicting behavior of simulated occupants,
  • 7. evacuation models and users often make assumptions and simplifications about occupant behavior (i.e., what people do during evacuations) that can be unrealistic and are likely to produce inaccurate results. A solution to this problem is to generate a robust, comprehensive, and validated theory on human behavior during evacuation from building fires. The social scientific literature can be gleaned to develop these theories, which can then be incorporated into the current evacuation models to accurately simulate occupant behavior during fire evacuations. These models can then achieve more realistic results which will lead to safer, more efficient building design. The purpose of this paper is to reevaluate our current egress modeling techniques and advocate for the inclusion of a comprehensive conceptual model of occupant behavior during building fires. The paper begins by describing the current state of evacuation modeling of human behavior in fires and identifying gaps in current behavioral techniques. The second part of the paper outlines a general process model for occupant response to physical and social cues in a building fire event. Keywords fire, evacuation, egress, evacuation models, building fires
  • 8. Acknowledgements Thanks to Kathleen Tierney, Liam Downey, William Grosshandler, Dennis Mileti, and Ross Corotis for their efforts. My appreciation to Richard Peacock, Jason Averill, Anthony Hamins, and Steve Gwynne for providing detailed and insightful suggestions. Modeling Human Behavior during Building Fires Introduction In 2006, structure fires injured over 14 000 people and killed over 2700 people in the United States (USFA 2007). In many fires, there may be occupants who are not able to self-evacuate, such as disabled or intoxicated occupants. However, research on fire injuries and deaths shows that over two-thirds of the injured and over half of the dead in building fires could have evacuated; these people were performing activities that delayed their safety, including fighting the fire, attempting to rescue others in the building, and moving to safety under untenable conditions inside the building (Hall 2004). Evacuation models, including engineering hand calculations (Nelson and Mowrer 2002) and computational tools, can be used to evaluate the level of safety provided by buildings during evacuation. Building designs and occupant procedures are based
  • 9. on the results produced from these models, primarily evacuation time results (i.e., how long building occupants require to evacuate a building). However, most evacuation models focus primarily on calculating and predicting evacuation movement (i.e., how long will it take an occupant to move from his/her initial position to safety), almost ignoring the prediction of behaviors that occupants perform before and during evacuation movement that can delay their exit. Instead of modeling and predicting behavior of simulated occupants, evacuation models and users often make assumptions and simplifications about occupant behavior that can be unrealistic and can produce inaccurate results. For example, some evacuation models allow for the user to assume that building occupants immediately begin to move to the stairs or exits upon hearing a fire alarm or sensing a fire. This assumption and others like it can represent a scenario that is unlikely to occur in an actual fire event and thereby inappropriately influence the evacuation time calculated by the model. In cases in which assumptions lead to an unrealistic underestimation of evacuation results (i.e., shorter evacuation times), buildings or procedures are designed according to an inadequate life safety design; e.g., insufficient egress routes and/or unsafe procedures for staff and/or occupants. In cases in which assumptions can lead to an unrealistic overestimation of evacuation results (i.e., longer evacuation times), buildings or procedures are designed based on an overestimation of egress needs, which can raise the cost of
  • 10. buildings unnecessarily. A solution to this problem is to generate a robust, comprehensive, and validated theory on human behavior during evacuation from building fires. The social scientific literature and case studies from disasters and building fires could be employed to develop this theory, which could then be incorporated into the current evacuation models to more accurately simulate occupant behavior during fire evacuations. These models would produce more accurate results and benefit building design. Until a comprehensive theory on occupant behavior is developed for inclusion into evacuation models, costly or ineffective egress or procedural designs may be developed based on the unquantified needs of the evacuating population. Scope The purpose of this paper is to reevaluate our current egress modeling techniques and advocate for the inclusion of a robust, comprehensive, and validated conceptual model of occupant behavior during building fires. This paper begins by describing the current state of evacuation modeling of human behavior in fires and identifying gaps in current behavioral prediction 6
  • 11. techniques. The second part of the paper outlines a model that can predict occupant behavior in response to the interpretations and decisions made regarding physical and social cues in a building fire. Although similar work has identified the lack of behavioral simulation in evacuation models (Santos and Aguirre 2005), no work has been completed to systematically examine research from the evacuation of both buildings and communities to develop behavioral theory for building evacuations. Building Evacuation Models How do building evacuation models work? Evacuation models quantify evacuation performance by calculating how long it takes for occupants to evacuate a building. In order to make this calculation, the model attempts to simulate two things: 1) the actions that people take and 2) how long it takes to perform each action. In addition to total building evacuation times, evacuation models can provide floor clearing times and the location of the congestion points throughout the building. However, due to the lack of data and theory on occupant behavior/actions, evacuation models significantly simplify the evacuation process and many focus primarily on how long it takes to perform one kind of action: the movement of occupants from their initial positions to the outside of the building. In other words, the current evacuation models primarily focus on the purposive evacuation movement of the occupants and do not simulate
  • 12. additional behaviors that may delay evacuation to safety*. In addition to purposive evacuation movement, occupants are likely to engage in a variety of other activities throughout their evacuation from the building that can delay their movement to safety. Such activities can include information gathering, preparing for the evacuation by gathering their personal belongings, assisting or even rescuing others, alerting others in the building, changing stairs, and fighting the fire. These actions can take place during either period of a building evacuation, either during the pre-evacuation period or the evacuation period. The pre-evacuation period is labeled as the period from the point when the occupant is notified that there is something wrong until s/he begins to travel an evacuation route out of the building. The evacuation period then ends when the occupant has reached a point of safety or outside of the building. In the models that can account for occupant actions†, there are two main methods used to simulate occupant behavior during a building evacuation. One method is for the user to assig time period of delay/waiting (e.g., a specific period of time, a distribution of times, etc.) to individuals or groups in the simulated building to account for any actions that they might perform during the evacuation (e.g., Simulex n a
  • 13. ‡ (Thompson, Wu and Marchant 1996; Spearpoint 2004), EXIT89 (Fahy 2000; 1996), GridFlow (Bensilum and Purser 2002)). Using this method, * Model exceptions to this include buildingEXODUS (Filippidis et al. 2006; Gwynne et al. 1999a) and CRISP (Fraser-Mitchell 1999). These models begin to address behaviors performed by occupants during building fires. † There are a number of models that do not simulate occupant behavior. These are labeled as movement models (Kuligowski and Peacock 2005). ‡ Certain commercial entities, equipment, or materials may be identified in this document in order to describe an experimental procedure or concept adequately. Such identification is not intended to imply recommendation or endorsement by the National Institute of Standards and Technology, nor is it intended to imply that the entities, materials, or equipment are necessarily the best available for the purpose. 7 simulated occupants remain stationary in their initial position for a set period of time and th begin purposive evacuation movement once this time period is over. The other method is for the user to assign a specific behavioral itinerary (i.e., a sequence of actions) or a specific action to a individual or group (e.g., CRISP (Fraser-Mitchell 1999), buildingEXODUS (Filippidis et al.
