SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Key Constructs in “Classical” and “New Wave” Cognitive
Behavioral
Psychotherapies: Relationships Among Each Other
and With Emotional Distress
Ioana A. Cristea,1,2 Guy H. Montgomery,3 Ştefan Szamoskozi,1
and Daniel David1,3
1 Babes-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca
2 University of Pisa
3 Mount Sinai School of Medicine
Objective: We aimed to relate key constructs from three forms
of cognitive behavioral ther-
apy that are often placed in competition: rational emotive
behavior therapy, cognitive therapy, and
acceptance and commitment therapy. The key constructs of the
underlying theories (i.e., irrational
beliefs/unconditional self-acceptance, dysfunctional cognitions,
experiential avoidance/psychological
inflexibility) of these therapies have not been explicitly studied
in their relationships to each other and
with emotional distress. Method: We used a cross-sectional
design. The variables were selected
to indicate key constructs of the three major forms of therapy
considered. Study 1 used a sample of
152 students, who were assessed during a stressful period of
their semester (mean age = 21.71;
118 females), while Study 2 used a clinical sample of 28
patients with generalized anxiety disorder
(mean age = 26.67; 26 females). Results: Results showed that
these constructs, central in the
therapies considered, had medium to high associations to each
other and to distress. Experiential
avoidance was found to mediate the relationship between the
other, schema-type cognitive constructs
and emotional distress. Moreover, multiple mediation analysis
in Study 2 seemed to indicate that the
influence of the more general constructs on distress was
mediated by experiential avoidance, whose
effect seemed to be carried on further by automatic thoughts
that were the most proximal to distress.
Conclusions: Although each of the cognitive constructs
considered comes with its underlying
theory, the relationships between them can no longer be ignored
and cognitive behavioral therapy
theoretical models reliably accounting for these relationships
should be proposed and tested. C© 2013
Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J. Clin. Psychol. 69:584–599, 2013.
Keywords: cognitive behavioral psychotherapy; rational-
emotive behavioral therapy; cognitive therapy;
acceptance and commitment therapy; mediation
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is one of the fastest
developing fields in psychotherapy.
Emerging from the “classical” CBT paradigm (e.g., Beck, 1976;
Ellis, 1962), new forms of CBT,
sometimes called the “third wave” or the “new wave,” have
been developed (Hayes, 2004). Their
shift relates to changing the way we look at the very basis of
CBT, namely, the status of cognitive
change.
Clark (1995), in common with other leading cognitive therapists
asserts that a fundamental
postulate of the cognitive model of psychopathology is that
cognitive change is central to treating
psychological disorders, stating that “all therapies work by
altering dysfunctional cognitions,
either directly or indirectly” (p. 158). While they still view
cognitions as highly relevant to
psychopathology, third wave CBTs consider change in cognitive
content as nonessential in
We thank Dr. Aurora Szentagotai Tatar and Dr. Florin Alin Sava
for valuable consultation regarding the
adaptation of the AAQ-II, Dr. Krisztina Szabo for providing the
study database for the Romanian adaptation
of the AAQ-II, as well as Dr. Andrew Hayes and Dr. Kristopher
Preacher for important advice on mediation
procedures.
Please address correspondence to: Ioana A. Cristea, Babes-
Bolyai University, Department of Clin-
ical Psychology and Psychotherapy, No.37, Republicii St.,
400015, Cluj-Napoca, Romania. E-mail:
[email protected]
JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 69(6), 584–599
(2013) C© 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jclp). DOI: 10.1002/jclp.21976
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 585
producing therapeutic change. More precisely, from this
perspective, while thoughts are still
essential in causing and maintaining emotional disorders, the
objective of therapy is not to alter
the actual content of dysfunctional thoughts, but to transform
their function in determining
psychopathology, by modifying the individual’s relationship to
these thoughts. As such, these
third wave CBTs choose to focus on different processes (e.g.,
psychological flexibility, acceptance,
defusion) that employ a more experiential approach to the
clients’ beliefs.
The classical and the third wave CBT perspectives seem to each
be working on their own,
diverging theories as part of the same broad paradigm. Classical
CBT focuses on measuring
cognitive change (i.e., changes in measures of cognitive
constructs, such as dysfunctional
beliefs) and relating it to changes in therapeutically relevant
outcomes, such as distress or
measures of specific psychopathology. Third wave CBTs focus
on measuring processes other
than cognitive change (e.g., experiential avoidance), deemed as
etiologically relevant in causing
psychopathology, and relating these processes to very similar
outcomes.
The Classic Wave in Cognitive Behavioral Psychotherapy
Rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT). The first form of
CBT, REBT’s model
states that it is not the event itself causing our emotional states,
but our beliefs related to
that event. Regarding beliefs, it distinguishes between two key
evaluation/appraisal “styles”:
irrational and rational. The irrational evaluations are not
logical, do not have factual support
in reality, and/or hinder the person from achieving his/her
goals, while the rational ones are
logical, have empirical support, and/or help the person achieve
his/her goals. The REBT
therapeutic stance involves changing the patient’s dysfunctional
emotions and behaviors by
means of changing the irrational beliefs into rational ones and
by promoting the patient’s
unconditional self-acceptance (David, Montgomery, Macavei, &
Bovbjerg, 2005). Studies have
linked irrationality to state anxiety, anger, guilt (David, Schnur,
& Belloiu, 2002), state anger
(Martin & Dahlen, 2004), and exam-related distress
(Montgomery, David, DiLorenzo, & Schnur,
2007). Irrationality was also linked to psychopathology, such as
depressive symptoms (Chang,
1997), major depressive disorder (Solomon, Arnow, Gotlib, &
Wind, 2003), and clinical anxiety
(Muran & Motta, 1993).
Cognitive therapy (CT). The basic difference between the REBT
and the CT (Beck, 1976)
therapeutic stances lies in their chief focus on one category of
thoughts–“hot” versus “cold”
cognitions (David, Miclea, & Opre, 2004). Beck’s CT focuses
primarily on “cold” cognitions,
mental representations of relevant circumstances (Lazarus,
1991), in the forms of dysfunctional
descriptions and inferences. The difference between these two is
that, whereas descriptions
merely present an observed fact (e.g., “It’s 10 o’clock and my
wife is not home”), inferences
extend further from the perceived fact (e.g., “The fact that she
is not home at this hour shows
she is having an affair”–“jumping to conclusion” dysfunctional
belief). In contrast, therapies
such as REBT focus mainly on “hot” cognitions in the form of
evaluations (appraisals), which
refer to the ways in which “cold” cognitions/representations are
processed in terms of their
relevance for personal well-being (Lazarus, 1991; e.g., “The
fact that she is having an affair is
catastrophic for me”–“awfulizing” irrational belief). Studies
have linked dysfunctional attitudes
to distress, such as depression, anxiety, and hostility (Whittal &
Dobson, 1991) and depressed
mood (Henriques & Leitenberg, 2002). They have also been
related to psychopathology, such as
depression (de Graaf, Roelofs & Huibers, 2009), hypomania
(Lex, Hautzinger & Meyer, 2011),
and bipolar disorders (Perich, Manicavasagar, Mitchell, & Ball,
2011).
The New Wave in Cognitive Behavior Psychotherapy
Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). ACT is one of the
most visible new wave
CBTs, especially regarding the investigation of its proposed
theory. New wave CBTs downplay
the importance of explicit cognitive restructuring/challenging of
the content of thoughts in
favor of methods that attempt to alter the function of the
thoughts and not their content. More
specifically, they aim to change the individual’s relationship to
dysfunctional beliefs (i.e., the
586 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013
significance of having these beliefs), a process through which
cognitions are thought to become
“neutralized” and the distress related to them is reduced or
accepted.
Therapeutic change is considered to be brought about through
the modification of key pro-
cesses postulated to be at the root of psychopathology. One such
process is experiential avoid-
ance/psychological inflexibility. Experiential avoidance (EA)
refers to excessive negative evalua-
tions of unwanted thoughts, feelings, bodily sensations,
memories, or behavioral predispositions,
as well as an unwillingness to experience these private events,
and deliberate efforts to control
them or be rid of them (Hayes et al., 1999). EA is proposed to
represent a stronger contributor
to psychopathology than the content (e.g., intensity, frequency,
negative valence) of private psy-
chological and emotional experiences (Hayes et al., 1999). In
clinical and nonclinical samples,
it was found to be strongly correlated with measures of general
psychopathology (Hayes et al.,
2004) and specific measures of anxiety and depression (Forsyth,
Parker, & Finlay, 2003; Marx
& Sloan, 2005; Roemer, Salters, Raffa, & Orsillo, 2005).
Objectives of the Studies
The aim of the present research is to clarify the
interrelationships among key theoretical processes
of three forms of CBT (REBT, CT, and ACT) in regard to their
relative contributions to distress.
For REBT, we considered irrational beliefs and unconditional
self-acceptance as they are the core
cognitive processes in REBT (see David, Lynn, & Ellis, 2009).
For CT we chose dysfunctional
attitudes (cognitive distortions) as they are at the heart of
cognitive therapy (Beck, 1995). Finally,
for ACT, the key process considered was experiential
avoidance/psychological inflexibility, since
it is at the heart of ACT and one of the most investigated ACT
components (Hayes et al., 2004). To
our knowledge, associations between irrational
beliefs/unconditional self-acceptance (REBT),
dysfunctional cognitions (CT), experiential
avoidance/psychological inflexibility (ACT), and
emotional distress have not been examined within a single
study.
Our other objective involved determining the relative
contributions of the constructs to
psychological distress. We set to explore the possible
mediational models using these concepts,
using distress as an outcome, to test whether the effect of
classical CBT constructs on distress
was mediated by the third wave construct of experiential
avoidance or vice versa.
General Method
Overview
We investigated these relations in two types of samples: a
healthy one nonetheless vulnerable to
experiencing distress (Study 1) and a clinical sample (Study 2).
The study design, as well as the
procedure and the instruments used, were consistent across
studies.
Measures
Clinical diagnosis. We used the Structured Clinical Interview
for Diagnostic and Statisti-
cal Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition Text Revision
(DSM-IV-TR) Axis I Disorders,
Patient Edition (SCID-I/P; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams,
2002). In Study 1 we applied
only the screening questionnaire to exclude participants with a
suspicion of psychopathology,
while in Study 2 we used the entire SCID to assess clinical
diagnostic status.
Irrational and rational beliefs. The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2
(ABS 2; DiGiuseppe,
Robin, Leaf, & Gormon, 1989) was devised as a measure of the
central constructs in REBT.
It comprised 72 items representing assertions with which the
person is asked to rate his/her
agreement on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (strongly
disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The
items denote beliefs an individual might hold about particular
situations and are formulated in
either irrational or rational terms. We computed both
irrationality and rationality scores. The
ABS 2 was adapted and validated on the Romanian population
(Macavei, 2002). Reliability was
assessed on a sample of 340 individuals and indicated good
internal consistency (Cronbach’s
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 587
alpha of 0.88 for the total scale). Factor analysis on the
Romanian ABS 2 supported the presence
of two distinct factors, corresponding to irrationality and
rationality (Fulop, 2007).
Unconditional self-acceptance. The Unconditional Self-
Acceptance Questionnaire
(USAQ; Chamberlain & Haaga, 2001) was developed based on
Albert Ellis’s theory of un-
conditional self acceptance, a central concept of REBT. The
person has to evaluate the degree
of agreement with 20 assertions on a 7-point Likert scale.
Higher scores indicate higher un-
conditional self-acceptance. The USAQ was adapted on the
Romanian population (Macavei,
2007). Reliability analysis on a sample of 437 individuals
indicated good internal consistency
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.73). Validity analysis showed positive
associations between unconditional
self-acceptance and rational beliefs, and negative ones between
unconditional self-acceptance
and different types of dysfunctional beliefs, as well as distress.
Dysfunctional attitudes. The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A
(DAS-A; Weissman &
Beck, 1978) offers information regarding the person’s
dysfunctional attitudes, which function as
schemata through which the individual builds his/her view of
reality. It comprises 40 items for-
mulated as assertions to be rated on a 7-point Likert scale,
ranging from total agreement to total
disagreement. Even though this scale was initially developed to
measure cognitions relevant to
depression, subsequent studies showed it measured more general
cognitive vulnerabilities (Dyck,
1992). The DAS-A was adapted for the Romanian population
(Macavei, 2006). Reliability analy-
sis on a sample of 701 individuals indicated a value of
Cronbach’s alpha of 0.86. Validity analysis
showed that the scale correlated positively with other measures
of dysfunctional thinking, as
well as with negative emotions. Moreover, the DAS-A was able
to discriminate between subjects
with high and low levels of negative emotions.
Experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility. The
Acceptance and Action Ques-
tionnaire (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) is a revised form of the
AAQ (Hayes et al., 2004), which was
originally developed to provide an internally consistent measure
of the ACT treatment model
and behavioral effectiveness. The instrument comprises seven
items, representing statements
that the person evaluates in terms of how true they are for
him/her on a 7-point Likert scale.
High scores indicate high experiential avoidance (or high
psychological inflexibility). Factorial
analysis showed the AAQ-II is a one-dimensional measure that
assesses the key ACT construct
referred to, variously, as experiential avoidance or
psychological inflexibility. Confirmatory fac-
tor analysis indicated that it did so in a comparable manner
across very different samples. The
AAQ-II was translated into Romanian for the purpose of this
study (details of the procedure are
available upon request). It presented very good internal
consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88
for the sample in Study 1 and 0.90 for the clinical sample in
Study 2).
Emotional distress. The Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of
Affective Distress
(PED/PAD; Opris & Macavei, 2005) is an instrument designed
to evaluate distress. It com-
prises 39 items that are adjectives describing emotions, both
negative and positive. The subject
is asked to rate the suitability of each item in assessing how
he/she has felt during the last
2 weeks, on a 5-point Likert scale. The global negative
emotions score was used in the current
studies as an index of distress. The scale was validated on the
Romanian population (Opris &
Macavei, 2007). The reliability analysis on a sample of 745
healthy volunteers indicated very
good internal consistency for the negative emotions scale
(Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.94). Validity
analysis revealed a two-factor structure, with a general distress
factor accounting for around
60% of the variance, and a second factor, named functional
distress, for around 28% of the
variance.
Procedure
Participants were asked for consent and then given the scales.
They were told the data would
be used for study purposes and they could receive a
personalized interpretation of the results
should they wish to.
588 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013
Data Analysis
We applied correlational and mediational analysis. For
mediational analysis, we used the boot-
strapping procedure for assessing indirect effects (Preacher &
Hayes, 2008). We used the Preacher
and Hayes (2008) mediation script for SPSS for calculations.
We calculated effect sizes for the mediational models following
the procedure recommended
by Preacher and Kelley (2011), using the MBESS package
(Kelley & Lai, 2010). Given the
inherent difficulties of estimating effect sizes for mediation
procedures, the authors recommend
a standardized index called kappa-squared (i.e., κ2), which
represents the magnitude of the
indirect effect relative to the maximum possible indirect effect,
given the design of the study and
the distributional particularities of the variables considered.
Study 1
Research demonstrates that the period before an exam (Malouff
et al., 1992) is often a stressful
one, which may negatively affect emotional health. In Study 1
we explored the relationships
between the constructs in a nonclinical sample in such a period,
before moving to a clinical
sample in Study 2.
Method
One hundred and fifty-two student participants took part in the
study. The gender distribution
was 22.4% males (n = 34) and 77.6% females (n = 118). Ages
ranged from 17 to 25 years, with
a mean age of 21.71 (standard deviation [SD] = 1.33). None of
the subjects had had any prior
experience with any of the forms of therapy taken into account.
Absence of psychopathology was
assessed with the screening questionnaire of the SCID-I/P
(First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams,
2002). One hundred seventy subjects were screened, 18 of
which were not included in the study
due to suspicion of psychopathology. Subjects were tested right
before their exam period.
Results and Discussions
Descriptive data. Descriptive data for the main variables are
presented in Table 1. The
mean score for distress, reported on the PED/PAD, qualifies the
sample as having a high level of
negative emotions, according to comparisons with Romanian
norms (Opris & Macavei, 2007).
Correlational analysis. The association between the cognitive
variables from each therapy
approach considered and their associations with distress are
presented in Table 2.
The results from the correlational analysis showed a significant
and high positive correla-
tion between irrational beliefs and dysfunctional attitudes, as
well as a significant and medium
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations (SD), Minimum (Min) and
Maximum (Max) Values for the Main
Variables in Study 1
N Min Max Mean SD
USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance 151 42 120 80.08 13.27
AAQ-II Experiential avoidance 152 7 46 21.21 8.51
DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes 149 44 214 124.01 29.65
ABS 2 Global Irrationality 152 4 216 99.14 42.83
ABS 2 Rationality 152 36 163 102.05 22.38
PED/PAD Distress 150 32 127 61.93 22.73
Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire;
AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques-
tionnaire II; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2
= The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; PED/
PAD = Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective
Distress.
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 589
Table 2
Correlations Between the Cognitive and Subjective/Emotional
Variables in Study 1
Cognitive variables 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance —
2. AAQ-II Experiential avoidance −.55∗ —
3. DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes −.61∗ .53∗ —
4. ABS 2 Global Irrationality −.38∗ .40∗ .60∗ —
5. ABS 2 Rationality .19 −.21 −.40∗ −.88∗ —
Subjective-emotional variables
6. PED/PAD Distress −.42∗ .60∗ .32∗ .24∗ −.14 —
Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire;
AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques-
tionnaire; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2 =
The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; PED/
PAD = Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective
Distress.
∗ p < .003 Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons.
negative correlation between irrational beliefs and
unconditional self-acceptance. Irrational be-
liefs and dysfunctional attitudes are both core beliefs, organized
as evaluative and, respectively,
descriptive/inferential schemas, and thus strongly related to
each other. One theoretical pos-
sibility is that the core irrational beliefs prime the generation of
dysfunctional cognitions in
negative situations and then both generate more specific
evaluative and descriptive/inferential
beliefs in the form of automatic thoughts (David et al., 2009).
The small correlation between rational beliefs and unconditional
self-acceptance may indicate
they represent different aspects of adaptive thinking.
Unconditional self-acceptance denotes the
notion that “the individual fully and unconditionally accepts
himself whether or not he be-
haves intelligently, correctly, or competently and whether or not
other people approve, respect,
or love him” (Ellis, 1977, p. 101). As such, it may represent a
different thinking process than
other types of rational/functional beliefs, aiming at more
radical, profound modifications of
an individual’s life philosophy (Ellis, 1994). Experiential
avoidance/psychological inflexibility
displayed medium to high positive correlations with cognitive
constructs related to dysfunc-
tional thinking (irrationality, dysfunctional attitudes), pointing
to the existence of a degree of
overlap.
Also experiential avoidance had a medium to high negative
correlation (r = −0.55) with
unconditional self-acceptance, which again could imply they
deal with related, yet distinct,
approaches to acceptance. The REBT concept of unconditional
self-acceptance might be related
to the ACT concept of acceptance (the opposite of experiential
avoidance, see Bond et al.,
2011). In this sense we could speculate that the unconditional
regard for oneself promoted by
REBT might also incorporate an acceptance of self-critical
cognitions or of painful experiences,
promoted by ACT. An intriguing association was the negative,
but small and nonsignificant,
correlation between experiential avoidance and rational beliefs,
supporting the idea the two
represent distinct constructs.
The associations between these constructs and distress were
consistent with the underlying
theories. Irrationality had medium positive correlation with
distress (see David et al., 2005).
Dysfunctional attitudes also had medium positive correlations to
distress (see de Graaf et
al., 2009). As expected from the ACT literature (e.g., Hayes et
al., 2004), experiential avoid-
ance/psychological inflexibility bore high positive associations
with distress.
Mediation analysis. We used bootstrapping tests with 5,000 re-
samples and reported
a bias corrected and accelerated confidence interval (Preacher &
Hayes, 2008). Mediation is
considered to have taken place when the confidence interval for
the estimation of the indirect
effect does not contain 0. We alternatively tested all possible
meditational models, using distress
as the outcome (experiential avoidance as a mediator and the
classic CBT constructs as respective
predictors; experiential avoidance as predictor and each of the
other, classic CBT constructs as
potential mediators).
590 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013
Figure 1. Simple mediation diagrams for Study 1. Values are
path coefficients representing standardized
regression weights and standard errors (in parentheses). The c
path coefficient refers to the total effect of the
independent variable (IV) on the dependent variable (DV). The
c-prime path coefficient refers to the direct
effect of the IV on the DV.
The results indicated that experiential avoidance/psychological
inflexibility acted as a media-
tor in the relationship between global irrationality and
emotional distress, indirect effect = .13,
standard error (SE) = .03, 95% confidence interval (CI; bias
corrected and accelerated) = .08 to
.20. Experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility also
mediated the relationship between
unconditional self-acceptance and emotional distress, indirect
effect = −.52, SE = .10, 95%
CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = –.74 to −.34, and between
dysfunctional attitudes and
emotional distress, indirect effect = .25, SE = .05, 95% CI (bias
corrected and accelerated) =
.17 to .36. For each of the alternative models, the confidence
intervals of the indirect effects
contained zero, indicating the absence of mediation. Figure 1
depicts the significant mediation
models.
Effect sizes were calculated for all significant mediation
models. In the first one, with experien-
tial avoidance mediating the relationship between global
irrationality and emotional distress, κ2
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 591
took the value of .25, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .14 to .35. In
the second model, where experien-
tial avoidance mediated the relationship between unconditional
self-acceptance and emotional
distress, κ2 was .28, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .19 to .37. In the
case of experiential avoidance
mediating the relationship between dysfunctional attitudes and
emotional distress, κ2 was .31,
95% CI (bias corrected) = .22 to .40.
We underscore that irrational beliefs and dysfunctional
cognitions are conceptualized as core
beliefs, vulnerability factors, coded as underlying schemata
(Beck, 1995; Ellis, 1994); hence, they
are more general and not easily experienced directly. Moreover,
by interaction with specific
activating events, they generate automatic thoughts that are
experienced consciously and are
associated with dysfunctional feelings and behaviors. According
to ACT theory, experiential
avoidance might include the lack of willingness to experience
(i.e., rather than alter the content
or frequency of) these automatic thoughts (i.e., unwanted
private events – in ACT terms; Hayes
et al., 1999). Thus, if these constructs are related to each other,
the effect of irrational beliefs and
dysfunctional cognitions on distress could be mediated on one
hand by experiential avoidance,
and on the other hand by automatic thoughts. Regarding
experiential avoidance, our study
provides support for this prediction. Other studies sustain the
mediating role of automatic
thoughts in the relationship between deeper level cognitive
constructs (e.g., irrational beliefs)
and distress (Szentagotai & Freeman, 2007). However, there are
no studies investigating all of
these constructs in the same research design.
Consequentially, some interesting conjectures emerged after
Study 1, regarding the rela-
tionship between experiential avoidance and automatic thoughts
as mediators between more
general, deeper, schema-type constructs and distress. One
theoretical possibility would be that
irrational beliefs and/or dysfunctional cognitions represent
underlying cognitive vulnerabilities
that in negative situations generate automatic thoughts (specific
cognitions, evaluative and/or
descriptive/inferential, related to the activating events), which
are then experientially avoided,
generating distress. The other would be that irrational beliefs
and/or dysfunctional cognitions,
as underlying cognitive vulnerability factors, prompt the
response of experiential avoidance,
which in turn activates automatic thoughts by a mechanism
similar to the paradoxical rebound
effect of suppression (i.e., the “white bear” effect–Wegner,
Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987).
Study 2
In Study 2, we aimed to see whether the mediation models
supported in Study 1 were valid in
the case of a clinical sample. We used the same measures of
beliefs and distress so as to make
the results comparable to the ones for the healthy sample. We
also wanted to check which of
the two theoretical predictions regarding the potential role of
automatic thoughts, advanced
consequently to Study 1, better described the relationships
between the constructs considered.
Thus, we also measured automatic thoughts as a potential
mediator in the relationship between
more profound cognitive structures (e.g., irrational beliefs),
experiential avoidance and distress.
Method
Twenty-eight participants (26 females, 2 males) diagnosed with
generalized anxiety disorder
(GAD) took part in this study. Ages ranged from 21 to 50 years,
with a mean age of 26.67
(SD = 6.29). None of the subjects had had any prior experience
with any of the forms of
therapy taken into account. Subjects were recruited from an
ongoing randomized clinical trial
comparing various forms of CBT for GAD. All participants
were diagnosed with GAD after
having been evaluated with SCID-I/P module for anxiety
disorders (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, &
Williams, 2002). The procedure used was the same as in Study
1, with the addition of a measure
of automatic thoughts. Questionnaires were completed at
baseline before the participants were
randomized in any of the treatment groups.
Automatic thoughts were measured with the Automatic
Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ; Hol-
lon & Kendall, 1980). This instrument comprises 15 statements
that represent dysfunctional
self-related automatic thoughts. The subject has to rate them in
terms of frequency of occurrence
on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (almost
always). The ATQ was adapted on
592 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013
the Romanian population (Moldovan, 2007), on a sample of 240
individuals, showing excellent
reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = .92). Validity analysis indicated
the scale correlated positively
with other measures of dysfunctional thinking (irrational
beliefs, dysfunctional attitudes) and
negatively with unconditional self-acceptance and self-esteem.
We employed the same data analysis procedure as in Study 1
(correlational and mediation
analysis), but additionally we tested multiple step mediation.
We applied the Hayes, Preacher,
and Myers (2011) multiple step multiple mediation procedure in
which mediators are allowed
to influence each other, implemented in the MEDTHREE script
for SPSS. We reported bias
corrected and accelerated confidence intervals for 5000
bootstrap samples.
Results and Discussions
Descriptive data. Means and standard deviations, as well as
minimum and maximum
values for the main variables included in the study, are
presented in Table 3. Predictably, the
clinical sample scored high on measures of emotional distress,
both compared with the normative
values of the PED/PAD scale (Opris & Macavei, 2007), as well
as to the sample from Study 1.
Correlational analysis. The association among variables from
each therapy approach
considered (REBT, CT, ACT) and their associations with
distress are presented in Table 4.
Table 3
Means, Standard Deviations (SD), Minimum (Min) and
Maximum (Max) Values for the Main
Variables in Study 2
N Min Max Mean SD
USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance 28 45 109 70.14 16.97
AAQ-II Experiential avoidance 28 13 47 33.25 8.81
DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes 27 106 231 151.67 31.17
ABS 2 Global Irrationality 28 29 234 121.75 52.81
ABS 2 Rationality 28 34 130 90.91 25.79
PED/PAD Distress 28 26 132 86.57 28.31
Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire;
AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques-
tionnaire II; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2
= The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; PED/
PAD = Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective
Distress.
Table 4
Correlations Between the Cognitive and Subjective/Emotional
Variables in Study 2
Cognitive variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance —
2. AAQ-II Experiential avoidance −.62∗ —
3. DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes −.67∗ .47∗ —
4. ABS 2 Global Irrationality −.77∗ .61∗ .80∗ —
5. ABS 2 Rationality .69∗ − .48 −.74∗ −.95∗ —
6. ATQ Automatic thoughts −.38 .74* .57∗ .58∗ −56∗ —
Subjective-emotional variables
7. PED/PAD Distress −.59∗ .75∗ .58∗ .75∗ −.70∗ .79∗ —
Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire;
AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques-
tionnaire; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2 =
The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; ATQ-
Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire; PED/PAD = Profile of
Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective Dis-
tress.
∗ p < .05 Holm-Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons.
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 593
The correlation pattern was similar to the one obtained in Study
1. However the correlations
were higher than those for the sample in Study 1. Functional,
protective constructs such as
rationality or unconditional self-acceptance had medium to high
degrees of correlation,
higher than in Study 1. Their associations with the ACT
construct of experiential avoid-
ance/psychological inflexibility were also medium to high
(albeit not significant for rationality).
Automatic thoughts were, as expected from the literature,
highly correlated with distress. They
displayed medium correlations with the more broad, core
cognitive constructs (irrationality, dys-
functional attitudes), indicating they represent different, but
related constructs. We also noted
there was a high positive correlation with experiential
avoidance.
We believe these results might suggest that vulnerability factors
such as irrational beliefs,
dysfunctional schema, automatic thoughts, and experiential
avoidance are more influential and
effective in generating emotional distress in the case of a
clinical sample.
Simple mediation analysis. The results indicated experiential
avoidance/psychological
inflexibility acted as a mediator in the relationship between
global irrationality and emotional
distress, indirect effect = .15, SE = .06, 95% CI (bias corrected
and accelerated) = .06 to
.30. Experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility also
mediated the relationship between
unconditional self-acceptance and emotional distress, indirect
effect = −.65, SE = .23, 95% CI
(bias corrected and accelerated) = – 1.21 to −.30, as well as
between dysfunctional attitudes
and emotional distress, indirect effect = .26, SE = .11, 95% CI
(bias corrected and accelerated)
= .09 to .53. The converse models were again nonsignificant
(the confidence interval of the
indirect effect contained zero). A graphic representation of the
mediation models is presented in
Figure 2.
Effect sizes were calculated for all three mediation models. In
the first one, with experiential
avoidance mediating the relationship between global
irrationality and emotional distress, κ2
took the value of .34, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .16 to .53. In
the second model, where experien-
tial avoidance mediated the relationship between unconditional
self-acceptance and emotional
distress, κ2 was .39, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .17 to .59. In the
case of experiential avoidance
mediating the relationship between dysfunctional attitudes and
emotional distress, κ2 = .32, 95%
CI (bias corrected) = .10 to .51.
The results were therefore consistent with those of Study 1,
indicating that the effects of
more general, schema-like cognitive variables (irrationality,
unconditional self-acceptance, dys-
functional attitudes) on emotional distress were carried out
through the mediation of the more
circumscribed construct of experiential avoidance.
Multiple step mediation analysis. We also tested two alternative
multiple mediation
models, corresponding to the two possible paths we anticipated
theoretically: with automatic
thoughts as mediator 1 and experiential avoidance as mediator
2, and, respectively, with ex-
periential avoidance as mediator 1 and automatic thoughts as
mediator 2. As predictors we
consecutively used each of the schema-type constructs, while as
outcome we used distress.
Our results showed significant mediation in the cases in which
experiential avoidance played
the role of mediator 1 and automatic thoughts the role of
mediator 2: with irrationality as the
predictor, indirect effect = .08, SE = .05, 95% CI (bias
corrected and accelerated) = .01 to .22;
with unconditional self-acceptance as a predictor, indirect effect
= −.40, SE = .25, 95% CI (bias
corrected and accelerated) = −1.08 to −.09; and with
dysfunctional attitudes as the predictor,
indirect effect = .11, SE = .08, 95% CI (bias corrected and
accelerated) = .002 to .31. For
each of the alternative models (automatic thoughts as mediator
1 and experiential avoidance as
mediator 2), the confidence intervals of the indirect effect
contained zero, indicating the absence
of mediation. A graphic representation of these models is
presented in Figure 3.
Summary and Concluding Discussion
The first major conclusion of our research was that these core
constructs, central for each of
the therapies considered, shared a degree of overlap. Their
associations were medium to high,
which could mean they measure similar, related, but still
distinct, processes. REBT unconditional
594 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013
Figure 2. Simple mediation diagrams for Study 2. Values are
path coefficients representing standardized
regression weights and standard errors (in parentheses). The c
path coefficient refers to the total effect of the
independent variable (IV) on the dependent variable (DV). The
c-prime path coefficient refers to the direct
effect of the IV on the DV.
self-acceptance shared significant variance with ACT
acceptance (understood as the opposite
process of experiential avoidance; see Bond et al., 2011 for
details), but each maintained a
standalone, distinct part. Future studies could further clarify the
nature of these associations.
For instance, one might speculate that REBT acceptance of the
person as a whole also includes
ACT acceptance of unwanted thoughts or experiences. REBT’s
preferential, rational formulation
of desires or goals (e.g., “I would like to get a good result and
do my best to get it, but I accept
that it might not happen no matter how hard I try”) might
intersect with core acceptance
components developed in ACT. Schema-type constructs like
irrational beliefs and dysfunctional
attitudes were significantly associated with experiential
avoidance, but a non-negligible part of
each of them remained distinct. The observed overlap may be
because of the characteristics of
rigidity and inflexibility expressed by all three constructs.
The second major conclusion was that experiential
avoidance/psychological inflexibility me-
diated the relationship between the cognitive constructs (e.g.,
irrationality, unconditional self-
acceptance, dysfunctional attitudes) and distress. The effect
held for both the student sample
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 595
Figure 3. Multiple step mediation diagrams for Study 2. Values
are path coefficients representing stan-
dardized regression weights and standard errors (in
parentheses). The c path coefficient refers to the total
effect of the independent variable (IV) on the dependent
variable (DV). The c-prime path coefficient refers
to the direct effect of the IV on the DV.
in Study 1, as well as for the clinical sample in Study 2. Our
results are consistent with the
mediation analyses conducted by Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, and
Steger (2006), in which rigid,
inflexible coping mechanisms affected distress and adaptation
via experiential avoidance. While
we looked at dysfunctional thinking patterns and not coping or
emotion regulation strategies,
it is possible that the rigid, inflexible character of these
different variables is responsible for
triggering avoidant response tendencies that in turn are
responsible for sustaining distress.
596 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013
An interesting key result, which emerged in the multiple step
mediation analysis of Study 2,
was that the effect of the more general, schema-type constructs
on distress was carried out by
acting upon experiential avoidance, which in turn primed
automatic thoughts that were the most
proximal to distress. In an integrated CBT theory we can argue
that during an activating event
deeper, schema type constructs activate experiential avoidance
which in turn primes automatic
thoughts presumably by a mechanism similar to Wegner et al.
(1987) “white bear effect” (i.e.,
avoided thoughts return with more frequency). However,
because of our limited sample size, we
recommend the testing of these multiple mediation models on
larger samples.
We used a robust method for testing mediation–bootstrapping–
which has the advantage of
being independent from sample sizes and not assuming a normal
distribution of the indirect
effects (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The values for our effect
sizes (around .30) indicated we
managed to show a consistent part of the maximum indirect
effect that could have been attained
given the design and distribution characteristics. Preacher and
Kelley (2011) also gave some
tentative benchmark values for kappa-squared, warning they are
to be interpreted cautiously.
The values proposed are same ones as for Cohen’s r2–small,
medium and large effect sizes
correspond to values of 0.01, 0.09, and 0.25, respectively
(Cohen, 1988, pp. 79–81). In our
case, all of the confidence intervals for the effect sizes included
0.25, indicating large effect
sizes.
Theoretically, from a classical CBT standpoint, these results
seem to reinforce the notion
that irrational beliefs and/or dysfunctional cognitions are
underlying cognitive vulnerability
factors that in negative situations activate experiential
avoidance, which in turn primes automatic
thoughts, possibly by a mechanism similar to Wegner et al.’s
(1987) white bear effect (i.e., avoided
thoughts return with more frequency, generating distress).
However, our results can also be
integrated in a third wave CBT (i.e., ACT) perspective. It could
be that irrational beliefs and
dysfunctional cognitions, by being rigid and inflexible,
represent barriers in the pursuit of valued
goals and, thus, foster experiential avoidance/psychological
inflexibility.