  • 14. 2006; Gwynne et al. 1999a)). Action sequences can be used to simulate behaviors that may interrupt continuous movement, such as searching for information, leaving the stairs, assisting other occupants, and returning to initial locations to retrieve personal or work items. Each action performed is assigned a specific time for each occupant. An example of a behavioral itine the following: Occupant A is assigned a “search and rescue” behavioral itinerary. To perform t search and rescue mission, the model simulates that the occupant moves from Point A (Occ A’s original position) to Point B (another room in the building where the rescue takes place), waits for an assigned period of time at Point B, and then begins purposive evacuation movem from Point B to the st en n rary is he upant ent airs. Both of these methods of simulating behavior during evacuation significantly simplify the behavioral processes that take place during the evacuation. In the first method, assigning a time delay, an emphasis is placed on the time delay itself rather than
  • 15. the decisions, actions, and interactions made by the occupants in response to conditions inside the building. The second method, assigning a behavioral itinerary, begins to simulate decisions and actions made in response to certain conditions during the evacuation, however, the entire behavior or behavioral itinerary is defined by the user before the simulation begins (rather than predicted by the model) and interactions among other simulated occupants is simplified or nonexistent. There are problems with the approaches used by models to simulate behavior during evacuation. First, no behavior is actually predicted by the evacuation models because behavioral information is provided as prior, pre-programmed assumptions. Behavior is determined by the user or probabilistically by the model based on prescribed information. More importantly, the user prescribes the actions that will occur, or that s/he assumes may occur, in each fire scenario. There is no consistency associated with the prescription of behaviors; this method relies entirely on the user’s expertise in understanding occupant behavior in building fires. This is an unrealistic expectation of the user since there is no guidance, comprehensive data set, or theory provided to users about what people actually do during building evacuations. What are building evacuation models used for? Currently, there are over 40 different evacuation models (Kuligowski and Peacock 2005; Gwynne
  • 16. et al. 1999b) available for use in three main types of projects. These projects are safety assessment evaluations (SAE), experimental work, and incident re-creation (Gwynne 2000). Each project type is unique to the purpose of the project, the use of the evacuation model, and the approval process used to evaluate the accuracy of the results, all of which are described below. During SAE projects, an evacuation model is used to assess the safety of a particular building design and/or egress procedure for the occupants in a building. For these types of projects, the model user is most likely an engineer or a life safety consultant evaluating a new building design or a design from an existing building undergoing a change, such as a use or physical layout. For SAE projects, the user typically runs various evacuation simulations for the building (e.g., occupants travel via different building routes, occupants move at various speeds, etc.) and compares the evacuation simulation results with results from fire modeling simulations. A building is deemed to provide a sufficient level of life safety for occupants if the amount of time needed for evacuation of the building (evacuation modeling results) is less than the time when conditions become untenable for occupants inside the building (fire modeling results). As a final 8 step in a SAE project, an authority having jurisdiction must
  • 17. approve the safety analysis made by the engineer, which is sometimes completed through a third- party review process. Evacuation models are also used in experimental projects. For these projects, the evacuation model is used to explore and investigate conditions that cannot be easily examined otherwise. The model user is often a consultant/engineer evaluating a variety of different designs for the same building or researchers and academics testing hypotheses on the impact of building conditions on results. For experimental projects, the user produces a variety of egress results from the same model from various input conditions. For example, the user can simulate a variety of different egress scenarios using the same model to test different aspects of the building design, such as the size of the stairwell(s), the number of stairs in the building, the width of the corridors, the width, location, and number of exits, etc. In this example, the user may be interested in which designs provide sufficiently fast evacuation times for building occupants. Other examples include the testing of hypotheses related to the impact of fire conditions on people movement through the building. Evacuation models can also be used in incident re-creation projects. The purpose of these projects is often to determine the cause(s) of actual incident outcomes (e.g., why so many people perished in a particular fire) and/or answer particular questions about the incident itself (e.g., what would
  • 18. have happened if the building was more densely populated during the fire event, if the building had more exits, etc?). Model users are likely to be fire investigators, researchers, engineers, consultants and others charged with answering questions about an actual event. In order to determine causes behind actual incident results, users will attempt to model the actual incident, which may include a series of modeling runs, as close to the actual conditions as possible by using all known conditions from the event. If additional questions are asked (e.g., what-if questions), the user can develop a base-line case and then alter specific conditions in the building to answer the appropriate questions (e.g., adding more people to the building and rerunning the simulation). Evacuation models were used in the investigations of the collapse of the World Trade Center Towers in 2001 (Galea et al. 2008; Averill et al. 2005) and the Rhode Island Nightclub fire (Galea et al. 2008; Grosshandler et al. 2005) to answer specific questions. A comprehensive theory of human behavior in fire can improve building evacuation models for all three types of projects. However, the generation of a comprehensive theory is most important for SAE and experimental projects. Whereas the user attempts to model behaviors that are already known in incident re-creation project, the user relies on the model for accuracy in simulating an event that has not yet occurred for SAE and experimental projects. Data and theory on human behavior during evacuations is necessary for accurate evacuation modeling results.