The cross-sectional nature of our study does not warrant us to
draw conclusions about
the effects that changes in these constructs may have on
changes in distress. An interesting
conjecture could be that a change only in experiential avoidance
and/or automatic thoughts
might momentarily reduce distress, but leave the client with
deeper, schema-type latent cognitive
vulnerabilities (e.g., irrational beliefs, dysfunctional
cognitions), which might become activated
in future situations and foster the cycle all over again. Future
randomized controlled trials with
follow-up analyses could test this hypothesis. In this direction,
a recent study (Kuyken et al.,
2010) showed that for participants with recurrent depression
who received mindfulness-based
cognitive therapy (MBCT), cognitive reactivity posttreatment
was not related with depressive
symptoms at 15 months follow-up, but that the relationship was
present in the medication group.
While this result seems to indicate that MBCT did indeed
change the very relationship between
dysfunctional thoughts and the emotional outcomes, we note
that another study (Manicavasagar,
Perich, & Parker, 2012) looking at cognitive predictors of
change (rumination) did not report
differences between MBCT and a classical CBT intervention.
Our research has several limitations. The most important is the
very nature of the study,
which used a cross-sectional design. Cross-sectional studies
have previously been used to test
relationships between constructs in psychotherapeutic models,
both in general and for specific
disorders. Nonetheless, because all measures were taken at the
same time point, we cannot
conclude that the proposed mediators do indeed account for
changes in the dependent variable
(distress). Future studies should address this limitation by
measuring hypothesized mediators
prior to assessing outcome.
Another limitation is that while classical CBT was represented
by more core constructs,
measuring different aspects on dysfunctional thinking, third
wave CBT was represented by just
one. Future studies could include more processes coming from
ACT and third wave approaches
in relationship to more classical CBT constructs and with
distress to test for complex mediation
models. Last, procedural limitations should also be noted. While
the measures for constructs
in classical CBT are among the most widely used and
psychometrically sound (Bridges &
Harnish, 2010) and have been adapted on the Romanian
population, the measure for experiential
avoidance/psychological inflexibility (AAQ-II) had not been
previously used. However we did
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 597
report good psychometric properties on both samples used in
our studies. Also it is possible that
the screening questionnaire of the SCID used to rule out
participants with psychopathology in
Study 1 might have had some false negatives.
The study should be replicated on other samples of participants,
especially on various clinical
samples. Moreover, research efforts should be devoted to
formulating a CBT paradigm that
would explain and integrate these findings.
References
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy for emotional disorders.
New York, NY: International University
Press.
Beck, J. S. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New
York, NY: Guilford.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M.,
Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., Waltz, T., et al.
(2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance
and Action Questionnaire-II: a revised
measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential
avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 676–688.
doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007
Bridges, K. R., & Harnish, R. J. (2010). Role of irrational
beliefs in depression and anxiety: A review. Health,
02(8), 862–877. doi:10.4236/health.2010.28130
Chamberlain, J. M., & Haaga, D. A. F. (2001). Unconditional
self-acceptance and psychological health. Jour-
nal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 19,
163–176. doi:10.1023/A:1011189416600
Chang, E. C. (1997). Irrational beliefs and negative life stress:
Testing a diathesis-stress model of depressive
symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 22(1), 115–
117. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00182-1
Clark, D. A. (1995). Perceived limitations of standard cognitive
therapy: A reconsideration of efforts to
revise Beck’s theory and therapy. Journal of Cognitive
Psychotherapy, 9(3), 153–172. Retrieved from
http://www.springerpub.com/product/08898391
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
David, D., Lynn, S. J., & Ellis, A. (2009). Rational and
irrational beliefs. Research, theory and clinical
practice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
David, D., Miclea, M., & Opre, A. (2004). The information-
processing approach to the human mind: Basics
and beyond. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60(4), 353–368.
doi:10.1002/jclp.10250
David, D., Montgomery, G. H., Macavei, B., & Bovbjerg, D.
(2005). An empirical investigation of Albert
Ellis’ binary model of distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
61, 499–516.
David, D., Schnur, J., & Belloiu, A. (2002). Another search for
the ‘‘hot’’cognitions: Appraisal, irrational
beliefs, attributions, and their relation to emotion. Journal of
Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior
Therapy, 29, 93–131. doi:10.1023/A:1019876601693
De Graaf, E. L., Roelofs, J., & Huibers, M. J. (2009).
Measuring dysfunctional attitudes in the general
population: The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (form A) Revised.
Cognitive Therapy and Research,
33(4), 345–355, doi:10.1007/s10608-009-9229-y
DiGiuseppe, R., Robin, M. W., Leaf, R., & Gormon, B. (1989).
A discriminative–Validation and factor
analysis of a measure of rational /irrational beliefs. Paper
presented at the poster section of the World
Congress of Cognitive Therapy, Oxford, UK.
Dyck, M. J. (1992). Subscales of the Dysfunctional Attitude
Scale. The British Journal of Clinical Psychol-
ogy/the British Psychological Society, 31(3), 333–335.
Retrieved from: www.bpsjournals.co.uk/bjcp
Ellis, A. (1977). Psychotherapy and the value of a human being.
In A. Ellis & R. Grieger (Eds.), Handbook
of rational-emotive therapy (pp. 99–112). New York, NY:
Springer.
Ellis, A. (1994). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy (rev.
ed.). Secaucus, NJ: Birch Lane Press.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W.
(2002). Structured Clinical Interview for
DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders-Research version, Patient Edition.
New York, NY: Biometrics Research,
New York State Psychiatric Institute.
Forsyth, J. P., Parker, J. D., & Finlay, C. G. (2003). Anxiety
sensitivity, controllability, and experiential
avoidance and their relation to drug of choice and addiction
severity in a residential sample of substance-
abusing veterans. Addictive Behaviors, 28(5), 851–870.
doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(02)00216-2
Fulop, E. (2007). A confirmatory factor analysis of the
Attitudes and Belief Scale 2. Journal of Cognitive
and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 7(2), 159–170. Retrieved from
http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/
598 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013
Hayes, A. F., Preacher, K. J., & Myers, T. A. (2011). Mediation
and the estimation of indirect effects
in political communication research. In E. P. Bucy & R. L.
Holbert (Eds), Sourcebook for political
communication research: Methods, measures and analytical
techniques. New York, NY: Routledge.
Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy,
relational frame theory, and the third wave of
behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35, 639–
665. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(04)80013-3
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999).
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An experiential
approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T.,
Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., . . . Mccurry, S.
M. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary
test of a working model. The Psychological
Record, 54, 553–578. Retrieved from:
http://www.thepsychologicalrecord.org
Henriques, G., & Leitenberg, H. (2002). An experimental
analysis of the role of cognitive errors in the
development of depressed mood following negative social
feedback. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
26(2), 245–260. doi:10.1023/A:1014577904837
Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self-
statements in depression: Development of an automatic
thoughts questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4,
109–143.
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F.
(2006). Experiential avoidance as a generalized
psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and
emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1301–1320.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.003
Kelley, K., & Lai, K. (2010). MBESS (Version 3.2.0)
[Computer software and manual]. Retrieved from
http://www.cran.r-project.org/
Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, R. S.,
Byford, S., . . . Dalgleish, T. (2010). How
does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work? Behaviour
research and therapy, 48(11), 1105–1112.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.003
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Lex, C., Hautzinger, M., & Meyer, T. D. (2011). Cognitive
styles in hypomanic episodes of bipolar I disorder.
Bipolar Disorders, 13(4), 355–364. doi:10.1111/j.1399-
5618.2011.00937.x
Macavei, B. (2002). Scala de atitudini şi convingeri 2 (ABS2) –
date preliminare pentru populaţia de
limbă română [Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2 (ABS2) –
preliminary data for the Romanian speak-
ing language population]. Romanian Journal of Cognitive and
Behavioral Psychotherapies, 2(2),
105–122.
Macavei, B. (2006). Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale, Form A:
Norms for the Romanian population. Journal of
Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 6(2), 157–171.
Retrieved from: http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/
Macavei, B. (2007) Chestionarul de acceptare neconditionata
[Unconditional self-acceptance questionnaire].
In D. David (ed.), Sistem de evaluare clinica [Clinical
Evaluation System], Cluj-Napoca: RTS Publishing.
Manicavasagar, V., Perich, T., & Parker, G. (2012). Cognitive
predictors of change in cognitive behaviour
therapy and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression.
Behavioural and cognitive psychother-
apy, 40(2), 227–232. doi:10.1017/S1352465811000634
Martin, R. C., & Dahlen, E. R. (2004). Irrational beliefs and the
experience and expres-
sion of anger. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-
Behavior Therapy, 22(1), 3–20.
doi:10.1023/B:JORE.0000011574.44362.8f
Marx, B. P., & Sloan, D. M. (2003). Peritraumatic dissociation
and experiential avoidance as pre-
dictors of posttraumatic stress symptomatology. Behavior
Research and Therapy, 43, 569–583.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.04.004
Moldovan, R. (2007) Chestionarul gandurilor automate
[Automatic thoughts questionnaire]. In D. David
(Ed.), Sistem de evaluare clinica [Clinical Evaluation System],
Cluj-Napoca, RO: RTS Publishing.
Montgomery, G. H., David, D., DiLorenzo, T. A., & Schnur, J.
B. (2007). Response expectancies and irra-
tional beliefs predict exam-related distress. Journal of Rational-
Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy,
25(1), 17–34. doi:10.1007/s10942-006-0029-y
Muran, E. M., & Motta, R. W. (1993). Cognitive distortions and
irrational beliefs in post-traumatic stress,
anxiety, and depressive disorders. Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 49(2), 166–176.
Opriş, D., & Macavei, B. (2005). The distinction between
functional and dysfunctional negative emotions:
An empirical analysis. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral
Psychotherapies, 5(2), 181–195. Retrieved
from: http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/
Opris, D., & Macavei, B. (2007). The profile of emotional
distress; Norms for the Romanian
population. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral
Psychotherapies, 7(2), 139–152. Retrieved from
http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/
Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 599
Perich, T., Manicavasagar, V., Mitchell, P. B., & Ball, J. R.
(2011). Mindfulness, response styles
and dysfunctional attitudes in bipolar disorder. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 134(1–3), 126–132.
doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.004
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and
resampling strategies for assessing and comparing
indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research
Methods, 40(3), 879–891. Retrieved
from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/
Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for
mediation models: quantitative strategies for
communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16(2),
93–115. doi:10.1037/a0022658
Roemer, L., Salters, K., Raffa, S., & Orsillo, S. M. (2005). Fear
and avoidance of internal experiences
in GAD: Preliminary tests of a conceptual model. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 29, 71–88.
doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1650-2
Solomon, A., Arnow, B. A., Gotlib, I. H., & Wind, B. (2003).
Individualized measurement of irrational
beliefs in remitted depressives. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
4, 439–455. doi:10.1002/jclp.10081
Szentagotai, A., & Freeman, A. (2007). An analysis of the
relationship between irrational beliefs and
automatic thoughts in predicting distress. Journal of Cognitive
and Behavioral Psychotherapies, VII(1),
1–9. Retrieved from http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L.
(1987). Paradoxical effects of thought
suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
53(1), 5–13.
Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978, November).
Development and validation of the Dysfunctional Atti-
tudes Scale. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association,
November, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Whittal, M., & Dobson, K. S. (1991). An investigation of the
temporal relationship between anxiety and
depression as a consequence of cognitive vulnerability to
interpersonal evaluation. Canadian Journal of
Behavioural Science, 23(4), 391–398. doi:10.1037/h0079036
Copyright of Journal of Clinical Psychology is the property of
John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
Oxytocin increases trust in humans
Michael Kosfeld
1*, Markus Heinrichs2*, Paul J. Zak3, Urs Fischbacher1 &
Ernst Fehr1,4
Trust pervades human societies1,2. Trust is indispensable in
friend-
ship, love, families and organizations, and plays a key role in
economic exchange and politics3. In the absence of trust among
trading partners, market transactions break down. In the absence
of trust in a country’s institutions and leaders, political
legitimacy
breaks down. Much recent evidence indicates that trust contrib-
utes to economic, political and social success4,5. Little is
known,
however, about the biological basis of trust among humans.
Here
we show that intranasal administration of oxytocin, a neuro-
peptide that plays a key role in social attachment and affiliation
in non-human mammals6–8, causes a substantial increase in
trust
among humans, thereby greatly increasing the benefits from
social
interactions. We also show that the effect of oxytocin on trust is
not due to a general increase in the readiness to bear risks. On
the
contrary, oxytocin specifically affects an individual’s
willingness
to accept social risks arising through interpersonal interactions.
These results concur with animal research suggesting an
essential
role for oxytocin as a biological basis of prosocial approach
behaviour.
In non-human mammals, the neuropeptide oxytocin has a central
role in general behavioural regulation, particularly in positive
social
interactions. Aside from its well-known physiological functions
in
milk letdown and during labour, oxytocin receptors are
distributed
in various brain regions associated with behaviour9,10,
including pair
bonding, maternal care, sexual behaviour, and the ability to
form
normal social attachments6–8,11–15. Thus, oxytocin seems to
permit
animals to overcome their natural avoidance of proximity and
thereby facilitates approach behaviour. Given that oxytocin is
believed to promote social attachment and affiliation in non-
human mammals, we hypothesized that oxytocin might also pro-
mote prosocial approach behaviours — such as trust — in
humans.
Recent research has shown that neuropeptides cross the blood-
brain
barrier after intranasal administration16, providing a useful
method
for studying the central nervous system effects of oxytocin in
humans17,18. We used a double-blind study design to compare
trusting behaviour in a group of subjects that received a single
dose
of intranasal oxytocin with that of subjects in a control group
that
received placebo.
We analysed the effect of exogenously administered oxytocin on
individuals’ decisions in a trust game with real monetary
stakes19–22.
In this trust game, two subjects interacting anonymously play
either
the role of an investor or a trustee (Fig. 1). First, the investor
has the
option of choosing a costly trusting action by giving money to
the
trustee. If the investor transfers money, the total amount
available for
distribution between the two players increases but, initially, the
trustee reaps the whole increase. The trustee is then informed
about the investor’s transfer and can honour the investor’s trust
by
sharing the monetary increase generated by the investor’s
transfer.
Thus, if the investor gives money to the trustee and the latter
shares
the proceeds of the transfer, both players end up with a higher
monetary payoff. However, the trustee also has the option of
violating the investor’s trust. As sharing the proceeds is costly
for
the trustee, a selfish trustee will never honour the investor’s
trust
because the investor and the trustee interact only once during
the
experiment.
The investor is therefore caught in a dilemma: if he trusts and
the
trustee shares, the investor increases his payoff, but he is also
subject
to the risk that the trustee will abuse this trust. In the latter
case, the
investor is worse off than if he had not trusted at all and, adding
insult
to injury, the trustee has an unfair payoff advantage relative to
the
investor. Substantial evidence exists to show that humans are
averse
to such risks22–24. Moreover, the aversion of investors to abuse
of trust
seems to have an important role across different human cultures
and
social groups in the context of our game22,25. The investors
have to
overcome their aversion against these risks in order to trust,
allowing
us to address the question of whether oxytocin modulates this
trusting behaviour in humans.
LETTERS
Figure 1 | The trust game. Both subjects receive an initial
endowment of 12
monetary units (MU). The investor can send 0, 4, 8 or 12 MU to
the trustee.
The experimenter triples each MU the investor transfers. After
the investor’s
decision is made, the trustee is informed about the investor’s
transfer. Then
the trustee has the option of sending any amount between zero
and his total
amount available back to the investor. For example, if the
investor has sent
12 MU, the trustee possesses 48 MU (12 MU own endowment þ
36 MU
tripled transfer) and can, therefore choose any back transfer
from 0 to
48 MUs. The experimenter does not triple the back transfer. The
investor’s
final payoff corresponds to the initial endowment minus the
transfer to the
trustee, plus the back transfer from the trustee. The trustee’s
final payoff is
given by his initial endowment plus the tripled transfer of the
investor,
minus the back transfer to the investor. At the end of the
experiment, the
earned MU are exchanged into real money according to a
publicly
announced exchange rate (see Methods). Each subject made four
decisions
in the same player role while paired with four different,
randomly selected
interaction partners.
1
University of Zurich, Institute for Empirical Research in
Economics, Blumlisalpstrasse 10, CH-8006 Zurich, Switzerland.
2
University of Zurich, Department of Clinical Psychology
and Psychotherapy, Zurichbergstrasse 43, CH-8044 Zurich,
Switzerland.
3
Center for Neuroeconomics Studies, Claremont Graduate
University, Claremont, California 91711-6165,
USA.
4
Collegium Helveticum, Schmelzbergstrasse 25, CH-8092
Zurich, Switzerland.
*These authors contributed equally to this work.
Vol 435|2 June 2005|doi:10.1038/nature03701
673
© 2005 Nature Publishing Group
Our hypothesis that oxytocin increases the trusting behaviour of
investors implies that the investors in the oxytocin group (n ¼
29)
will show higher money transfers than those in the placebo
group
(n ¼ 29). In fact, our data show that oxytocin increases
investors’
trust considerably. Out of the 29 subjects, 13 (45%) in the
oxytocin
group showed the maximal trust level, whereas only 6 of the 29
subjects (21%) in the placebo group showed maximal trust (Fig.
2a).
In contrast, only 21% of the subjects in the oxytocin group had
a trust
level below 8 monetary units (MU), but 45% of the subjects in
the
control group showed such low levels of trust. These
differences in
the distribution of trust result in higher average and median
trust
levels for subjects given oxytocin (Table 1). The investors’
average
transfer is 17% higher in the oxytocin group (Mann-Whitney U-
test;
z ¼ 21.897, P ¼ 0.029, one-sided), and the median transfer in
the
oxytocin group is 10 MU, compared to a median of only 8 MU
for
subjects in the placebo group.
In the trust game, the risk on the part of the investor’s is due to
the
uncertainty of the trustee’s behaviour — that is, a social
interaction
with a specific trustee constitutes the risk. This raises the
question of
whether oxytocin helps humans to overcome a general aversion
against risks or whether oxytocin specifically affects trusting
beha-
viour in social interactions. In order to answer this question, we
conducted a risk experiment in which the investor faced the
same
choices as in the trust game but in which a random mechanism,
not
the trustee’s decision, determined the investor’s risk. The
random
mechanism in the risk experiment replicated the trustees’
decisions in
the trust experiment. Therefore, the investors faced exactly the
same
risk as in the trust experiment (see Methods); however, their
transfer
decisions were not embedded in a social interaction because
there
were no trustees in the risk experiment.
In this risk experiment, the investors’ behaviour does not differ
between the oxytocin and the placebo groups (Table 1 and Fig.
2b).
The median transfer is 8 MU and the average transfer is 7.5 MU
in
both groups (Mann-Whitney U-test; z ¼ 0.022, P ¼ 0.983; two-
sided test, n ¼ 31 in oxytocin group, n ¼ 30 in placebo group).
Moreover, there is no significant difference in a comparison of
the
placebo group in the trust experiment with the oxytocin group
and
the placebo group in the risk experiment (Kruskal-Wallis test;
x
2
¼ 0.533, d.f. ¼ 2, P ¼ 0.766), with identical median transfers
across groups (Table 1). However, if we add the oxytocin group
in
the trust experiment to these three samples, significant
differences
are observed (Kruskal-Wallis test; x2 ¼ 8.610, d.f. ¼ 3, P ¼
0.035),
indicating that only the investors in the oxytocin group of the
trust
experiment behave differently. Thus, oxytocin increases the
investors’
transfer levels in the trust experiment but not in the risk
experiment.
This finding is illustrated by a comparison of Figs 2a and b,
which
show that only 10% of the subjects with oxytocin choose the
maximal
transfer level in the risk experiment, whereas 45% choose the
maximal level in the trust experiment. Therefore, the
differences
between the oxytocin group in the trust experiment and the
oxytocin
group in the risk experiment are highly significant (Mann-
Whitney
U-test; z ¼ 22.563, P ¼ 0.010, two-sided), suggesting that
oxytocin
specifically affects trust in interpersonal interactions.
The risk experiment constitutes a powerful control for the
effects
of oxytocin on trusting behaviour because everything is kept
con-
stant relative to the trust experiment, except that the investors’
risk in
the risk experiment is not generated through a social
interaction.
Specifically, all the indirect effects of oxytocin on the state of a
subject, such as possible effects on mood or calmness, would be
present in both the trust and the risk experiment. Therefore,
these
potential indirect effects of oxytocin cannot be responsible for
the
effect of oxytocin on trusting behaviour. Moreover, in order to
provide an additional control for non-specific effects that might
be
associated with oxytocin administration, we explicitly measured
mood and calmness before substance administration and 50 min
after administration (but before subjects played the trust or the
risk
game). We used a questionnaire suitable for repeated measures
within short periods of time, one that is widely used in
neuropharma-
cological studies in humans26 and correlates with physiological
measures17. There were no statistical differences in the levels
of
mood and calmness before and after the administration of
oxytocin
in either the trust or the risk experiment. (Trust experiment:
z ¼ 21.541, P ¼ 0.123 for calmness; z ¼ 1.452, P ¼ 0.146 for
mood; n ¼ 29. Risk experiment: z ¼ 0.620, P ¼ 0.535 for
calmness;
z ¼ 20.841, P ¼ 0.400 for mood; n ¼ 31; two-sided Wilcoxon
signed rank tests.) This provides further support for our
conclusion
Figure 2 | Transfers in the trust and the risk experiment. Each
observation
represents the average transfer amount (in MU) over four
transfer decisions
per investor. a, Relative frequency of investors’ average
transfers in oxytocin
(filled bars) and placebo (open bars) groups in the trust
experiment
(n ¼ 58). Subjects given oxytocin show significantly higher
transfer levels.
b, Relative frequency of investors’ average transfers in oxytocin
(filled bars)
and placebo (open bars) groups in the risk experiment (n ¼ 61).
Subjects in
the oxytocin and the placebo group show statistically identical
transfer
levels.
Table 1 | Median and average transfer behaviour of investors
Trust experiment Risk experiment
Oxytocin group Placebo group Oxytocin group Placebo group
Mean average transfer (MU) 9.6 8.1 7.5 7.5
Median average transfer (MU) 10 8 8 8
Standard deviation of transfers (MU) 2.8 3.1 3.3 3.4
Number of observations 29 29 31 30
LETTERS NATURE|Vol 435|2 June 2005
674
© 2005 Nature Publishing Group
that the effect of oxytocin on human trust is not caused by non-
specific, psychotropic effects of oxytocin.
What mechanisms might be involved in generating the effect of
oxytocin on trusting behaviour? One possibility is that oxytocin
causes a general increase in prosocial inclinations. This implies
that
oxytocin should affect not only the prosocial behaviour of the
investors but also that of the trustees. We would therefore
predict
that those trustees who are given oxytocin should make higher
back
transfers at any given level than the trustees who received
placebo.
However, trustees given oxytocin do not show more trustworthy
behaviour (Fig. 3). At every positive transfer level (4, 8 or 12
MU),
their back transfers are statistically indistinguishable from those
of
placebo trustees (Mann Whitney U-tests; P . 0.243, two-sided
tests
for each positive transfer level). Thus, oxytocin does not
increase
the general inclination to behave prosocially. Rather, oxytocin
specifically affects the trusting behaviour of investors.
We hypothesize that the differing effect of oxytocin on the
behaviour of investors and trustees is related to the fact that
investors
and trustees face rather different situations. Specifically,
investors
have to make the first step; they have to ‘approach’ the trustee
by
transferring money. In contrast, the trustees can condition their
behaviour on the basis of the investors’ actions. Thus, the
psychology
of trust is important for investors, whereas the psychology of
strong
reciprocity27 is relevant for trustees. The fact that oxytocin
affects
subjects’ approach or trust behaviour, but not their degree of
reciprocity, is in agreement with animal studies. There is
substantial
evidence that oxytocin promotes prosocial approach behaviour
by
inhibiting defensive behaviours6,13, but there is no evidence
that
oxytocin affects reciprocity in animals.
A second mechanism behind the effect of oxytocin on trust
could
be based on subjects’ beliefs. Oxytocin might render subjects
more
optimistic about the likelihood of a good outcome. In order to
address this question, we measured the investor’s subjective
expec-
tation about the trustee’s back transfer after every transfer
decision.
A Mann-Whitney U-test indicates that these expectations do not
differ significantly between oxytocin and placebo groups at
every
feasible positive transfer level (P . 0.357, two-sided tests at
transfer
levels of 4, 8 or 12 MU). Thus, the investors given oxytocin
show
more trusting behaviour but do not hold significantly different
beliefs about the trustworthiness of others. Moreover, oxytocin
does not affect investors’ beliefs about the likelihood of a good
outcome in the risk experiment (P . 0.128, two-sided Mann
Whitney U-tests for transfer levels of 4, 8 or 12 MU).
Finally, there is the possibility that oxytocin helps subjects to
overcome their betrayal aversion in social interactions. This
expla-
nation is consistent with the differing effects of oxytocin across
the
trust and the risk experiments, and is further supported by the
fact
that investors faced a considerable betrayal risk. An increase in
the
transfer level from 4 or 8 MU to 12 MU decreased the investor’s
average payoff slightly, whereas it increased the objective risk
of very
low back transfers by the trustee. However, betrayal aversion
alone
cannot explain why investors given oxytocin make higher
transfers in
the trust experiment compared with the risk experiment, because
betrayal is impossible in the risk experiment. The higher
transfers in
the trust experiment can be reconciled with betrayal aversion if
one
acknowledges that investors’ behaviour in the trust experiment
is also
likely to be driven by the motive to increase the available
amount for
distribution between the two players28. As this motive cannot
operate
in the risk experiment, it can only increase transfers levels in
the trust
experiment. Our interpretation of oxytocin’s effect on trust in
terms
of betrayal aversion may be seen in the light of animal studies
indicating that increased availability of oxytocin in the central
nervous system facilitates approach behaviour, by linking the
over-
coming of social avoidance with the activation of brain circuits
implicated in reward (for example, the nucleus
accumbens)12,15.
The ubiquity of trusting behaviour is perhaps one of the dis-
tinguishing features of the human species. An element of trust
characterizes almost all human social interactions. Here we
have
sought to examine the effect of oxytocin on trust in humans.
Research in non-human mammals suggests that oxytocin has a
key
role in social attachment and affiliation. We find that intranasal
administration of oxytocin causes a substantial increase in
trusting
behaviour. Subjects given oxytocin seem better able to
overcome
trust obstacles such as betrayal aversion. Of course, this finding
could
be misused to induce trusting behaviours that selfish actors sub-
sequently exploit. However, our findings may also have positive
clinical implications for patients with mental disorders that are
associated with social dysfunctions (for example, social phobia
or
autism). In particular, social phobia ranks as the third most
common
mental health disorder and is characterized by marked social
deficits,
including persistent fear and avoidance of social interactions.
Thus,
our results might lead to fertile research on the role of oxytocin
in
several mental health disorders with major public health
significance.
METHODS
Subjects. A total of 194 healthy male students (mean age ^ s.d.,
22.0 ^ 3.4 yr)
from different universities in Zurich participated in the study.
The trust
experiment had 128 participants, and 66 subjects participated in
the risk
experiment. Exclusion criteria for participation were significant
medical or
psychiatric illness, medication, smoking more than 15 cigarettes
per day, and
drug or alcohol abuse. Subjects were instructed to abstain from
food and drink
(other than water) for 2 h before the experiment, and from
alcohol, smoking and
caffeine for 24 h before the experiment. Participants were
informed at the time of
recruitment that the experiment would evaluate the effects of a
hormone on
decision making. In total, 16 individuals out of the original
sample of 194 were
excluded because of incorrect substance administration (7 in the
trust experi-
ment, 5 in the risk experiment) or their stated disbelief that the
opponent in the
trust game was actually a human being (4 participants). The
study protocol was
approved by the ethics committee of the University of Zurich.
All subjects gave
written, informed consent before participation.
Substance administration. Subjects received a single intranasal
dose of 24 IU
oxytocin (Syntocinon-Spray, Novartis; 3 puffs per nostril, each
with 4 IU
oxytocin) or placebo 50 min before the start of the trust or the
risk experiment.
Subjects were randomly assigned to the oxytocin or placebo
group (double-
blind, placebo-controlled study design). In order to avoid any
subjective
substance effects (for example, olfactory effects) other than
those caused by
oxytocin, the placebo contained all inactive ingredients except
for the
neuropeptide.
Behavioural experiment and questionnaires. After substance
administration,
Figure 3 | Average back transfer of trustees to their investors.
The graph
shows the average back transfer by trustees for different levels
of investor
transfer in the oxytocin and placebo groups. The dotted line
shows the level
of the back transfer necessary to achieve payoff equality
between the investor
and the trustee. The dashed line shows a level of back transfer
equal to the
investor’s transfer to the trustee. The trustees’ back transfers
are on average
slightly higher than the amount sent by the investor. Trustees in
both
treatment groups make higher back transfers in response to
higher original
investor transfer levels. However, there is no statistically
significant
difference in back transfers between subjects in the oxytocin
and the placebo
groups.
NATURE|Vol 435|2 June 2005 LETTERS
675
© 2005 Nature Publishing Group
subjects completed questionnaires on a computer to measure
demographic
items and psychological characteristics. Owing to the crucial
role of the social
environment in triggering behavioural effects of oxytocin (as
shown in animal
research)13,29, subjects were asked to wait in the rest area
while the next part of the
experiment was prepared. During this 5-min waiting period,
subjects were seated
at different tables. Subjects at the same table could talk to each
other, but at the
beginning of the experiment they were informed that they would
not be
interacting with those subjects who sat at the same table. When
subjects re-
entered the laboratory for both experiments, they received
written instructions
(available from the authors on request) explaining the payoff
structure of the
experiment and the private payment procedure at the end of the
experiment.
Subjects were randomly and anonymously assigned to the role
of investor or
trustee in the trust experiment, and did not know the identity of
the persons with
whom they were matched. After subjects had read the
instructions in each
experiment, we checked whether they understood the payoff
structure by means
of several hypothetical examples. All subjects (with one
exception) answered the
control questions correctly. One subject did not answer the
control questions
correctly and was excluded from the data set (this subject also
did not apply the
substance correctly). In addition, subjects received an oral
summary of the
instructions.
Each subject in the trust experiment made four decisions in the
same player
role while paired with different, randomly selected interaction
partners. No pair
of subjects interacted twice. Subjects in the role of the investor
received no
feedback about the trustee’s decision between the different
interactions. After
every transfer decision, each investor was asked about his belief
with regard to
the expected back transfer from the trustee. Notably, trust levels
were statistically
constant across the four decisions. There is no time trend in
investors’ decisions
in either the oxytocin or the placebo group. In the risk
experiment, everything
was identical to the trust experiment, except that all subjects
played the role of an
investor who could transfer 0, 4, 8, or 12 MU into a project
rather than to a
trustee. In particular, an investor’s payoff risk (that is, the
distribution of payoffs)
in the risk experiment was identical to that in the trust
experiment at any feasible
transfer level.
To measure alterations in the psychological state of subjects
throughout the
course of the experiment, we assessed their mood and calmness
at the beginning
of the experiment (before substance administration) and
immediately before the
trust experiment or the risk experiment, by means of a suitable
questionnaire26.
All decisions in the experiments and the answers to the
questionnaires were
entered on a computer using z-Tree software30. Subjects
received a flat fee of 80
Swiss francs for participation in the experiment; each MU
earned in the trust and
the risk experiment was worth 0.40 Swiss francs.
Received 20 April; accepted 5 May 2005.
1. Luhmann, N. Trust and Power (Wiley, New York, 1979).
2. Coleman, J. Foundations of Social Theory 91–-118 (Belknap
Press of Harvard Univ.
Press, Cambridge, 1990).
3. Arrow, K. Gifts and exchanges. Phil. Publ. Affairs 1, 343–-
362 (1972).
4. Knack, S. & Keefer, P. Does social capital have an economic
payoff? A cross-
country investigation. Q. J. Econ. 112, 1251–-1288 (1997).
5. Zak, P. J. & Knack, S. Trust and growth. Econ. J. 111, 295–-
321 (2001).
6. Carter, C. S. Neuroendocrine perspectives on social
attachment and love.
Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–-818 (1998).
7. Uvnas-Moberg, K. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of
positive social
interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 819–-
835 (1998).
8. Insel, T. R. & Young, L. J. The neurobiology of attachment.
Nature Rev. Neurosci.
2, 129–-136 (2001).
9. Landgraf, R. & Neumann, I. D. Vasopressin and oxytocin
release within the
brain: a dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of
neuropeptide
communication. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25, 150–-176 (2004).
10. Huber, D., Pierre, V. & Ron, S. Vasopressin and oxytocin
excite distinct
neuronal populations in the central amygdala. Science 308, 245–
-248 (2005).
11. Carter, C. S., Altemus, M. & Chrousos, G. P.
Neuroendocrine and emotional
changes in the post-partum period. Prog. Brain Res. 133, 241–-
249 (2001).
12. Young, L. J., Lim, M. M., Gingrich, B. & Insel, T. R.
Cellular mechanisms of
social attachment. Horm. Behav. 40, 133–-138 (2001).
13. Pedersen, C. A. Oxytocin control of maternal behavior.
Regulation by sex
steroids and offspring stimuli. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 807, 126–-
145 (1997).
14. Heinrichs, M., Neumann, I. & Ehlert, U. Lactation and
stress: protective effects
of breast-feeding in humans. Stress 5, 195–-203 (2002).
15. Insel, T. R. & Shapiro, L. E. Oxytocin receptor distribution
reflects social
organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proc. Natl
Acad. Sci. USA
89, 5981–-5985 (1992).
16. Born, J. et al. Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach
to the human brain.
Nature Neurosci. 5, 514–-516 (2002).
17. Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C. & Ehlert,
U. Social support and
oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses
to psychosocial
stress. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1389–-1398 (2003).
18. Heinrichs, M., Meinlschmidt, G., Wippich, W., Ehlert, U. &
Hellhammer, D. H.
Selective amnesic effects of oxytocin on human memory.
Physiol. Behav. 83,
31–-38 (2004).
19. Camerer, C. & Weigelt, K. Experimental tests of a
sequential equilibrium
reputation model. Econometrica 56, 1–-36 (1988).
20. Fehr, E., Kirchsteiger, G. & Riedl, A. Does fairness prevent
market clearing? An
experimental investigation. Q. J. Econ. 108, 437–-459 (1993).
21. Berg, J., Dickhaut, J. & McCabe, K. Trust, reciprocity and
social history. Games
Econ. Behav. 10, 122–-142 (1995).
22. Bohnet, I. & Zeckhauser, R. Trust, risk and betrayal. J.
Econ. Behav. Organ. 55,
467–-484 (2004).
23. Holt, C. & Laury, S. Risk aversion and incentive effects.
Am. Econ. Rev. 92,
1644–-1655 (2002).
24. Fehr, E. & Schmidt, K. M. A theory of fairness,
competition, and cooperation.
Q. J. Econ. 114, 817–-868 (1999).
25. Hong, K. & Bohnet, I. Status and Distrust: the Relevance of
Inequality and Betrayal
Aversion (Working Paper RW04-041, Kennedy School, Harvard
Univ.,
Cambridge, 2004).
26. Steyer, R., Schwenkmezger, P., Notz, P. & Eid, M. Der
Mehrdimensionale
Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF) [Multidimensional mood
questionnaire]
(Hogrefe, Göttingen, 1997).
27. Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R. & Fehr, E. Explaining
altruistic behavior in
humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 153–-172 (2003).