  • 19. What is needed to improve building evacuation models? A comprehensive theory of occupant behavior in evacuations from building fires is needed to improve the current building evacuation models. The theory should be able to predict individual behavior and group dynamics that are likely to occur in a building fire, rather than relying on ad- hoc user-prescription. This would take the burden away from the user to prescribe actions, which can lead to inconsistency and inaccuracy, and actually allow the model to predict behaviors that emerge from situational conditions. More specifically, a theory should simulate the variety of behaviors performed by occupants in a building fire (e.g., seek information, warn, rescue, and prepare). In mass crowd events, where occupants are densely located in the same area and receive cues together as a group, occupants are likely to respond in similar ways to the cues presented (Purser and Gwynne 2007; Santos and 9 Aguirre 2005). In these types of events, most occupants are likely to be influenced by the group throughout the entire evacuation. However, in most building fires, occupants are located in different places throughout the building, many times receiving different instructions or cues from the event. Occupants respond in a variety of ways based on the
  • 20. different cues that are presented to them; and even occupants presented with the same cues are likely to act in different ways (Mileti and Sorensen 1990). This is because occupants’ actions vary based on the cues that they perceive, their interpretations of the event and risk, and the decisions that they make about next steps. With this in mind, it is crucial to develop a theory of occupant behavior in building fires based on social/behavior processes. Theory of Occupant Behavior during Building Fires Social scientific theory has acknowledged for over 70 years (Mead 1938) that human action or response is the result of a process. Instead of actions based on random chance or even actions resulting directly from a change in the environment, an individual’s actions are frequently the result of a decision-making process. Research in disasters, based on social scientific theory, has led to the development of social-psychological process models for public warning response (Mileti and Sorensen 1990; Perry, Lindell and Greene 1981; Mileti and Beck 1975). These models specify that people go through a process of specific phases, including hearing, understanding, believing, and personalizing the warning, in which they consider aspects of their response before performing an act (Mileti and Sorensen 1990). Additionally, researchers of fire evacuations (Bryan 2002; Feinberg and Johnson 1995; Tong and Canter 1985; Edelman, Herz and Bickman 1980; Breaux, Canter and Sime 1976) have shown that
  • 21. a process involving the phases of recognition and interpretation of the environment influence occupant actions. In these process models, there are specific cue- and occupant-related factors that influence the outcome of each phase of the process (e.g., whether the person hears the warning or interprets the situation correctly). Cue-related factors are described later in this paper and occupant-related factors include demographics (e.g., gender, age, income, education, race, and marital status), previous experiences, and knowledge. An understanding of the behavioral process and the influential factors of each phase can be developed into a conceptual model to predict the types of individual behaviors that are likely to occur in building fires. The behavioral process for the pre-evacuation or evacuation period of building fires is shown in Figure 1. This process suggests that an occupant’s actions are a result of his/her perceptions, interpretations and decisions made based upon the external and internal (occupant-based) cues presented in the fire situation. During a building fire, occupants or groups will begin a behavioral process only when presented with event-related information that interrupts their daily routine. A new behavioral process begins each time an occupant/group receives new information relating to the fire event, and a specific action is likely to occur based on whether the information is perceived, the interpretations of the cue, the situation, and the risk are developed, and the decisions are made on what to do next.
  • 22. Disaster and fire theory (Mileti and Sorensen 1990; Perry, Lindell and Greene 1981; Edelman, Herz and Bickman 1980; Breaux, Canter and Sime 1976; Mileti and Beck 1975) suggests that individuals engage in a sequence of phases during each behavioral process. In other words, in building fires, interpretation of the cue is possible only if the cue is perceived; an accurate interpretation of the situation is more likely if cues are interpreted accurately; the interpretation of the risk to themselves and others is more likely if the situation is interpreted accurately; and the occupants are more likely to decide on a certain type of action if they perceive cues and formulate 10 accurate interpretations of the event and risk. Each phase will be described in further detail in the following section. Phase 3: Decision-making Phase 2: Interpretation of the cue, situation, and risk Cue- and occupant- related factors Phase 4: Actions Phase 1: Perception
  • 23. of the cue(s) Figure 1: A conceptual model of the behavioral process for building fires Phase 1 of the behavioral process involves occupants perceiving or receiving external physical and social cues from their environment. In a building fire, occupants are constantly presented with external cues (Brennan 1999). These cues can be physical or social in nature, meaning that they arise from the physical environment or the social environment, respectively. Examples of physical cues in a building fire include flames, smoke, breaking glass, debris, tone alarms, and automatic warnings. Examples of social cues in a building fire include attempted communication from others inside or outside of the building, unofficial or authority-given warnings, and actions taken by the building population. These cues can be presented one by one or several at a time, depending upon the event. The perception phase involves an occupant receiving or noticing cues that makes him/her aware that something in his/her environment has changed (Weick 1995; Starbuck and Milliken 1988; Canter, Breaux and Sime 1980). Physical and social cues produced in a building fire can be perceived by occupants through hearing (e.g., an alarm or authority warning), smelling (e.g., smoke), seeing (e.g., others running), tasting (e.g., sulfur dioxide or hydrogen chloride), and/or touching (e.g., heat). In the interpretation phase, Phase 2, the occupant or group
  • 24. attempts to interpret the information provided by the cues perceived during the perception phase (Weick 1995; Canter, Donald and Chalk 1992; Turner and Killian 1987). Interpretation can be seen as the process of organizing perceived cues into a framework (Weick 1995); constructing a meaningful story based on an outcome (that is, the cue itself) (Weick 1993); and/or making sense of a situation by imagining what is going on (Rudolph and Morrison 2007; Klein 1999). Interpretation methods include the recall of previously developed behavioral scripts (or a sequence of expected behaviors based upon a situation) (Gioia and Poole 1984), mental simulation (Klein 1999), the use of mental models (Burns 2005), sensemaking (Rudolph and Raemer 2004; Weick 1995; Weick 1993), and collective behavior processes such as milling or intensified interaction (Dynes and Tierney 1994; 11 Marx and McAdam 1994; Goode 1992; McPhail 1991; Miller 1985; Berk 1974; Smelser 1962; Turner and Killian 1957). Occupants engage in such methods during fires and other emergencies, because these events create the need for interpretation by interrupting and disrupting normal interaction patterns and creating uncertainty. Behavioral scripts, mental simulation and modeling, and individual sensemaking are interpretation processes performed internally by an occupant to mentally formulate an interpretation. Occupants use behavioral scripts to interpret an event when