28. Engelmann, D. & Strobel, M. Inequality aversion,
efficiency, and maximin
preferences in simple distribution experiments. Am. Econ. Rev.
94, 857–-869
(2004).
29. Kendrick, K. M. et al. Neural control of maternal behaviour
and olfactory
recognition of offspring. Brain Res. Bull. 44, 383–-395 (1997).
30. Fischbacher, U. z-Tree. Zurich Toolbox for Readymade
Economic Experiments
(Working Paper No. 21, Institute for Empirical Research in
Economics, Univ.,
Zurich, 1999).
Acknowledgements This paper is part of the Research Priority
Program
‘Foundations of Human Social Behaviour — Altruism versus
Egoism’ at the
University of Zurich. Financial support from the MacArthur
Foundation
(Network on Economic Environments and the Evolution of
Individual Preferences
and Social Norms) and the Cogito Foundation is gratefully
acknowledged. M.H.
also acknowledges support from the Swiss National Science
Foundation. We
thank F. Heusi, A. Enzler, S. Gilomen, E. Götz, M.
Koenigsberg, K. Korsunsky,
S. Krammer, S. Lauber, P. Obrist-Rybar, M. Schellhammer, B.
Studer, R. Stünzi
and M. Trottmann for research assistance.
Author Information Reprints and permissions information is
available at
npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions. The authors declare no
competing
financial interests. Correspondence and requests for materials
should be
addressed to E.F. ([email protected]) and M.H.
([email protected]).
LETTERS NATURE|Vol 435|2 June 2005
676
© 2005 Nature Publishing Group

More Related Content

PDF
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson Publishers
PPTX
Cognitive Behavioural Therapy in psychotherapy.pptx
DOCX
Momentary Assessment of Interpersonal Process in Psychotherapy.docx
PDF
MSc-UCL-Dissertation-9
PDF
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
PPTX
cbt-refresher.pptx
PPTX
CBT Refresher basics approach in CBT for
DOCX
Available online at www.sciencedirect.comScienceDirectBe.docx
The Importance of Multiple Perspectives in Psychiatry | Crimson Publishers
Cognitive Behavioural Therapy in psychotherapy.pptx
Momentary Assessment of Interpersonal Process in Psychotherapy.docx
MSc-UCL-Dissertation-9
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
cbt-refresher.pptx
CBT Refresher basics approach in CBT for
Available online at www.sciencedirect.comScienceDirectBe.docx

Similar to Key Constructs in Classical” and New Wave” Cognitive Behavio.docx (11)

DOCX
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docx
PDF
Comps paper
DOCX
Cognitive Behavioral Family Settings Versus Individual Settings.docx
PPTX
cbt-refresbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbher.pptx
PPTX
cbt-refreshervvvvvvvvvvvvvbbbbbbbbbbb.pptx
DOCX
Respond to at least two colleagues by explaining how they could use .docx
DOCX
Comparing Psychotherapy with Other Approaches.docx
PDF
Hyland2008 motivational concordance an important mechanism
DOCX
Application of The Cognitive Psychology in Mental Illness or Traum.docx
DOCX
Application of The Cognitive Psychology in Mental Illness or Traum.docx
PPTX
Abstract.pptx
COUPLE PSYCHOANALYTICPSYCHOTHERAPY AS THE TREATMENTOF CH.docx
Comps paper
Cognitive Behavioral Family Settings Versus Individual Settings.docx
cbt-refresbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbbher.pptx
cbt-refreshervvvvvvvvvvvvvbbbbbbbbbbb.pptx
Respond to at least two colleagues by explaining how they could use .docx
Comparing Psychotherapy with Other Approaches.docx
Hyland2008 motivational concordance an important mechanism
Application of The Cognitive Psychology in Mental Illness or Traum.docx
Application of The Cognitive Psychology in Mental Illness or Traum.docx
Abstract.pptx
Ad

More from tawnyataylor528 (20)

DOCX
•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx
DOCX
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx
DOCX
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx
DOCX
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx
DOCX
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx
DOCX
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx
DOCX
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx
DOCX
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx
DOCX
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx
DOCX
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx
DOCX
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx
DOCX
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx
DOCX
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx
DOCX
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx
DOCX
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx
DOCX
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx
DOCX
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx
DOCX
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx
DOCX
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx
DOCX
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx
•Reflective Log•Your reflective log should include the.docx
•The philosophers Thomas Hobbes and John Locke disagreed on the un.docx
•From the first e-Activity, examine two (2) economic effects that yo.docx
• What are the NYS Physical Education Standards, and how do they ali.docx
• Choose a health problem in the human population. Some examples i.docx
•Key elements to GE’s learning culture include active experimentat.docx
• This summative assessment can be completed in class or at any .docx
• 2 pages• APA• how the airport uses sustainability at the o.docx
¿Lógico o ilógicoIndicate whether each of the doctors statemen.docx
·Which of the following is considered a hybrid organizational fo.docx
·Write aresearch paper of three (3) body pages on a narrow aspec.docx
·InterviewConduct an interview and document it.During this c.docx
·Submit a 50- to 100-word response to each of the followin.docx
·Section 3·Financial management, quality and marketing asp.docx
·Why is the effort to standardize the language used in reporti.docx
·Humans belong to the genus Homo and chimpanzees to the genus .docx
·Crash House II and add resources and costs—remember, only crash.docx
·What is the main difference between the approaches of CONFLICT .docx
·What is the work of art’s historical and cultural context·.docx
·Review the steps of the SDLC. Explain why quality service deliv.docx
Ad

Recently uploaded (20)

PPTX
A powerpoint presentation on the Revised K-10 Science Shaping Paper
PPTX
B.Sc. DS Unit 2 Software Engineering.pptx
PDF
A GUIDE TO GENETICS FOR UNDERGRADUATE MEDICAL STUDENTS
PDF
advance database management system book.pdf
PDF
IGGE1 Understanding the Self1234567891011
PDF
Hazard Identification & Risk Assessment .pdf
PDF
RTP_AR_KS1_Tutor's Guide_English [FOR REPRODUCTION].pdf
PDF
Paper A Mock Exam 9_ Attempt review.pdf.
PDF
OBE - B.A.(HON'S) IN INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE -Ar.MOHIUDDIN.pdf
PPTX
ELIAS-SEZIURE AND EPilepsy semmioan session.pptx
PDF
Computing-Curriculum for Schools in Ghana
PDF
medical_surgical_nursing_10th_edition_ignatavicius_TEST_BANK_pdf.pdf
PDF
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
PDF
Black Hat USA 2025 - Micro ICS Summit - ICS/OT Threat Landscape
PPTX
TNA_Presentation-1-Final(SAVE)) (1).pptx
PDF
Τίμαιος είναι φιλοσοφικός διάλογος του Πλάτωνα
PPTX
Share_Module_2_Power_conflict_and_negotiation.pptx
PPTX
20th Century Theater, Methods, History.pptx
PDF
Vision Prelims GS PYQ Analysis 2011-2022 www.upscpdf.com.pdf
PPTX
CHAPTER IV. MAN AND BIOSPHERE AND ITS TOTALITY.pptx
A powerpoint presentation on the Revised K-10 Science Shaping Paper
B.Sc. DS Unit 2 Software Engineering.pptx
A GUIDE TO GENETICS FOR UNDERGRADUATE MEDICAL STUDENTS
advance database management system book.pdf
IGGE1 Understanding the Self1234567891011
Hazard Identification & Risk Assessment .pdf
RTP_AR_KS1_Tutor's Guide_English [FOR REPRODUCTION].pdf
Paper A Mock Exam 9_ Attempt review.pdf.
OBE - B.A.(HON'S) IN INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE -Ar.MOHIUDDIN.pdf
ELIAS-SEZIURE AND EPilepsy semmioan session.pptx
Computing-Curriculum for Schools in Ghana
medical_surgical_nursing_10th_edition_ignatavicius_TEST_BANK_pdf.pdf
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
Black Hat USA 2025 - Micro ICS Summit - ICS/OT Threat Landscape
TNA_Presentation-1-Final(SAVE)) (1).pptx
Τίμαιος είναι φιλοσοφικός διάλογος του Πλάτωνα
Share_Module_2_Power_conflict_and_negotiation.pptx
20th Century Theater, Methods, History.pptx
Vision Prelims GS PYQ Analysis 2011-2022 www.upscpdf.com.pdf
CHAPTER IV. MAN AND BIOSPHERE AND ITS TOTALITY.pptx

Key Constructs in Classical” and New Wave” Cognitive Behavio.docx

  • 1. Key Constructs in “Classical” and “New Wave” Cognitive Behavioral Psychotherapies: Relationships Among Each Other and With Emotional Distress Ioana A. Cristea,1,2 Guy H. Montgomery,3 Ştefan Szamoskozi,1 and Daniel David1,3 1 Babes-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca 2 University of Pisa 3 Mount Sinai School of Medicine Objective: We aimed to relate key constructs from three forms of cognitive behavioral ther- apy that are often placed in competition: rational emotive behavior therapy, cognitive therapy, and acceptance and commitment therapy. The key constructs of the underlying theories (i.e., irrational beliefs/unconditional self-acceptance, dysfunctional cognitions, experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility) of these therapies have not been explicitly studied in their relationships to each other and with emotional distress. Method: We used a cross-sectional design. The variables were selected to indicate key constructs of the three major forms of therapy considered. Study 1 used a sample of 152 students, who were assessed during a stressful period of their semester (mean age = 21.71; 118 females), while Study 2 used a clinical sample of 28 patients with generalized anxiety disorder (mean age = 26.67; 26 females). Results: Results showed that these constructs, central in the
  • 2. therapies considered, had medium to high associations to each other and to distress. Experiential avoidance was found to mediate the relationship between the other, schema-type cognitive constructs and emotional distress. Moreover, multiple mediation analysis in Study 2 seemed to indicate that the influence of the more general constructs on distress was mediated by experiential avoidance, whose effect seemed to be carried on further by automatic thoughts that were the most proximal to distress. Conclusions: Although each of the cognitive constructs considered comes with its underlying theory, the relationships between them can no longer be ignored and cognitive behavioral therapy theoretical models reliably accounting for these relationships should be proposed and tested. C© 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J. Clin. Psychol. 69:584–599, 2013. Keywords: cognitive behavioral psychotherapy; rational- emotive behavioral therapy; cognitive therapy; acceptance and commitment therapy; mediation Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is one of the fastest developing fields in psychotherapy. Emerging from the “classical” CBT paradigm (e.g., Beck, 1976; Ellis, 1962), new forms of CBT, sometimes called the “third wave” or the “new wave,” have been developed (Hayes, 2004). Their shift relates to changing the way we look at the very basis of CBT, namely, the status of cognitive change. Clark (1995), in common with other leading cognitive therapists asserts that a fundamental postulate of the cognitive model of psychopathology is that cognitive change is central to treating
  • 3. psychological disorders, stating that “all therapies work by altering dysfunctional cognitions, either directly or indirectly” (p. 158). While they still view cognitions as highly relevant to psychopathology, third wave CBTs consider change in cognitive content as nonessential in We thank Dr. Aurora Szentagotai Tatar and Dr. Florin Alin Sava for valuable consultation regarding the adaptation of the AAQ-II, Dr. Krisztina Szabo for providing the study database for the Romanian adaptation of the AAQ-II, as well as Dr. Andrew Hayes and Dr. Kristopher Preacher for important advice on mediation procedures. Please address correspondence to: Ioana A. Cristea, Babes- Bolyai University, Department of Clin- ical Psychology and Psychotherapy, No.37, Republicii St., 400015, Cluj-Napoca, Romania. E-mail: [email protected] JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 69(6), 584–599 (2013) C© 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jclp). DOI: 10.1002/jclp.21976 Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 585 producing therapeutic change. More precisely, from this perspective, while thoughts are still essential in causing and maintaining emotional disorders, the objective of therapy is not to alter the actual content of dysfunctional thoughts, but to transform their function in determining psychopathology, by modifying the individual’s relationship to
  • 4. these thoughts. As such, these third wave CBTs choose to focus on different processes (e.g., psychological flexibility, acceptance, defusion) that employ a more experiential approach to the clients’ beliefs. The classical and the third wave CBT perspectives seem to each be working on their own, diverging theories as part of the same broad paradigm. Classical CBT focuses on measuring cognitive change (i.e., changes in measures of cognitive constructs, such as dysfunctional beliefs) and relating it to changes in therapeutically relevant outcomes, such as distress or measures of specific psychopathology. Third wave CBTs focus on measuring processes other than cognitive change (e.g., experiential avoidance), deemed as etiologically relevant in causing psychopathology, and relating these processes to very similar outcomes. The Classic Wave in Cognitive Behavioral Psychotherapy Rational emotive behavior therapy (REBT). The first form of CBT, REBT’s model states that it is not the event itself causing our emotional states, but our beliefs related to that event. Regarding beliefs, it distinguishes between two key evaluation/appraisal “styles”: irrational and rational. The irrational evaluations are not logical, do not have factual support in reality, and/or hinder the person from achieving his/her goals, while the rational ones are logical, have empirical support, and/or help the person achieve his/her goals. The REBT therapeutic stance involves changing the patient’s dysfunctional
  • 5. emotions and behaviors by means of changing the irrational beliefs into rational ones and by promoting the patient’s unconditional self-acceptance (David, Montgomery, Macavei, & Bovbjerg, 2005). Studies have linked irrationality to state anxiety, anger, guilt (David, Schnur, & Belloiu, 2002), state anger (Martin & Dahlen, 2004), and exam-related distress (Montgomery, David, DiLorenzo, & Schnur, 2007). Irrationality was also linked to psychopathology, such as depressive symptoms (Chang, 1997), major depressive disorder (Solomon, Arnow, Gotlib, & Wind, 2003), and clinical anxiety (Muran & Motta, 1993). Cognitive therapy (CT). The basic difference between the REBT and the CT (Beck, 1976) therapeutic stances lies in their chief focus on one category of thoughts–“hot” versus “cold” cognitions (David, Miclea, & Opre, 2004). Beck’s CT focuses primarily on “cold” cognitions, mental representations of relevant circumstances (Lazarus, 1991), in the forms of dysfunctional descriptions and inferences. The difference between these two is that, whereas descriptions merely present an observed fact (e.g., “It’s 10 o’clock and my wife is not home”), inferences extend further from the perceived fact (e.g., “The fact that she is not home at this hour shows she is having an affair”–“jumping to conclusion” dysfunctional belief). In contrast, therapies such as REBT focus mainly on “hot” cognitions in the form of evaluations (appraisals), which refer to the ways in which “cold” cognitions/representations are processed in terms of their relevance for personal well-being (Lazarus, 1991; e.g., “The
  • 6. fact that she is having an affair is catastrophic for me”–“awfulizing” irrational belief). Studies have linked dysfunctional attitudes to distress, such as depression, anxiety, and hostility (Whittal & Dobson, 1991) and depressed mood (Henriques & Leitenberg, 2002). They have also been related to psychopathology, such as depression (de Graaf, Roelofs & Huibers, 2009), hypomania (Lex, Hautzinger & Meyer, 2011), and bipolar disorders (Perich, Manicavasagar, Mitchell, & Ball, 2011). The New Wave in Cognitive Behavior Psychotherapy Acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT). ACT is one of the most visible new wave CBTs, especially regarding the investigation of its proposed theory. New wave CBTs downplay the importance of explicit cognitive restructuring/challenging of the content of thoughts in favor of methods that attempt to alter the function of the thoughts and not their content. More specifically, they aim to change the individual’s relationship to dysfunctional beliefs (i.e., the 586 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013 significance of having these beliefs), a process through which cognitions are thought to become “neutralized” and the distress related to them is reduced or accepted. Therapeutic change is considered to be brought about through the modification of key pro-
  • 7. cesses postulated to be at the root of psychopathology. One such process is experiential avoid- ance/psychological inflexibility. Experiential avoidance (EA) refers to excessive negative evalua- tions of unwanted thoughts, feelings, bodily sensations, memories, or behavioral predispositions, as well as an unwillingness to experience these private events, and deliberate efforts to control them or be rid of them (Hayes et al., 1999). EA is proposed to represent a stronger contributor to psychopathology than the content (e.g., intensity, frequency, negative valence) of private psy- chological and emotional experiences (Hayes et al., 1999). In clinical and nonclinical samples, it was found to be strongly correlated with measures of general psychopathology (Hayes et al., 2004) and specific measures of anxiety and depression (Forsyth, Parker, & Finlay, 2003; Marx & Sloan, 2005; Roemer, Salters, Raffa, & Orsillo, 2005). Objectives of the Studies The aim of the present research is to clarify the interrelationships among key theoretical processes of three forms of CBT (REBT, CT, and ACT) in regard to their relative contributions to distress. For REBT, we considered irrational beliefs and unconditional self-acceptance as they are the core cognitive processes in REBT (see David, Lynn, & Ellis, 2009). For CT we chose dysfunctional attitudes (cognitive distortions) as they are at the heart of cognitive therapy (Beck, 1995). Finally, for ACT, the key process considered was experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility, since it is at the heart of ACT and one of the most investigated ACT components (Hayes et al., 2004). To
  • 8. our knowledge, associations between irrational beliefs/unconditional self-acceptance (REBT), dysfunctional cognitions (CT), experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility (ACT), and emotional distress have not been examined within a single study. Our other objective involved determining the relative contributions of the constructs to psychological distress. We set to explore the possible mediational models using these concepts, using distress as an outcome, to test whether the effect of classical CBT constructs on distress was mediated by the third wave construct of experiential avoidance or vice versa. General Method Overview We investigated these relations in two types of samples: a healthy one nonetheless vulnerable to experiencing distress (Study 1) and a clinical sample (Study 2). The study design, as well as the procedure and the instruments used, were consistent across studies. Measures Clinical diagnosis. We used the Structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statisti- cal Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002). In Study 1 we applied only the screening questionnaire to exclude participants with a
  • 9. suspicion of psychopathology, while in Study 2 we used the entire SCID to assess clinical diagnostic status. Irrational and rational beliefs. The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2 (ABS 2; DiGiuseppe, Robin, Leaf, & Gormon, 1989) was devised as a measure of the central constructs in REBT. It comprised 72 items representing assertions with which the person is asked to rate his/her agreement on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The items denote beliefs an individual might hold about particular situations and are formulated in either irrational or rational terms. We computed both irrationality and rationality scores. The ABS 2 was adapted and validated on the Romanian population (Macavei, 2002). Reliability was assessed on a sample of 340 individuals and indicated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 587 alpha of 0.88 for the total scale). Factor analysis on the Romanian ABS 2 supported the presence of two distinct factors, corresponding to irrationality and rationality (Fulop, 2007). Unconditional self-acceptance. The Unconditional Self- Acceptance Questionnaire (USAQ; Chamberlain & Haaga, 2001) was developed based on Albert Ellis’s theory of un- conditional self acceptance, a central concept of REBT. The person has to evaluate the degree
  • 10. of agreement with 20 assertions on a 7-point Likert scale. Higher scores indicate higher un- conditional self-acceptance. The USAQ was adapted on the Romanian population (Macavei, 2007). Reliability analysis on a sample of 437 individuals indicated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.73). Validity analysis showed positive associations between unconditional self-acceptance and rational beliefs, and negative ones between unconditional self-acceptance and different types of dysfunctional beliefs, as well as distress. Dysfunctional attitudes. The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A (DAS-A; Weissman & Beck, 1978) offers information regarding the person’s dysfunctional attitudes, which function as schemata through which the individual builds his/her view of reality. It comprises 40 items for- mulated as assertions to be rated on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from total agreement to total disagreement. Even though this scale was initially developed to measure cognitions relevant to depression, subsequent studies showed it measured more general cognitive vulnerabilities (Dyck, 1992). The DAS-A was adapted for the Romanian population (Macavei, 2006). Reliability analy- sis on a sample of 701 individuals indicated a value of Cronbach’s alpha of 0.86. Validity analysis showed that the scale correlated positively with other measures of dysfunctional thinking, as well as with negative emotions. Moreover, the DAS-A was able to discriminate between subjects with high and low levels of negative emotions. Experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility. The Acceptance and Action Ques-
  • 11. tionnaire (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) is a revised form of the AAQ (Hayes et al., 2004), which was originally developed to provide an internally consistent measure of the ACT treatment model and behavioral effectiveness. The instrument comprises seven items, representing statements that the person evaluates in terms of how true they are for him/her on a 7-point Likert scale. High scores indicate high experiential avoidance (or high psychological inflexibility). Factorial analysis showed the AAQ-II is a one-dimensional measure that assesses the key ACT construct referred to, variously, as experiential avoidance or psychological inflexibility. Confirmatory fac- tor analysis indicated that it did so in a comparable manner across very different samples. The AAQ-II was translated into Romanian for the purpose of this study (details of the procedure are available upon request). It presented very good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88 for the sample in Study 1 and 0.90 for the clinical sample in Study 2). Emotional distress. The Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective Distress (PED/PAD; Opris & Macavei, 2005) is an instrument designed to evaluate distress. It com- prises 39 items that are adjectives describing emotions, both negative and positive. The subject is asked to rate the suitability of each item in assessing how he/she has felt during the last 2 weeks, on a 5-point Likert scale. The global negative emotions score was used in the current studies as an index of distress. The scale was validated on the Romanian population (Opris & Macavei, 2007). The reliability analysis on a sample of 745
  • 12. healthy volunteers indicated very good internal consistency for the negative emotions scale (Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.94). Validity analysis revealed a two-factor structure, with a general distress factor accounting for around 60% of the variance, and a second factor, named functional distress, for around 28% of the variance. Procedure Participants were asked for consent and then given the scales. They were told the data would be used for study purposes and they could receive a personalized interpretation of the results should they wish to. 588 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013 Data Analysis We applied correlational and mediational analysis. For mediational analysis, we used the boot- strapping procedure for assessing indirect effects (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). We used the Preacher and Hayes (2008) mediation script for SPSS for calculations. We calculated effect sizes for the mediational models following the procedure recommended by Preacher and Kelley (2011), using the MBESS package (Kelley & Lai, 2010). Given the inherent difficulties of estimating effect sizes for mediation procedures, the authors recommend a standardized index called kappa-squared (i.e., κ2), which
  • 13. represents the magnitude of the indirect effect relative to the maximum possible indirect effect, given the design of the study and the distributional particularities of the variables considered. Study 1 Research demonstrates that the period before an exam (Malouff et al., 1992) is often a stressful one, which may negatively affect emotional health. In Study 1 we explored the relationships between the constructs in a nonclinical sample in such a period, before moving to a clinical sample in Study 2. Method One hundred and fifty-two student participants took part in the study. The gender distribution was 22.4% males (n = 34) and 77.6% females (n = 118). Ages ranged from 17 to 25 years, with a mean age of 21.71 (standard deviation [SD] = 1.33). None of the subjects had had any prior experience with any of the forms of therapy taken into account. Absence of psychopathology was assessed with the screening questionnaire of the SCID-I/P (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002). One hundred seventy subjects were screened, 18 of which were not included in the study due to suspicion of psychopathology. Subjects were tested right before their exam period. Results and Discussions Descriptive data. Descriptive data for the main variables are presented in Table 1. The
  • 14. mean score for distress, reported on the PED/PAD, qualifies the sample as having a high level of negative emotions, according to comparisons with Romanian norms (Opris & Macavei, 2007). Correlational analysis. The association between the cognitive variables from each therapy approach considered and their associations with distress are presented in Table 2. The results from the correlational analysis showed a significant and high positive correla- tion between irrational beliefs and dysfunctional attitudes, as well as a significant and medium Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations (SD), Minimum (Min) and Maximum (Max) Values for the Main Variables in Study 1 N Min Max Mean SD USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance 151 42 120 80.08 13.27 AAQ-II Experiential avoidance 152 7 46 21.21 8.51 DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes 149 44 214 124.01 29.65 ABS 2 Global Irrationality 152 4 216 99.14 42.83 ABS 2 Rationality 152 36 163 102.05 22.38 PED/PAD Distress 150 32 127 61.93 22.73 Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire; AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques- tionnaire II; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2 = The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; PED/ PAD = Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective Distress.
  • 15. Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 589 Table 2 Correlations Between the Cognitive and Subjective/Emotional Variables in Study 1 Cognitive variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 1. USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance — 2. AAQ-II Experiential avoidance −.55∗ — 3. DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes −.61∗ .53∗ — 4. ABS 2 Global Irrationality −.38∗ .40∗ .60∗ — 5. ABS 2 Rationality .19 −.21 −.40∗ −.88∗ — Subjective-emotional variables 6. PED/PAD Distress −.42∗ .60∗ .32∗ .24∗ −.14 — Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire; AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques- tionnaire; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2 = The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; PED/ PAD = Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective Distress. ∗ p < .003 Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons. negative correlation between irrational beliefs and unconditional self-acceptance. Irrational be- liefs and dysfunctional attitudes are both core beliefs, organized as evaluative and, respectively, descriptive/inferential schemas, and thus strongly related to each other. One theoretical pos- sibility is that the core irrational beliefs prime the generation of dysfunctional cognitions in negative situations and then both generate more specific evaluative and descriptive/inferential
  • 16. beliefs in the form of automatic thoughts (David et al., 2009). The small correlation between rational beliefs and unconditional self-acceptance may indicate they represent different aspects of adaptive thinking. Unconditional self-acceptance denotes the notion that “the individual fully and unconditionally accepts himself whether or not he be- haves intelligently, correctly, or competently and whether or not other people approve, respect, or love him” (Ellis, 1977, p. 101). As such, it may represent a different thinking process than other types of rational/functional beliefs, aiming at more radical, profound modifications of an individual’s life philosophy (Ellis, 1994). Experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility displayed medium to high positive correlations with cognitive constructs related to dysfunc- tional thinking (irrationality, dysfunctional attitudes), pointing to the existence of a degree of overlap. Also experiential avoidance had a medium to high negative correlation (r = −0.55) with unconditional self-acceptance, which again could imply they deal with related, yet distinct, approaches to acceptance. The REBT concept of unconditional self-acceptance might be related to the ACT concept of acceptance (the opposite of experiential avoidance, see Bond et al., 2011). In this sense we could speculate that the unconditional regard for oneself promoted by REBT might also incorporate an acceptance of self-critical cognitions or of painful experiences, promoted by ACT. An intriguing association was the negative, but small and nonsignificant,
  • 17. correlation between experiential avoidance and rational beliefs, supporting the idea the two represent distinct constructs. The associations between these constructs and distress were consistent with the underlying theories. Irrationality had medium positive correlation with distress (see David et al., 2005). Dysfunctional attitudes also had medium positive correlations to distress (see de Graaf et al., 2009). As expected from the ACT literature (e.g., Hayes et al., 2004), experiential avoid- ance/psychological inflexibility bore high positive associations with distress. Mediation analysis. We used bootstrapping tests with 5,000 re- samples and reported a bias corrected and accelerated confidence interval (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). Mediation is considered to have taken place when the confidence interval for the estimation of the indirect effect does not contain 0. We alternatively tested all possible meditational models, using distress as the outcome (experiential avoidance as a mediator and the classic CBT constructs as respective predictors; experiential avoidance as predictor and each of the other, classic CBT constructs as potential mediators). 590 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013 Figure 1. Simple mediation diagrams for Study 1. Values are path coefficients representing standardized regression weights and standard errors (in parentheses). The c
  • 18. path coefficient refers to the total effect of the independent variable (IV) on the dependent variable (DV). The c-prime path coefficient refers to the direct effect of the IV on the DV. The results indicated that experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility acted as a media- tor in the relationship between global irrationality and emotional distress, indirect effect = .13, standard error (SE) = .03, 95% confidence interval (CI; bias corrected and accelerated) = .08 to .20. Experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility also mediated the relationship between unconditional self-acceptance and emotional distress, indirect effect = −.52, SE = .10, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = –.74 to −.34, and between dysfunctional attitudes and emotional distress, indirect effect = .25, SE = .05, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = .17 to .36. For each of the alternative models, the confidence intervals of the indirect effects contained zero, indicating the absence of mediation. Figure 1 depicts the significant mediation models. Effect sizes were calculated for all significant mediation models. In the first one, with experien- tial avoidance mediating the relationship between global irrationality and emotional distress, κ2 Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 591 took the value of .25, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .14 to .35. In the second model, where experien-
  • 19. tial avoidance mediated the relationship between unconditional self-acceptance and emotional distress, κ2 was .28, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .19 to .37. In the case of experiential avoidance mediating the relationship between dysfunctional attitudes and emotional distress, κ2 was .31, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .22 to .40. We underscore that irrational beliefs and dysfunctional cognitions are conceptualized as core beliefs, vulnerability factors, coded as underlying schemata (Beck, 1995; Ellis, 1994); hence, they are more general and not easily experienced directly. Moreover, by interaction with specific activating events, they generate automatic thoughts that are experienced consciously and are associated with dysfunctional feelings and behaviors. According to ACT theory, experiential avoidance might include the lack of willingness to experience (i.e., rather than alter the content or frequency of) these automatic thoughts (i.e., unwanted private events – in ACT terms; Hayes et al., 1999). Thus, if these constructs are related to each other, the effect of irrational beliefs and dysfunctional cognitions on distress could be mediated on one hand by experiential avoidance, and on the other hand by automatic thoughts. Regarding experiential avoidance, our study provides support for this prediction. Other studies sustain the mediating role of automatic thoughts in the relationship between deeper level cognitive constructs (e.g., irrational beliefs) and distress (Szentagotai & Freeman, 2007). However, there are no studies investigating all of these constructs in the same research design.
  • 20. Consequentially, some interesting conjectures emerged after Study 1, regarding the rela- tionship between experiential avoidance and automatic thoughts as mediators between more general, deeper, schema-type constructs and distress. One theoretical possibility would be that irrational beliefs and/or dysfunctional cognitions represent underlying cognitive vulnerabilities that in negative situations generate automatic thoughts (specific cognitions, evaluative and/or descriptive/inferential, related to the activating events), which are then experientially avoided, generating distress. The other would be that irrational beliefs and/or dysfunctional cognitions, as underlying cognitive vulnerability factors, prompt the response of experiential avoidance, which in turn activates automatic thoughts by a mechanism similar to the paradoxical rebound effect of suppression (i.e., the “white bear” effect–Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987). Study 2 In Study 2, we aimed to see whether the mediation models supported in Study 1 were valid in the case of a clinical sample. We used the same measures of beliefs and distress so as to make the results comparable to the ones for the healthy sample. We also wanted to check which of the two theoretical predictions regarding the potential role of automatic thoughts, advanced consequently to Study 1, better described the relationships between the constructs considered. Thus, we also measured automatic thoughts as a potential mediator in the relationship between more profound cognitive structures (e.g., irrational beliefs),
  • 21. experiential avoidance and distress. Method Twenty-eight participants (26 females, 2 males) diagnosed with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) took part in this study. Ages ranged from 21 to 50 years, with a mean age of 26.67 (SD = 6.29). None of the subjects had had any prior experience with any of the forms of therapy taken into account. Subjects were recruited from an ongoing randomized clinical trial comparing various forms of CBT for GAD. All participants were diagnosed with GAD after having been evaluated with SCID-I/P module for anxiety disorders (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002). The procedure used was the same as in Study 1, with the addition of a measure of automatic thoughts. Questionnaires were completed at baseline before the participants were randomized in any of the treatment groups. Automatic thoughts were measured with the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ; Hol- lon & Kendall, 1980). This instrument comprises 15 statements that represent dysfunctional self-related automatic thoughts. The subject has to rate them in terms of frequency of occurrence on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (almost always). The ATQ was adapted on 592 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013 the Romanian population (Moldovan, 2007), on a sample of 240
  • 22. individuals, showing excellent reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = .92). Validity analysis indicated the scale correlated positively with other measures of dysfunctional thinking (irrational beliefs, dysfunctional attitudes) and negatively with unconditional self-acceptance and self-esteem. We employed the same data analysis procedure as in Study 1 (correlational and mediation analysis), but additionally we tested multiple step mediation. We applied the Hayes, Preacher, and Myers (2011) multiple step multiple mediation procedure in which mediators are allowed to influence each other, implemented in the MEDTHREE script for SPSS. We reported bias corrected and accelerated confidence intervals for 5000 bootstrap samples. Results and Discussions Descriptive data. Means and standard deviations, as well as minimum and maximum values for the main variables included in the study, are presented in Table 3. Predictably, the clinical sample scored high on measures of emotional distress, both compared with the normative values of the PED/PAD scale (Opris & Macavei, 2007), as well as to the sample from Study 1. Correlational analysis. The association among variables from each therapy approach considered (REBT, CT, ACT) and their associations with distress are presented in Table 4. Table 3 Means, Standard Deviations (SD), Minimum (Min) and
  • 23. Maximum (Max) Values for the Main Variables in Study 2 N Min Max Mean SD USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance 28 45 109 70.14 16.97 AAQ-II Experiential avoidance 28 13 47 33.25 8.81 DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes 27 106 231 151.67 31.17 ABS 2 Global Irrationality 28 29 234 121.75 52.81 ABS 2 Rationality 28 34 130 90.91 25.79 PED/PAD Distress 28 26 132 86.57 28.31 Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire; AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques- tionnaire II; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2 = The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; PED/ PAD = Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective Distress. Table 4 Correlations Between the Cognitive and Subjective/Emotional Variables in Study 2 Cognitive variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1. USAQ Unconditional self-acceptance — 2. AAQ-II Experiential avoidance −.62∗ — 3. DAS-A Dysfunctional attitudes −.67∗ .47∗ — 4. ABS 2 Global Irrationality −.77∗ .61∗ .80∗ — 5. ABS 2 Rationality .69∗ − .48 −.74∗ −.95∗ — 6. ATQ Automatic thoughts −.38 .74* .57∗ .58∗ −56∗ — Subjective-emotional variables 7. PED/PAD Distress −.59∗ .75∗ .58∗ .75∗ −.70∗ .79∗ — Note. USAQ = Unconditional Self-Acceptance Questionnaire; AAQ-II = Acceptance and Action Ques-
  • 24. tionnaire; DAS-A = Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale A; ABS 2 = The Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2; ATQ- Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire; PED/PAD = Profile of Emotional Distress/Profile of Affective Dis- tress. ∗ p < .05 Holm-Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons. Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 593 The correlation pattern was similar to the one obtained in Study 1. However the correlations were higher than those for the sample in Study 1. Functional, protective constructs such as rationality or unconditional self-acceptance had medium to high degrees of correlation, higher than in Study 1. Their associations with the ACT construct of experiential avoid- ance/psychological inflexibility were also medium to high (albeit not significant for rationality). Automatic thoughts were, as expected from the literature, highly correlated with distress. They displayed medium correlations with the more broad, core cognitive constructs (irrationality, dys- functional attitudes), indicating they represent different, but related constructs. We also noted there was a high positive correlation with experiential avoidance. We believe these results might suggest that vulnerability factors such as irrational beliefs, dysfunctional schema, automatic thoughts, and experiential avoidance are more influential and effective in generating emotional distress in the case of a clinical sample.