  • 25. the cues evoke memories of a previous situation in which a previous interpretation was formed (Gioia and Poole 1984). Mental simulation and modeling allows the occupant to develop cognitive images of what is going on in his/her environment based on the cues that s/he has received. Essentially, the occupant begins to paint a mental picture or story of the event based on the outcome (e.g., the cues). Group sensemaking and collective behavior involve interaction among occupants to collectively develop an understanding of the emergent situation. In new and/or ambiguous situations (Turner and Killian 1987) and times of urgency (Aguirre, Wenger and Vigo 1998), occupants are likely to interact with others around them. This type of interaction, which is intensified in densely populated buildings, has been documented in a variety of different incidents (e.g., Averill et al. 2005; Bryan 1983) as a means to establish what is going on, define the new situation at hand, and propose and adopt new appropriate norms for behavior (Aguirre, Wenger and Vigo 1998; Turner and Killian 1987). It is through individual and group-based reasoning strategies that occupants can begin to construct meaning from the cues that they perceive in fire situations. In these types of situations, leaders can emerge that suggest interpretations of the event, which can then be incorporated into the occupant’s interpretation. In fires and other extreme events, there are three main interpretation stages: interpreting the cues received, interpreting the situation (i.e. as a fire), and interpreting or defining the risk to the self
  • 26. and/or others. This process is shown in Figure 2. These three interpretation stages do not follow a linear, ordered pattern; instead, interpretive stages can overlap and inform one another in various ways. In fire events, however, it is likely that if occupants interpret a cue correctly (e.g., as a fire alarm, smoke, or an explosion), they are likely to interpret the situation correctly (e.g., as a fire situation), which in turn makes it more likely that they will interpret the situation as risky to themselves and/or to others (Wiegman et al. 1992; Perry and Greene 1983). Risk Situation Cue Interpretation Figure 2: The interpretation phase involves interpreting the cue, the situation, and the risk If the occupant recognizes the cue, defines the situation correctly, and understands the risk, s/he is likely to perform protective actions in order to begin the evacuation process. Initially, however, occupants are predisposed to interpret the situation as if nothing is wrong, known as normalcy bias (Okabe and Mikami 1982), and that they are not at risk. In a state of normalcy, inaction or waiting is likely to occur. The interpretation of the risk phase is essential to understand, because in order for people to act, they must interpret a situation as dangerous (Aguirre 2005).
  • 27. 12 Phase 3 of the behavioral process, decision-making, involves occupants or groups making decisions on what to do next based on their interpretations of the cues, situations, and risks. The decision-making phase is a two-step process in which occupants initially search for options of what to do and then choose one of the options (Gigerenzer and Selton 2001). The first step in the decision-making phase is to search for options of what to do based on interpretations of the event. Research literature suggests that occupants develop their options by performing mental simulation (Gwynne et al. 1999a; Thompson et al. 1997), similar to the methods of developing interpretations. Mental simulation (Klein 1999) allows an occupant to mentally structure scenarios on what s/he would do and how s/he would do it in the current situation. The search for options becomes the process of mentally developing scenarios of action before actually performing the act. The search for options of what to do can also occur collectively in groups (Turner and Killian 1987). In addition to interpreting an event, groups work together to plan a coordinated action that will solve the problem presented by the interpretation, if any. Suggestions for actions can come from any member of the group, although leaders are likely to emerge with suggestions of next actions (Connell 2001; Turner and Killian 1987). In the face of
  • 28. uncertainty and time pressure, people are likely to come together, share their interpretations, and define plans for collective action in an event. Occupants or groups are unlikely to search for a large number of options during the decision- making phase. Research suggests that individuals and groups are likely to develop a very small, even narrow range of decision options due to the following conditions: 1) perceived time pressure (Karau and Kelly 1992; Zakay 1993; Janis 1982; Ben-Zur and Breznitz 1981); 2) limited mental resources (Simon 1956; Gigerenzer and Selten 2001; Vaughan 1999); and/or 3) training and knowledge of procedures (Klein 1999; Thompson et al. 1997). Time pressure, likely in a fire event, causes occupants/groups to perceive a fewer number of cues, process the information less thoroughly and in turn, to consider a narrow set of options (Karau and Kelly 1992). Also, people do not expend large amounts of intellectual resources, but rather are likely to envision only the scenarios that they believe are necessary to reach a goal (Gigerenzer and Selten 2001). Finally, research suggests that occupants who are highly trained and/or know of specific procedures will be guided by training and will likely not develop more than one option at a time (Klein 1999). The second stage in the decision-making phase is to choose one of the options to perform. Rationally-based research claims that occupants will optimize their decision-making by considering all options developed and choosing the best one – known as rational choice strategy
  • 29. (Slovic, Fischhoff and Lichtenstein 1977; Peterson and Beach 1967). In a fire situation, weighing of multiple options is unlikely to occur. Research on decision- making under uncertainty indicates that occupants use a variety of heuristics to make this choice (Klein 1999; Kahneman, Slovic and Tversky 1982). Heuristics are simple rules to explain how individuals make decisions. Whereas some research might view the use of heuristics as a source of bias in decision-making (Tversky and Kahneman 1982), other researchers see heuristics as strengths based on the use of expertise (Flin et al. 1997). Examples of heuristics that occupants employ in choosing options include anchoring or focusing on the first option developed (Kahneman, Slovic and Tversky 1982), choosing the most available option (the easiest to develop or recall) (Kahneman, Slovic and Tversky 1982), comparing all options with each other and choosing one based on the evaluation criteria (Orasanu and Fischer 1997; Janis and Mann 1977; Hammond and Adelman 1976), and satisficing (Simon 1956). 13 Satisficing (Slovic, Kunreuther and White 1974; Gigerenzer and Selten 2001) is a method in which an individual chooses the first option that seems to work, though not necessarily the best option overall (Klein 1999). The satisficing heuristic actually combines the processes of option development and option choice together in one step. As the
  • 30. decision-maker develops options, s/he evaluates each one as it is developed and stops developing options when one is deemed to satisfy the search criteria. Whereas the rational choice strategy is more likely to be used when people attempt to optimize a decision (Klein 1999), satificing is more likely to be used in situations with a greater time pressure, dynamic conditions, and ill-defined goals (Klein 1999). In Phase 4 of the behavioral process, occupants may perform the action that they decided upon in the decision-making phase. If new information/cues are presented before an action is performed, the occupant will discard the current action and begin the behavioral process again. The action involves performing some type of physical act, although the act could be waiting or even inaction, that takes some amount of time to complete. Both summary research (e.g., Bryan 2002; Proulx 2002; Tong and Canter 1985) and research on specific incidents (e.g., Averill et al. 2005; Isner and Klem 1993; Bryan 1982; Best 1977) highlight certain actions in which occupants are likely to engage. These actions, depending upon the situation, can include seeking information, waiting, investigating the incident, alerting others, preparing for evacuation, assisting others, fighting the fire, and searching for and rescuing others. For general purposes, labeling these activities as actions would be appropriate; however, when developing a behavioral model and eventually a computer model, it is important to distinguish between the goals and actions that occupants undertake (Ozel 1985). A goal is an overall objective of the occupant (e.g., fighting the