  • 25. Simple mediation analysis. The results indicated experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility acted as a mediator in the relationship between global irrationality and emotional distress, indirect effect = .15, SE = .06, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = .06 to .30. Experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility also mediated the relationship between unconditional self-acceptance and emotional distress, indirect effect = −.65, SE = .23, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = – 1.21 to −.30, as well as between dysfunctional attitudes and emotional distress, indirect effect = .26, SE = .11, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = .09 to .53. The converse models were again nonsignificant (the confidence interval of the indirect effect contained zero). A graphic representation of the mediation models is presented in Figure 2. Effect sizes were calculated for all three mediation models. In the first one, with experiential avoidance mediating the relationship between global irrationality and emotional distress, κ2 took the value of .34, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .16 to .53. In the second model, where experien- tial avoidance mediated the relationship between unconditional self-acceptance and emotional distress, κ2 was .39, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .17 to .59. In the case of experiential avoidance mediating the relationship between dysfunctional attitudes and emotional distress, κ2 = .32, 95% CI (bias corrected) = .10 to .51.
  • 26. The results were therefore consistent with those of Study 1, indicating that the effects of more general, schema-like cognitive variables (irrationality, unconditional self-acceptance, dys- functional attitudes) on emotional distress were carried out through the mediation of the more circumscribed construct of experiential avoidance. Multiple step mediation analysis. We also tested two alternative multiple mediation models, corresponding to the two possible paths we anticipated theoretically: with automatic thoughts as mediator 1 and experiential avoidance as mediator 2, and, respectively, with ex- periential avoidance as mediator 1 and automatic thoughts as mediator 2. As predictors we consecutively used each of the schema-type constructs, while as outcome we used distress. Our results showed significant mediation in the cases in which experiential avoidance played the role of mediator 1 and automatic thoughts the role of mediator 2: with irrationality as the predictor, indirect effect = .08, SE = .05, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = .01 to .22; with unconditional self-acceptance as a predictor, indirect effect = −.40, SE = .25, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = −1.08 to −.09; and with dysfunctional attitudes as the predictor, indirect effect = .11, SE = .08, 95% CI (bias corrected and accelerated) = .002 to .31. For each of the alternative models (automatic thoughts as mediator 1 and experiential avoidance as mediator 2), the confidence intervals of the indirect effect contained zero, indicating the absence of mediation. A graphic representation of these models is
  • 27. presented in Figure 3. Summary and Concluding Discussion The first major conclusion of our research was that these core constructs, central for each of the therapies considered, shared a degree of overlap. Their associations were medium to high, which could mean they measure similar, related, but still distinct, processes. REBT unconditional 594 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013 Figure 2. Simple mediation diagrams for Study 2. Values are path coefficients representing standardized regression weights and standard errors (in parentheses). The c path coefficient refers to the total effect of the independent variable (IV) on the dependent variable (DV). The c-prime path coefficient refers to the direct effect of the IV on the DV. self-acceptance shared significant variance with ACT acceptance (understood as the opposite process of experiential avoidance; see Bond et al., 2011 for details), but each maintained a standalone, distinct part. Future studies could further clarify the nature of these associations. For instance, one might speculate that REBT acceptance of the person as a whole also includes ACT acceptance of unwanted thoughts or experiences. REBT’s preferential, rational formulation of desires or goals (e.g., “I would like to get a good result and do my best to get it, but I accept that it might not happen no matter how hard I try”) might
  • 28. intersect with core acceptance components developed in ACT. Schema-type constructs like irrational beliefs and dysfunctional attitudes were significantly associated with experiential avoidance, but a non-negligible part of each of them remained distinct. The observed overlap may be because of the characteristics of rigidity and inflexibility expressed by all three constructs. The second major conclusion was that experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility me- diated the relationship between the cognitive constructs (e.g., irrationality, unconditional self- acceptance, dysfunctional attitudes) and distress. The effect held for both the student sample Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 595 Figure 3. Multiple step mediation diagrams for Study 2. Values are path coefficients representing stan- dardized regression weights and standard errors (in parentheses). The c path coefficient refers to the total effect of the independent variable (IV) on the dependent variable (DV). The c-prime path coefficient refers to the direct effect of the IV on the DV. in Study 1, as well as for the clinical sample in Study 2. Our results are consistent with the mediation analyses conducted by Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, and Steger (2006), in which rigid, inflexible coping mechanisms affected distress and adaptation via experiential avoidance. While we looked at dysfunctional thinking patterns and not coping or emotion regulation strategies,
  • 29. it is possible that the rigid, inflexible character of these different variables is responsible for triggering avoidant response tendencies that in turn are responsible for sustaining distress. 596 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013 An interesting key result, which emerged in the multiple step mediation analysis of Study 2, was that the effect of the more general, schema-type constructs on distress was carried out by acting upon experiential avoidance, which in turn primed automatic thoughts that were the most proximal to distress. In an integrated CBT theory we can argue that during an activating event deeper, schema type constructs activate experiential avoidance which in turn primes automatic thoughts presumably by a mechanism similar to Wegner et al. (1987) “white bear effect” (i.e., avoided thoughts return with more frequency). However, because of our limited sample size, we recommend the testing of these multiple mediation models on larger samples. We used a robust method for testing mediation–bootstrapping– which has the advantage of being independent from sample sizes and not assuming a normal distribution of the indirect effects (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The values for our effect sizes (around .30) indicated we managed to show a consistent part of the maximum indirect effect that could have been attained given the design and distribution characteristics. Preacher and Kelley (2011) also gave some
  • 30. tentative benchmark values for kappa-squared, warning they are to be interpreted cautiously. The values proposed are same ones as for Cohen’s r2–small, medium and large effect sizes correspond to values of 0.01, 0.09, and 0.25, respectively (Cohen, 1988, pp. 79–81). In our case, all of the confidence intervals for the effect sizes included 0.25, indicating large effect sizes. Theoretically, from a classical CBT standpoint, these results seem to reinforce the notion that irrational beliefs and/or dysfunctional cognitions are underlying cognitive vulnerability factors that in negative situations activate experiential avoidance, which in turn primes automatic thoughts, possibly by a mechanism similar to Wegner et al.’s (1987) white bear effect (i.e., avoided thoughts return with more frequency, generating distress). However, our results can also be integrated in a third wave CBT (i.e., ACT) perspective. It could be that irrational beliefs and dysfunctional cognitions, by being rigid and inflexible, represent barriers in the pursuit of valued goals and, thus, foster experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility. The cross-sectional nature of our study does not warrant us to draw conclusions about the effects that changes in these constructs may have on changes in distress. An interesting conjecture could be that a change only in experiential avoidance and/or automatic thoughts might momentarily reduce distress, but leave the client with deeper, schema-type latent cognitive vulnerabilities (e.g., irrational beliefs, dysfunctional
  • 31. cognitions), which might become activated in future situations and foster the cycle all over again. Future randomized controlled trials with follow-up analyses could test this hypothesis. In this direction, a recent study (Kuyken et al., 2010) showed that for participants with recurrent depression who received mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT), cognitive reactivity posttreatment was not related with depressive symptoms at 15 months follow-up, but that the relationship was present in the medication group. While this result seems to indicate that MBCT did indeed change the very relationship between dysfunctional thoughts and the emotional outcomes, we note that another study (Manicavasagar, Perich, & Parker, 2012) looking at cognitive predictors of change (rumination) did not report differences between MBCT and a classical CBT intervention. Our research has several limitations. The most important is the very nature of the study, which used a cross-sectional design. Cross-sectional studies have previously been used to test relationships between constructs in psychotherapeutic models, both in general and for specific disorders. Nonetheless, because all measures were taken at the same time point, we cannot conclude that the proposed mediators do indeed account for changes in the dependent variable (distress). Future studies should address this limitation by measuring hypothesized mediators prior to assessing outcome. Another limitation is that while classical CBT was represented by more core constructs, measuring different aspects on dysfunctional thinking, third
  • 32. wave CBT was represented by just one. Future studies could include more processes coming from ACT and third wave approaches in relationship to more classical CBT constructs and with distress to test for complex mediation models. Last, procedural limitations should also be noted. While the measures for constructs in classical CBT are among the most widely used and psychometrically sound (Bridges & Harnish, 2010) and have been adapted on the Romanian population, the measure for experiential avoidance/psychological inflexibility (AAQ-II) had not been previously used. However we did Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 597 report good psychometric properties on both samples used in our studies. Also it is possible that the screening questionnaire of the SCID used to rule out participants with psychopathology in Study 1 might have had some false negatives. The study should be replicated on other samples of participants, especially on various clinical samples. Moreover, research efforts should be devoted to formulating a CBT paradigm that would explain and integrate these findings. References Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy for emotional disorders. New York, NY: International University Press.
  • 33. Beck, J. S. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York, NY: Guilford. Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., Waltz, T., et al. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: a revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 676–688. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007 Bridges, K. R., & Harnish, R. J. (2010). Role of irrational beliefs in depression and anxiety: A review. Health, 02(8), 862–877. doi:10.4236/health.2010.28130 Chamberlain, J. M., & Haaga, D. A. F. (2001). Unconditional self-acceptance and psychological health. Jour- nal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 19, 163–176. doi:10.1023/A:1011189416600 Chang, E. C. (1997). Irrational beliefs and negative life stress: Testing a diathesis-stress model of depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 22(1), 115– 117. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00182-1 Clark, D. A. (1995). Perceived limitations of standard cognitive therapy: A reconsideration of efforts to revise Beck’s theory and therapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 9(3), 153–172. Retrieved from http://www.springerpub.com/product/08898391 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. David, D., Lynn, S. J., & Ellis, A. (2009). Rational and
  • 34. irrational beliefs. Research, theory and clinical practice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. David, D., Miclea, M., & Opre, A. (2004). The information- processing approach to the human mind: Basics and beyond. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60(4), 353–368. doi:10.1002/jclp.10250 David, D., Montgomery, G. H., Macavei, B., & Bovbjerg, D. (2005). An empirical investigation of Albert Ellis’ binary model of distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61, 499–516. David, D., Schnur, J., & Belloiu, A. (2002). Another search for the ‘‘hot’’cognitions: Appraisal, irrational beliefs, attributions, and their relation to emotion. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 29, 93–131. doi:10.1023/A:1019876601693 De Graaf, E. L., Roelofs, J., & Huibers, M. J. (2009). Measuring dysfunctional attitudes in the general population: The Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (form A) Revised. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33(4), 345–355, doi:10.1007/s10608-009-9229-y DiGiuseppe, R., Robin, M. W., Leaf, R., & Gormon, B. (1989). A discriminative–Validation and factor analysis of a measure of rational /irrational beliefs. Paper presented at the poster section of the World Congress of Cognitive Therapy, Oxford, UK. Dyck, M. J. (1992). Subscales of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale. The British Journal of Clinical Psychol- ogy/the British Psychological Society, 31(3), 333–335. Retrieved from: www.bpsjournals.co.uk/bjcp
  • 35. Ellis, A. (1977). Psychotherapy and the value of a human being. In A. Ellis & R. Grieger (Eds.), Handbook of rational-emotive therapy (pp. 99–112). New York, NY: Springer. Ellis, A. (1994). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy (rev. ed.). Secaucus, NJ: Birch Lane Press. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders-Research version, Patient Edition. New York, NY: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute. Forsyth, J. P., Parker, J. D., & Finlay, C. G. (2003). Anxiety sensitivity, controllability, and experiential avoidance and their relation to drug of choice and addiction severity in a residential sample of substance- abusing veterans. Addictive Behaviors, 28(5), 851–870. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(02)00216-2 Fulop, E. (2007). A confirmatory factor analysis of the Attitudes and Belief Scale 2. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 7(2), 159–170. Retrieved from http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/ 598 Journal Of Clinical Psychology, June 2013 Hayes, A. F., Preacher, K. J., & Myers, T. A. (2011). Mediation and the estimation of indirect effects in political communication research. In E. P. Bucy & R. L. Holbert (Eds), Sourcebook for political communication research: Methods, measures and analytical techniques. New York, NY: Routledge.
  • 36. Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35, 639– 665. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(04)80013-3 Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., . . . Mccurry, S. M. (2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. The Psychological Record, 54, 553–578. Retrieved from: http://www.thepsychologicalrecord.org Henriques, G., & Leitenberg, H. (2002). An experimental analysis of the role of cognitive errors in the development of depressed mood following negative social feedback. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26(2), 245–260. doi:10.1023/A:1014577904837 Hollon, S. D., & Kendall, P. C. (1980). Cognitive self- statements in depression: Development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 109–143. Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1301–1320. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.003 Kelley, K., & Lai, K. (2010). MBESS (Version 3.2.0)
  • 37. [Computer software and manual]. Retrieved from http://www.cran.r-project.org/ Kuyken, W., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Taylor, R. S., Byford, S., . . . Dalgleish, T. (2010). How does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work? Behaviour research and therapy, 48(11), 1105–1112. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.003 Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Lex, C., Hautzinger, M., & Meyer, T. D. (2011). Cognitive styles in hypomanic episodes of bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 13(4), 355–364. doi:10.1111/j.1399- 5618.2011.00937.x Macavei, B. (2002). Scala de atitudini şi convingeri 2 (ABS2) – date preliminare pentru populaţia de limbă română [Attitudes and Beliefs Scale 2 (ABS2) – preliminary data for the Romanian speak- ing language population]. Romanian Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 2(2), 105–122. Macavei, B. (2006). Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale, Form A: Norms for the Romanian population. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 6(2), 157–171. Retrieved from: http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/ Macavei, B. (2007) Chestionarul de acceptare neconditionata [Unconditional self-acceptance questionnaire]. In D. David (ed.), Sistem de evaluare clinica [Clinical Evaluation System], Cluj-Napoca: RTS Publishing. Manicavasagar, V., Perich, T., & Parker, G. (2012). Cognitive
  • 38. predictors of change in cognitive behaviour therapy and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression. Behavioural and cognitive psychother- apy, 40(2), 227–232. doi:10.1017/S1352465811000634 Martin, R. C., & Dahlen, E. R. (2004). Irrational beliefs and the experience and expres- sion of anger. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive- Behavior Therapy, 22(1), 3–20. doi:10.1023/B:JORE.0000011574.44362.8f Marx, B. P., & Sloan, D. M. (2003). Peritraumatic dissociation and experiential avoidance as pre- dictors of posttraumatic stress symptomatology. Behavior Research and Therapy, 43, 569–583. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.04.004 Moldovan, R. (2007) Chestionarul gandurilor automate [Automatic thoughts questionnaire]. In D. David (Ed.), Sistem de evaluare clinica [Clinical Evaluation System], Cluj-Napoca, RO: RTS Publishing. Montgomery, G. H., David, D., DiLorenzo, T. A., & Schnur, J. B. (2007). Response expectancies and irra- tional beliefs predict exam-related distress. Journal of Rational- Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 25(1), 17–34. doi:10.1007/s10942-006-0029-y Muran, E. M., & Motta, R. W. (1993). Cognitive distortions and irrational beliefs in post-traumatic stress, anxiety, and depressive disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(2), 166–176. Opriş, D., & Macavei, B. (2005). The distinction between functional and dysfunctional negative emotions: An empirical analysis. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral
  • 39. Psychotherapies, 5(2), 181–195. Retrieved from: http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/ Opris, D., & Macavei, B. (2007). The profile of emotional distress; Norms for the Romanian population. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 7(2), 139–152. Retrieved from http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro/ Key Constructs in Cognitive Behavioral Therapies 599 Perich, T., Manicavasagar, V., Mitchell, P. B., & Ball, J. R. (2011). Mindfulness, response styles and dysfunctional attitudes in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 134(1–3), 126–132. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.004 Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–891. Retrieved from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/ Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 93–115. doi:10.1037/a0022658 Roemer, L., Salters, K., Raffa, S., & Orsillo, S. M. (2005). Fear and avoidance of internal experiences in GAD: Preliminary tests of a conceptual model. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 71–88. doi:10.1007/s10608-005-1650-2
  • 40. Solomon, A., Arnow, B. A., Gotlib, I. H., & Wind, B. (2003). Individualized measurement of irrational beliefs in remitted depressives. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 4, 439–455. doi:10.1002/jclp.10081 Szentagotai, A., & Freeman, A. (2007). An analysis of the relationship between irrational beliefs and automatic thoughts in predicting distress. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, VII(1), 1–9. Retrieved from http://jcbp.psychotherapy.ro Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5–13. Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978, November). Development and validation of the Dysfunctional Atti- tudes Scale. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, November, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Whittal, M., & Dobson, K. S. (1991). An investigation of the temporal relationship between anxiety and depression as a consequence of cognitive vulnerability to interpersonal evaluation. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 23(4), 391–398. doi:10.1037/h0079036 Copyright of Journal of Clinical Psychology is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
  • 41. permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Oxytocin increases trust in humans Michael Kosfeld 1*, Markus Heinrichs2*, Paul J. Zak3, Urs Fischbacher1 & Ernst Fehr1,4 Trust pervades human societies1,2. Trust is indispensable in friend- ship, love, families and organizations, and plays a key role in economic exchange and politics3. In the absence of trust among trading partners, market transactions break down. In the absence of trust in a country’s institutions and leaders, political legitimacy breaks down. Much recent evidence indicates that trust contrib- utes to economic, political and social success4,5. Little is known, however, about the biological basis of trust among humans. Here we show that intranasal administration of oxytocin, a neuro- peptide that plays a key role in social attachment and affiliation in non-human mammals6–8, causes a substantial increase in trust among humans, thereby greatly increasing the benefits from social interactions. We also show that the effect of oxytocin on trust is not due to a general increase in the readiness to bear risks. On the contrary, oxytocin specifically affects an individual’s willingness to accept social risks arising through interpersonal interactions.