  • 31. fire) which translates into a series of actions that lead toward achieving that objective (e.g., occupant will travel to the location of the fire extinguisher, then the location of the fire, etc.). Discussion The current evacuation models, as shown in Table 1, either do not simulate occupant behavior at all (Table 1a) or simulate occupant behavior by relying on the user to pre-determine the types of response delays or occupant actions that are likely to occur (Table 1b). In both instances, occupant behavior is simplified in such a way that may inappropriately influence the evacuation time(s) calculated by the model. In one case, occupant behavior is ignored (Table 1a) and in the other case, occupant behavior is simulated either as a distributed delay time or as an imposed action or action sequence that involves isolated movement and delays rather than occupant interaction and group dynamics (Table 1b). In addition, the current behavioral models (Table 1b) require users to provide a large amount of input data on occupant delays and/or action sequences. Most of this information would be impossible to provide since it is required before the evacuation simulation begins, i.e., before the conditions of the scenario are established by the model. Therefore, this paper proposes the development of a conceptual model (shown in Table 1c) that relies on data and theory imbedded in the evacuation model to predict occupant actions. In the proposed conceptual model, occupant actions are a result of the
  • 32. developing conditions of the simulation (the fire, the building, and the actions of other occupants) which become input into the occupant decision-making process. There are many benefits to the development of a comprehensive conceptual model for the field of human behavior in building fires. The inclusion of a conceptual model into computer evacuation tools will enable a comprehensive model that can actually predict occupant behavior in a building fire and require the user to provide only initial input for the scenario (i.e., information about the fire scenario, the building and the characteristics of the people). A computer model that incorporates a complete behavioral conceptual model would be able to predict situations rather than engineer an outcome based heavily on user input. 14 A conceptual model will reduce the burden placed on users of evacuation models and rely on the model to simulate behavior during an event. Additionally, a comprehensive behavioral model of building evacuations illustrates where more data needs to be collected in order to truly understand human behavior in future fire evacuations. Table 1: Diagrams representing three different evacuation model types a. Current Non- Behavioral Model
  • 33. b. Current Behavioral Model c. Proposed Behavioral Model No behavior is simulated. The population or the individual begins to evacuate via the defined route as soon as the event begins. The user determines the types of responses that occupants are likely to take during the model configuration stage and these actions are carried out during the simulation. A group or individual in the simulation can either delay and/or perform an action or series of actions during the evacuation based on initial, user-defined inputs. The model’s underlying data and theory predict the behavioral responses taken by the occupants based on their perceived cues and the interpretations/decisions made. An individual in the simulation develops an interpretation of the cue, the situation and the risk and
  • 34. then makes a decision on what to do next. This can occur for an individual each time a cue is received. Movement to Safety Event is Initiated Event is Initiated Event is Initiated Cue Interpretations Response User Action(s) Delay Future Research – How Can We Use This Theory? The next step in developing a comprehensive theory is to analyze qualitative data from actual events to identify the various cue-related and occupant-related factors that influence each phase of the behavioral process. An example is provided here: data
  • 35. from an actual event could show that certain cues, for example, a fire alarm, influence a “false alarm interpretation” for certain types of occupants and “only a low amount of perceived risk,” which leads to the performance of certain longer-delay activities: e.g., continued working, continued sleeping, milling behavior; whereas instructions to evacuate provided by a member of the fire department produce a 15 completely different behavioral process and eventual set of actions. Once these influential factors are linked to specific interpretations, risk perceptions and activities, a behavioral model can be developed. Then, the behavioral theory could be translated into programming language that can be tested and used in current evacuation models. This model will need to be validated, however, with behavioral data from other fire evacuation events. Conclusion Evacuation models are incomplete and oversimplified – they do not account for actual occupant behavior during buildings fires. A solution to this problem is to generate a comprehensive, robust, and validated theory on human behavior during evacuation from building fires. Behavior during a building fire evacuation is the result of a behavioral process. Each process
  • 36. begins with new cues and information from the physical and social environment. First, cues need to be perceived, then they are interpreted, and then a decision is made as to what action (including inaction) is undertaken. During an evacuation, individuals repeat this process several times as they engage in a variety of different activities both before and during purposive evacuation movement. The social scientific literature and case studies from fires and disasters can be gleaned to develop this theory, which can then be incorporated to update the current evacuation models to more accurately simulate occupant behavior during fire evacuations. With more accurate and realistic evacuation models and results, engineers and sociologists can develop safer and more cost- effective procedures and building designs in the future. 16 References Aguirre, Benigno E. 2005. “Emergency Evacuations, Panic, and Social Psychology: Commentary on ‘Understanding Mass Panic and Other Collective Responses to Threat and Disaster.’” Article #402. Newark, DE: University of Delaware, Disaster Research Center. Aguirre, B. E., Dennis Wenger, and Gabriela Vigo. 1998. “A Test of the Emergent Norm Theory of
  • 37. Collective Behavior.” Sociological Forum 13:301-320. Averill, Jason D., Dennis S. Mileti, Richard D. Peacock, Erica D. Kuligowski, Norman Groner, Guylene Proulx, Paul A. Reneke and Harold E. Nelson. 2005. “Federal Building and Fire Safety Investigation of the World Trade Center Disaster: Occupant Behavior, Egress, and Emergency Communications.” Report NCSTAR 1-7. Gaithersburg, MD: National Institute of Standards and Technology. http://wtc.nist.gov/NISTNCSTAR1-7.pdf Bensilum, M. and David A. Purser. 2002. “Gridflow: An object- oriented building evacuation model combining pre-movement and movement behaviours for performance-based design.” In 7th International Symposium on Fire Safety Science, edited by D. Evans. London, England: Interscience Communications Ltd. Ben Zur, H. and S. J. Breznitz. 1981. “The Effect of Time Pressure on Risky Choice Behavior.” Acta Psychologica 47:89-104. Berk, Richard A. 1974. Collective Behavior. Dubuque, IO: WM. C. Brown Company Publishers. Best, R. L. 1977. Reconstruction of a Tragedy: The Beverly Hills Supper Club Fire. Quincy, MA: National
  • 38. Fire Protection Association. Breaux, J., David Canter, and Jonathan D. Sime. 1976. “Psychological Aspects of Behaviour of People in Fire Situations.” Pp. 39-50 in International Fire Protection Seminar, 5th. Karlsruhe, West Germany. Brennan, Patricia. 1999. “Modelling Cue Recognition and Pre- Evacuation Response.” Pp. 1029-1040 in Proceedings – 6th International Symposium of Fire Safety Science. London, England: International Association for Fire Safety Science. Bryan, John L. 2002. “Behavioral Response to Fire and Smoke.” Pp. 3-315 – 3-341 in The SFPE Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering Third Edition, edited by P.J. DiNenno. Quincy, MA: National Fire Protection Association. Bryan, John L. 1983. An Examination and Analysis of the Dynamics of the Human Behavior in the MGM Grand Hotel Fire. Quincy, MA: National Fire Protection Association. Bryan, John L. 1982. “Human Behavior in the MGM Grand Hotel Fire.” Fire Journal 76:37-48. Burns, Kevin. 2005. “Mental Models and Normal Errors.” Pp.
  • 39. 15-28 in How Professionals Make Decisions, edited by H. Montgomery, R. Lipshitz and B. Brehmer. Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum Associates. Canter, David, John Breaux and Jonathan Sime. 1980. “Domestic, Multiple Occupancy and Hospital Fires.” Pp. 117-136 in Fires and Human Behaviour, edited by David Canter. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Canter, David, Ian Donald, and Judith Chalk. 1992. “Pedestrian Behaviour during Emergencies Underground: The psychology of crowd control under life threatening circumstances.” Pp. 135- 150 in Safety in Road and Rail Tunnels, edited by A. Vardy. Bedford: Independent Technical Conferences Ltd. 17 http://wtc.nist.gov/NISTNCSTAR1-7.pdf Connell, Rory. 2001. “Collective Behavior in the September 11, 2001 Evacuation of the World Trade Center.” Preliminary Paper #313. Newark: DE: University of Delaware Disaster Research Center.
  • 40. Dynes, Russell R. and Kathleen J. Tierney. 1994. Disasters, Collective Behavior, and Social Organizations. Newark, DE: University of Delaware Press. Edelman, P., E. Herz, and L. Bickman. 1980. “A Model of Behaviour in Fires Applied to a Nursing Home Fire.” Pp. 181-203 in Fires and Human Behaviour, edited by D. Canter. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Fahy, Rita F. 2000. “New Developments in EXIT89.” Pp. 153- 159 in Proceedings of the 15th Joint Panel Meeting of the U.S./Japan Government Cooperative Program on Natural Resources. Fire Research and Safety, edited by S.L. Bryner. San Antonio, TX. Fahy, Rita F. 1996. “EXIT89 -- High-rise Evacuation Model -- Recent Enhancements and Example Applications.” Pp. 1001-1005 in Interflam '96, International Interflam Conference -- 7th Proceedings, edited by C. A. Franks and S. Grayson. London, England: Interscience Communications Ltd. Feinberg, W. E. and Norris R. Johnson. 1995. “Firescap: A Computer Simulation Model of Reaction to a Fire Alarm.” Journal of Mathematical Sociology 20:247-269.
  • 41. Filippidis, L., E. Galea, S. Gwynne, P. Lawrence. 2006. “Representing the Influence of Signage on Evacuation Behaviour within an Evacuation Model.” Journal of Fire Protection Engineering 16:37-73. Flin, Rhona, Eduardo Salas, Michael Strub, and Lynne Martin. 1997. Decision Making Under Stress. England, UK: Ashgate Publishing Ltd. Fraser-Mitchell, J. N. 1999. “Modelling Human Behavior within the Fire Risk Assessment Tool “CRISP.” Fire and Materials 23:349-355. Galea, E. R., G. Sharp, P. J. Lawrence, and R. Holden. 2008. “Approximating the Evacuation of the World Trade Center North Tower using Computer Simulation.” Journal of Fire Protection Engineering 18:85-115. Galea, Edwin, Zhaozhi Wang, Anand Veeraswamy, Fuchen Jia, Peter J. Lawrence, and John Ewer. 2008. “Coupled Fire/Evacuation Analysis of the Station Nightclub Fire.” In the 9th International Symposium on Fire Safety Science. Gigerenzer, G. and R. Selten. 2001. Bounded Rationality: The Adaptive Toolbox. Cambridge, MA: The
  • 42. MIT Press. Gioia, Dennis A. and Peter P. Poole. 1984. “Scripts in Organizational Behavior.” Academy of Management Review 9:449-459. Goode, Erich. 1992. Collective Behavior. New York, NY: Magickal Childe Inc. Grosshandler, William, Nelson Bryner, Daniel Madrzykowski, and K. Kuntz. 2005. “Report of the Technical Investigation of The Station Nightclub Fire” Report NCSTAR 2: Vol. 1. Gaithersburg, MD: National Institute of Standards and Technology. Gwynne, Steve M. V. 2000. “The introduction of adaptive social decision-making in the mathematical modelling of egress behaviour.” Ph.D. Thesis, University of Greenwich, London, UK. 18 Gwynne, S., E. R. Galea, P. J. Lawrence, M. Owen and L. Filippidis. 1999a. “Adaptive Decision-Making in buildingEXODUS.” Journal of Applied Fire Science 8:265-289. Gwynne, S., E. R. Galea, P. J. Lawrence, M. Owen, and L.
  • 43. Filippidis. 1999b. “A Review of the Methodologies used in the Computer Simulation of Evacuation from the Built Environment.” Building and Environment 34:741-749. Hall, John R. 2004. “How Many People Can Be Saved from Home Fires if Given More Time to Escape?” Fire Technology 40:117-126. Hammond, K. R. and L. Adelman. 1976. “Science, Values, and Human Judgment.” Science 194:389-396. Isner, M. S. and T. J. Klem. 1993. World Trade Center Explosion and Fire, New York, New York, February 26, 1993. Fire Investigation Report. Quincy, MA: National Fire Protection Association. Janis, I. L. 1982. “Decision Making Under Stress.” Pp. 69-87 in Handbook of Stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects, edited by L. Goldberger and S. Breznitz. New York, NY: The Free Press. Janis, I. L. and L. Mann. 1977. Decision Making: A psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York, NY: Free Press. Kahneman, Daniel, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky. 1982. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • 44. Karau, Steven J. and Janice R. Kelly. 1992. “The Effects of Time Scarcity and Time Abundance on Group Performance Quality and Interaction Process.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 28:542-571. Klein, Gary. 1999. Sources of Power: How People Make Decisions. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Kuligowski, Erica D. and Richard D. Peacock. 2005. “Review of Building Evacuation Models.” Report NIST TN 1471. Gaithersburg, MD: National Institute of Standards and Technology. Marx, Gary T. and Douglas McAdam. 1994. Collective Behavior and Social Movements. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc. McPhail, Clark. 1991. The Myth of the Madding Crowd. New York, NY: Walter de Gruyter, Inc. Mead. George Herbert. 1938. The Philosophy of the Act. Edited by C.W. Morris with J.M. Brewster, A.M. Dunham, & D. Miller. Chicago: University of Chicago. Mileti, Dennis S. and E. M. Beck. 1975. "Communication in Crisis: Explaining Evacuation Symbolically." Communication Research 2(1):24-49.
  • 45. Mileti, Dennis S. and John H. Sorensen. 1990. “Communication of Emergency Public Warnings: A Social Science Perspective and State-of-the-Art Assessment.” Oak Ridge, TN: Oak Ridge National Laboratory, U.S. Department of Energy. Miller, David L. 1985. Introduction to Collective Behavior. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press, Inc. Nelson, Harold E. and Frederick W. Mowrer. 2002. “Emergency Movement.” Pp. 3-367 – 3-380 in The SFPE Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering Third Edition, edited by P.J. DiNenno. Quincy, MA: National Fire Protection Association. 19 Okabe, Keizo, and Shunji Mikami. 1982. “A Study on the Socio-Psychological Effect of a False Warning of the Tokai Earthquake in Japan.” A Paper presented at the Tenth World Congress of Sociology, Mexico City, Mexico, August. Orasanu, J. and U. Fischer. 1997. “Finding Decisions in Natural Environments: The view from the cockpit.” Pp. 343-358 in Naturalistic Decision Making, edited by C. Zsambok and G. Klein. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • 46. Ozel, F. 1985. “A Stochastic Computer Simulation of the Behavior of People in Fires: An Environmental Cognitive Approach.” In Proceedings of the International Conference on Building Use and Safety Technology. Washington, DC: National Institute of Building Sciences. Perry, Ronald W. and Marjorie R. Greene. 1983. Citizen Response to Volcanic Eruptions: The Case of Mt. St. Helens. New York, NY: Irvington Publishers. Perry, Ronald W., Michael K. Lindell, and Marjorie R. Greene. 1981. Evacuation Planning in Emergency Management. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Peterson, C. R. and L. R. Beach. 1967. “Man as an Intuitive Statistician.” Psychological Bulletin 68:29-46. Proulx, Guylene. 2002. “Movement of People: The Evacuation Timing.” Pp. 3-342 – 3-365 in The SFPE Handbook of Fire Protection Engineering Third Edition, edited by P.J. DiNenno. Quincy, MA: National Fire Protection Association. Purser, David A. and Steven M. V. Gwynne. 2007 “Identifying Critical Evacuation Factors and the Application of Egress Models. Pp. 203-214 in Proceedings of
  • 47. the 11th International Interflam Conference, edited by C. A. Franks and S. Grayson. London, England: Interscience Communications Ltd. Rudolph, Jenny W., J. Bradley Morrison. 2007. “Confidence, Error, and Ingenuity in Diagnostic Problem Solving: Clarifying the Role of Exploration and Exploitation.” Best Paper Proceedings of the Academy of Management Annual Meeting. Rudolph, Jenny W. and Daniel B. Raemer. 2004. “Diagnostic problem solving during simulated crises in the OR. Anesthesia and Analgesia 98:S34. Santos, Gabriel and Aguirre, Benigno E. 2005. “Critical Review of Emergency Evacuation Simulation Models.” Pp. 27-52 in Workshop on Building Occupant Movement During Fire Emergencies, edited by R. D. Peacock and E. D. Kuligowski. Gaithersburg, MD: National Institute of Standards and Technology. Simon, H. A. 1956. “Rational Choice and the Structure of Environments.” Psych. Rev. 63:129-138. Slovic, Paul, Baruch Fischhoff and Sarah Lichtenstein. 1977. “Behavioral Decision Theory.” Annual Review of Psychology 28:1-39.
  • 48. Slovic, Paul, Howard Kunreuther and Gilbert F. White. 1974. “Decision Processes, Rationality, and Adjustments to Natural Hazards.” Pp. 187-205 in Natural Hazards, edited by Gilbert F. White. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Smelser, Neil J. 1962. Theory of Collective Behavior. New York, NY: The Free Press. Spearpoint, M. 2004. “Effect of Pre-evacuation Distributions on Evacuation Times in the Simulex Model.” Journal of Fire Protection Engineering 14:33-54. 20 21 Starbuck, W. H. and F. J. Milliken. 1988. “Executives’ perceptual filters: What they notice and how they make sense. Pp. 35-65 in The executive effect: Concepts and methods for studying top managers, edited by D. C. Hambrick. Greenwich, CT: JAI. Thompson, Catherine M., John A. Forester, Susan E. Cooper, Dennis C. Bley, and John Wreathall. 1997. Pp. 9-13-9-17 in Proceedings of the IEEE Sixth Annual Human Factors Meeting.
  • 49. Thompson, Peter A., J. Wu, and E. W. Marchant. 1996. “Modelling Evacuation in Multi-storey Buildings with Simulex.” Fire Engineering 56:7-11. Tong, D. and David Canter. 1985. “The Decision to Evacuate: A Study of the Motivations which Contribute to Evacuation in the Event of a Fire.” Fire Safety Journal 9:257-265. Turner, Ralph H. and Lewis M. Killian. 1987. Collective Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc. Turner, Ralph H. and Lewis M. Killian. 1957. Collective Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc. Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman. 1982. “Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.” Pp. 3- 20 in Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, edited by D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, and A. Tversky. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Vaughan, Diane. 1999. “The Dark Side of Organizations: Mistake, Misconduct, and Disaster.” Annual Review of Sociology 25:271-305. Weick, Karl E. 1995. Sensemaking in Organizations. Thousand
  • 50. Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Weick, Karl E. 1993. “The Collapse of Sensemaking in Organizations: The Mann Gulch Disaster.” Administrative Science Quarterly 38: 628-652. Wiegman, Oene, Egli Komilis, Bernard Cadet, Henk Boer, and Jan M. Gutteling. 1992. “The Response of Local Residents to a Chemical Hazard Warning: Prediction of Behavioral Intentions in Greece, France and the Netherlands.” International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters 10:499- 515. USFA. 2007. http://www.usfa.dhs.gov/statistics/national/index.shtm Zakay, Dan. 1993. “The Impact of Time Perception Processes on Decision Making Under Time Stress.” Pp. 59-72 in Time Pressure and Stress in Human Judgment and Decision Making, edited by Ola Svenson and A. John Maule. New York, NY: Plenum Press. http://www.usfa.dhs.gov/statistics/national/index.shtm FIR 4306, Human Behavior in Fire 1
  • 51. Course Learning Outcomes for Unit VII Upon completion of this unit, students should be able to: 1. Explain how building evacuation models work. 2. Explain the use of building evacuation models. 3. Explain how building evacuation models can be improved. 4. Discuss the theory of occupant behavior during building fires. Reading Assignment Kuligowski, E.D., (2008). Modeling human behavior during building fires (NIST Technical Note 1619). Washington DC: United States of America Department of Commerce. Retrieved from http://fire.nist.gov/bfrlpubs/fire09/art018.html In order to access the resources below, you must first log into the myCSU Student Portal and access the ABI/Inform Complete database within the CSU Online Library. Kuligowski, E. (2013, 01). Predicting human behavior during fires. Fire Technology, 49, 101-120. Wales, D., & Thompson, O. F. (2013). Human behaviour in fire: Should the fire service stop telling and start listening? International Journal of Emergency Services, 2(2), 94-103.
  • 52. Unit Lesson Have you ever been in a public building like a hotel or office building when a fire alarm goes off? What is your first response? Do you immediately head to the exit, or do you stop and gather your things? Maybe you hesitate or talk to others to see if the alarm is really a drill or a serious situation. What about the evacuation maps located in hotels and public buildings? When you check into a hotel, do you take the time to locate the emergency exits? Do you calculate how long it would take you and your family to evacuate a hotel if a fire were to break out? Emergency personnel, researchers, and other professionals use evacuation models to determine escape routes in an emergency, but do they take all the factors necessary into account for an accurate prediction of evacuation procedures? This unit looks at how evacuation models work, how they are used, and how evacuation models can be improved by taking occupant behavior into account during fires. Evacuation models can be used to evaluate a level of safety provided by buildings during evacuations. Most evacuation models focus on calculating and predicting evacuation movements. The calculations can be conducted through engineering programs and in some case through hand calculation. The problem in a lot of situations is that these models typically ignore the predictions of behaviors of occupants or make wrong assumptions about occupant behavior. A solution to this challenge is to generate a robust, comprehensive and validated theory on human behavior during evacuation of building fires. Research on fire injuries and deaths shows that two-thirds of injured victims and fatalities
  • 53. could have been evacuated (Kuligowski, 2008). In what way do building evacuation models work? Evacuation models quantify evacuation performance by calculating how long it takes for occupants to evacuate a building (Kuligowski, 2008). The model attempts to simulate two things, the actions occupants participate in and how long it takes to perform these actions. For example, evacuation models can offer floor clearing times and locate congestion points showing how people may converge together and form cluster in halls or stairways. Along with specific evacuation movements, occupants may partake in a variety of other activities during evacuation that can hinder arrival to a safe location. Occupants may take the time to gather personal items, assist fellow occupants, or help put out a fire. UNIT VII STUDY GUIDE Modeling Human Behavior During Building Fires FIR 4306, Human Behavior in Fire 2 There are typically two methods used to account for occupant behavior during building evacuation. One method is for the user to assign a time period of delay or waiting to individuals or groups. Another method is for the user to assign a specific behavior itinerary of actions or specific action to an individual or group (Kuligowski, 2008). There are problems with the approaches
  • 54. that models use to simulate the behaviors during evacuation. First, no behavior is actually predicted by the models. Behavioral information is provided based on prior, pre-programmed assumptions. Behavior is determined by the user or the model based on prescribed information and in turn, can have a design or user bias built in (Kuligowski, 2008). Experimental projects rely on evaluation, especially in situations where real-time data is not available or conditions cannot easily be investigated. Evacuation models can also be used in incident recreation projects. The models in these situations help investigators determine why a situation happened, why a fire resulted in so many fatalities, or why there was a problem with the evacuation method. It may also answer questions related to the specific incidents. Model users are likely to be fire investigators, researchers, engineers, and/or consultants (Kuligowski, 2008). It is important that a theory takes into account the range of behaviors of those involved in a building fire. Occupants respond in a variety of ways based on the different cues presented to them. Even when occupants are presented with the same cues, they are likely to act differently (Kuligowski, 2008). This is because humans are individuals and will react differently based on their past experience, what they perceive is happening around them in relationship to the fire, and how they view the risks involved during the situation. All of these issues help determine the ultimate decisions that will be made as to the next steps to take during the fire situation. A conceptual model of behavior process for building fires has four phases. These are: perception of the cues, interpretation of the cues, situation, and risk, decision-making, and actions.
  • 55. Engineers, fire fighters, and even psychologists and sociologists need to work together to create authentic and true-to-life evacuation models. These accurate models should allow emergency personnel to make more informed decisions during a rescue attempt, and provide occupants with a higher chance to reach safety during a building fire. Reference Kuligowski, E. D., (2008). Modeling human behavior during building fires (NIST Technical Note 1619). Washington DC: United States of America Department of Commerce. Learning Activities (Non-Graded) If you chose to create a blog in Unit III, take the time now to update it one last time. Discuss the topics in this course that you believe will help you in your professional career. Describe a behavior change in yourself that may have occurred based on what you have learned during this course. If you choose, email your instructor, reminding them of your blog site address and let them know you have updated your blog. Non-graded Learning Activities are provided to aid students in their course of study. You do not have to submit them. If you have questions, contact your instructor for further guidance and information.