  • 42. These results concur with animal research suggesting an essential role for oxytocin as a biological basis of prosocial approach behaviour. In non-human mammals, the neuropeptide oxytocin has a central role in general behavioural regulation, particularly in positive social interactions. Aside from its well-known physiological functions in milk letdown and during labour, oxytocin receptors are distributed in various brain regions associated with behaviour9,10, including pair bonding, maternal care, sexual behaviour, and the ability to form normal social attachments6–8,11–15. Thus, oxytocin seems to permit animals to overcome their natural avoidance of proximity and thereby facilitates approach behaviour. Given that oxytocin is believed to promote social attachment and affiliation in non- human mammals, we hypothesized that oxytocin might also pro- mote prosocial approach behaviours — such as trust — in humans. Recent research has shown that neuropeptides cross the blood- brain barrier after intranasal administration16, providing a useful method for studying the central nervous system effects of oxytocin in humans17,18. We used a double-blind study design to compare trusting behaviour in a group of subjects that received a single dose of intranasal oxytocin with that of subjects in a control group that received placebo.
  • 43. We analysed the effect of exogenously administered oxytocin on individuals’ decisions in a trust game with real monetary stakes19–22. In this trust game, two subjects interacting anonymously play either the role of an investor or a trustee (Fig. 1). First, the investor has the option of choosing a costly trusting action by giving money to the trustee. If the investor transfers money, the total amount available for distribution between the two players increases but, initially, the trustee reaps the whole increase. The trustee is then informed about the investor’s transfer and can honour the investor’s trust by sharing the monetary increase generated by the investor’s transfer. Thus, if the investor gives money to the trustee and the latter shares the proceeds of the transfer, both players end up with a higher monetary payoff. However, the trustee also has the option of violating the investor’s trust. As sharing the proceeds is costly for the trustee, a selfish trustee will never honour the investor’s trust because the investor and the trustee interact only once during the experiment. The investor is therefore caught in a dilemma: if he trusts and the trustee shares, the investor increases his payoff, but he is also subject to the risk that the trustee will abuse this trust. In the latter case, the
  • 44. investor is worse off than if he had not trusted at all and, adding insult to injury, the trustee has an unfair payoff advantage relative to the investor. Substantial evidence exists to show that humans are averse to such risks22–24. Moreover, the aversion of investors to abuse of trust seems to have an important role across different human cultures and social groups in the context of our game22,25. The investors have to overcome their aversion against these risks in order to trust, allowing us to address the question of whether oxytocin modulates this trusting behaviour in humans. LETTERS Figure 1 | The trust game. Both subjects receive an initial endowment of 12 monetary units (MU). The investor can send 0, 4, 8 or 12 MU to the trustee. The experimenter triples each MU the investor transfers. After the investor’s decision is made, the trustee is informed about the investor’s transfer. Then the trustee has the option of sending any amount between zero and his total amount available back to the investor. For example, if the investor has sent 12 MU, the trustee possesses 48 MU (12 MU own endowment þ 36 MU
  • 45. tripled transfer) and can, therefore choose any back transfer from 0 to 48 MUs. The experimenter does not triple the back transfer. The investor’s final payoff corresponds to the initial endowment minus the transfer to the trustee, plus the back transfer from the trustee. The trustee’s final payoff is given by his initial endowment plus the tripled transfer of the investor, minus the back transfer to the investor. At the end of the experiment, the earned MU are exchanged into real money according to a publicly announced exchange rate (see Methods). Each subject made four decisions in the same player role while paired with four different, randomly selected interaction partners. 1 University of Zurich, Institute for Empirical Research in Economics, Blumlisalpstrasse 10, CH-8006 Zurich, Switzerland. 2 University of Zurich, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Zurichbergstrasse 43, CH-8044 Zurich, Switzerland. 3 Center for Neuroeconomics Studies, Claremont Graduate University, Claremont, California 91711-6165, USA. 4 Collegium Helveticum, Schmelzbergstrasse 25, CH-8092
  • 46. Zurich, Switzerland. *These authors contributed equally to this work. Vol 435|2 June 2005|doi:10.1038/nature03701 673 © 2005 Nature Publishing Group Our hypothesis that oxytocin increases the trusting behaviour of investors implies that the investors in the oxytocin group (n ¼ 29) will show higher money transfers than those in the placebo group (n ¼ 29). In fact, our data show that oxytocin increases investors’ trust considerably. Out of the 29 subjects, 13 (45%) in the oxytocin group showed the maximal trust level, whereas only 6 of the 29 subjects (21%) in the placebo group showed maximal trust (Fig. 2a). In contrast, only 21% of the subjects in the oxytocin group had a trust level below 8 monetary units (MU), but 45% of the subjects in the control group showed such low levels of trust. These differences in the distribution of trust result in higher average and median trust levels for subjects given oxytocin (Table 1). The investors’ average transfer is 17% higher in the oxytocin group (Mann-Whitney U-
  • 47. test; z ¼ 21.897, P ¼ 0.029, one-sided), and the median transfer in the oxytocin group is 10 MU, compared to a median of only 8 MU for subjects in the placebo group. In the trust game, the risk on the part of the investor’s is due to the uncertainty of the trustee’s behaviour — that is, a social interaction with a specific trustee constitutes the risk. This raises the question of whether oxytocin helps humans to overcome a general aversion against risks or whether oxytocin specifically affects trusting beha- viour in social interactions. In order to answer this question, we conducted a risk experiment in which the investor faced the same choices as in the trust game but in which a random mechanism, not the trustee’s decision, determined the investor’s risk. The random mechanism in the risk experiment replicated the trustees’ decisions in the trust experiment. Therefore, the investors faced exactly the same risk as in the trust experiment (see Methods); however, their transfer decisions were not embedded in a social interaction because there were no trustees in the risk experiment. In this risk experiment, the investors’ behaviour does not differ between the oxytocin and the placebo groups (Table 1 and Fig.
  • 48. 2b). The median transfer is 8 MU and the average transfer is 7.5 MU in both groups (Mann-Whitney U-test; z ¼ 0.022, P ¼ 0.983; two- sided test, n ¼ 31 in oxytocin group, n ¼ 30 in placebo group). Moreover, there is no significant difference in a comparison of the placebo group in the trust experiment with the oxytocin group and the placebo group in the risk experiment (Kruskal-Wallis test; x 2 ¼ 0.533, d.f. ¼ 2, P ¼ 0.766), with identical median transfers across groups (Table 1). However, if we add the oxytocin group in the trust experiment to these three samples, significant differences are observed (Kruskal-Wallis test; x2 ¼ 8.610, d.f. ¼ 3, P ¼ 0.035), indicating that only the investors in the oxytocin group of the trust experiment behave differently. Thus, oxytocin increases the investors’ transfer levels in the trust experiment but not in the risk experiment. This finding is illustrated by a comparison of Figs 2a and b, which show that only 10% of the subjects with oxytocin choose the maximal transfer level in the risk experiment, whereas 45% choose the maximal level in the trust experiment. Therefore, the differences between the oxytocin group in the trust experiment and the oxytocin
  • 49. group in the risk experiment are highly significant (Mann- Whitney U-test; z ¼ 22.563, P ¼ 0.010, two-sided), suggesting that oxytocin specifically affects trust in interpersonal interactions. The risk experiment constitutes a powerful control for the effects of oxytocin on trusting behaviour because everything is kept con- stant relative to the trust experiment, except that the investors’ risk in the risk experiment is not generated through a social interaction. Specifically, all the indirect effects of oxytocin on the state of a subject, such as possible effects on mood or calmness, would be present in both the trust and the risk experiment. Therefore, these potential indirect effects of oxytocin cannot be responsible for the effect of oxytocin on trusting behaviour. Moreover, in order to provide an additional control for non-specific effects that might be associated with oxytocin administration, we explicitly measured mood and calmness before substance administration and 50 min after administration (but before subjects played the trust or the risk game). We used a questionnaire suitable for repeated measures within short periods of time, one that is widely used in neuropharma- cological studies in humans26 and correlates with physiological measures17. There were no statistical differences in the levels of mood and calmness before and after the administration of oxytocin in either the trust or the risk experiment. (Trust experiment:
  • 50. z ¼ 21.541, P ¼ 0.123 for calmness; z ¼ 1.452, P ¼ 0.146 for mood; n ¼ 29. Risk experiment: z ¼ 0.620, P ¼ 0.535 for calmness; z ¼ 20.841, P ¼ 0.400 for mood; n ¼ 31; two-sided Wilcoxon signed rank tests.) This provides further support for our conclusion Figure 2 | Transfers in the trust and the risk experiment. Each observation represents the average transfer amount (in MU) over four transfer decisions per investor. a, Relative frequency of investors’ average transfers in oxytocin (filled bars) and placebo (open bars) groups in the trust experiment (n ¼ 58). Subjects given oxytocin show significantly higher transfer levels. b, Relative frequency of investors’ average transfers in oxytocin (filled bars) and placebo (open bars) groups in the risk experiment (n ¼ 61). Subjects in the oxytocin and the placebo group show statistically identical transfer levels. Table 1 | Median and average transfer behaviour of investors Trust experiment Risk experiment Oxytocin group Placebo group Oxytocin group Placebo group Mean average transfer (MU) 9.6 8.1 7.5 7.5 Median average transfer (MU) 10 8 8 8 Standard deviation of transfers (MU) 2.8 3.1 3.3 3.4
  • 51. Number of observations 29 29 31 30 LETTERS NATURE|Vol 435|2 June 2005 674 © 2005 Nature Publishing Group that the effect of oxytocin on human trust is not caused by non- specific, psychotropic effects of oxytocin. What mechanisms might be involved in generating the effect of oxytocin on trusting behaviour? One possibility is that oxytocin causes a general increase in prosocial inclinations. This implies that oxytocin should affect not only the prosocial behaviour of the investors but also that of the trustees. We would therefore predict that those trustees who are given oxytocin should make higher back transfers at any given level than the trustees who received placebo. However, trustees given oxytocin do not show more trustworthy behaviour (Fig. 3). At every positive transfer level (4, 8 or 12 MU), their back transfers are statistically indistinguishable from those of placebo trustees (Mann Whitney U-tests; P . 0.243, two-sided tests for each positive transfer level). Thus, oxytocin does not increase the general inclination to behave prosocially. Rather, oxytocin specifically affects the trusting behaviour of investors.
  • 52. We hypothesize that the differing effect of oxytocin on the behaviour of investors and trustees is related to the fact that investors and trustees face rather different situations. Specifically, investors have to make the first step; they have to ‘approach’ the trustee by transferring money. In contrast, the trustees can condition their behaviour on the basis of the investors’ actions. Thus, the psychology of trust is important for investors, whereas the psychology of strong reciprocity27 is relevant for trustees. The fact that oxytocin affects subjects’ approach or trust behaviour, but not their degree of reciprocity, is in agreement with animal studies. There is substantial evidence that oxytocin promotes prosocial approach behaviour by inhibiting defensive behaviours6,13, but there is no evidence that oxytocin affects reciprocity in animals. A second mechanism behind the effect of oxytocin on trust could be based on subjects’ beliefs. Oxytocin might render subjects more optimistic about the likelihood of a good outcome. In order to address this question, we measured the investor’s subjective expec- tation about the trustee’s back transfer after every transfer decision. A Mann-Whitney U-test indicates that these expectations do not differ significantly between oxytocin and placebo groups at every
  • 53. feasible positive transfer level (P . 0.357, two-sided tests at transfer levels of 4, 8 or 12 MU). Thus, the investors given oxytocin show more trusting behaviour but do not hold significantly different beliefs about the trustworthiness of others. Moreover, oxytocin does not affect investors’ beliefs about the likelihood of a good outcome in the risk experiment (P . 0.128, two-sided Mann Whitney U-tests for transfer levels of 4, 8 or 12 MU). Finally, there is the possibility that oxytocin helps subjects to overcome their betrayal aversion in social interactions. This expla- nation is consistent with the differing effects of oxytocin across the trust and the risk experiments, and is further supported by the fact that investors faced a considerable betrayal risk. An increase in the transfer level from 4 or 8 MU to 12 MU decreased the investor’s average payoff slightly, whereas it increased the objective risk of very low back transfers by the trustee. However, betrayal aversion alone cannot explain why investors given oxytocin make higher transfers in the trust experiment compared with the risk experiment, because betrayal is impossible in the risk experiment. The higher transfers in the trust experiment can be reconciled with betrayal aversion if one acknowledges that investors’ behaviour in the trust experiment is also likely to be driven by the motive to increase the available amount for
  • 54. distribution between the two players28. As this motive cannot operate in the risk experiment, it can only increase transfers levels in the trust experiment. Our interpretation of oxytocin’s effect on trust in terms of betrayal aversion may be seen in the light of animal studies indicating that increased availability of oxytocin in the central nervous system facilitates approach behaviour, by linking the over- coming of social avoidance with the activation of brain circuits implicated in reward (for example, the nucleus accumbens)12,15. The ubiquity of trusting behaviour is perhaps one of the dis- tinguishing features of the human species. An element of trust characterizes almost all human social interactions. Here we have sought to examine the effect of oxytocin on trust in humans. Research in non-human mammals suggests that oxytocin has a key role in social attachment and affiliation. We find that intranasal administration of oxytocin causes a substantial increase in trusting behaviour. Subjects given oxytocin seem better able to overcome trust obstacles such as betrayal aversion. Of course, this finding could be misused to induce trusting behaviours that selfish actors sub- sequently exploit. However, our findings may also have positive clinical implications for patients with mental disorders that are associated with social dysfunctions (for example, social phobia or autism). In particular, social phobia ranks as the third most common mental health disorder and is characterized by marked social
  • 55. deficits, including persistent fear and avoidance of social interactions. Thus, our results might lead to fertile research on the role of oxytocin in several mental health disorders with major public health significance. METHODS Subjects. A total of 194 healthy male students (mean age ^ s.d., 22.0 ^ 3.4 yr) from different universities in Zurich participated in the study. The trust experiment had 128 participants, and 66 subjects participated in the risk experiment. Exclusion criteria for participation were significant medical or psychiatric illness, medication, smoking more than 15 cigarettes per day, and drug or alcohol abuse. Subjects were instructed to abstain from food and drink (other than water) for 2 h before the experiment, and from alcohol, smoking and caffeine for 24 h before the experiment. Participants were informed at the time of recruitment that the experiment would evaluate the effects of a hormone on decision making. In total, 16 individuals out of the original sample of 194 were excluded because of incorrect substance administration (7 in the trust experi- ment, 5 in the risk experiment) or their stated disbelief that the opponent in the trust game was actually a human being (4 participants). The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Zurich.
  • 56. All subjects gave written, informed consent before participation. Substance administration. Subjects received a single intranasal dose of 24 IU oxytocin (Syntocinon-Spray, Novartis; 3 puffs per nostril, each with 4 IU oxytocin) or placebo 50 min before the start of the trust or the risk experiment. Subjects were randomly assigned to the oxytocin or placebo group (double- blind, placebo-controlled study design). In order to avoid any subjective substance effects (for example, olfactory effects) other than those caused by oxytocin, the placebo contained all inactive ingredients except for the neuropeptide. Behavioural experiment and questionnaires. After substance administration, Figure 3 | Average back transfer of trustees to their investors. The graph shows the average back transfer by trustees for different levels of investor transfer in the oxytocin and placebo groups. The dotted line shows the level of the back transfer necessary to achieve payoff equality between the investor and the trustee. The dashed line shows a level of back transfer equal to the investor’s transfer to the trustee. The trustees’ back transfers are on average slightly higher than the amount sent by the investor. Trustees in
  • 57. both treatment groups make higher back transfers in response to higher original investor transfer levels. However, there is no statistically significant difference in back transfers between subjects in the oxytocin and the placebo groups. NATURE|Vol 435|2 June 2005 LETTERS 675 © 2005 Nature Publishing Group subjects completed questionnaires on a computer to measure demographic items and psychological characteristics. Owing to the crucial role of the social environment in triggering behavioural effects of oxytocin (as shown in animal research)13,29, subjects were asked to wait in the rest area while the next part of the experiment was prepared. During this 5-min waiting period, subjects were seated at different tables. Subjects at the same table could talk to each other, but at the beginning of the experiment they were informed that they would not be interacting with those subjects who sat at the same table. When subjects re- entered the laboratory for both experiments, they received written instructions
  • 58. (available from the authors on request) explaining the payoff structure of the experiment and the private payment procedure at the end of the experiment. Subjects were randomly and anonymously assigned to the role of investor or trustee in the trust experiment, and did not know the identity of the persons with whom they were matched. After subjects had read the instructions in each experiment, we checked whether they understood the payoff structure by means of several hypothetical examples. All subjects (with one exception) answered the control questions correctly. One subject did not answer the control questions correctly and was excluded from the data set (this subject also did not apply the substance correctly). In addition, subjects received an oral summary of the instructions. Each subject in the trust experiment made four decisions in the same player role while paired with different, randomly selected interaction partners. No pair of subjects interacted twice. Subjects in the role of the investor received no feedback about the trustee’s decision between the different interactions. After every transfer decision, each investor was asked about his belief with regard to the expected back transfer from the trustee. Notably, trust levels were statistically constant across the four decisions. There is no time trend in investors’ decisions
  • 59. in either the oxytocin or the placebo group. In the risk experiment, everything was identical to the trust experiment, except that all subjects played the role of an investor who could transfer 0, 4, 8, or 12 MU into a project rather than to a trustee. In particular, an investor’s payoff risk (that is, the distribution of payoffs) in the risk experiment was identical to that in the trust experiment at any feasible transfer level. To measure alterations in the psychological state of subjects throughout the course of the experiment, we assessed their mood and calmness at the beginning of the experiment (before substance administration) and immediately before the trust experiment or the risk experiment, by means of a suitable questionnaire26. All decisions in the experiments and the answers to the questionnaires were entered on a computer using z-Tree software30. Subjects received a flat fee of 80 Swiss francs for participation in the experiment; each MU earned in the trust and the risk experiment was worth 0.40 Swiss francs. Received 20 April; accepted 5 May 2005. 1. Luhmann, N. Trust and Power (Wiley, New York, 1979). 2. Coleman, J. Foundations of Social Theory 91–-118 (Belknap Press of Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1990). 3. Arrow, K. Gifts and exchanges. Phil. Publ. Affairs 1, 343–-
  • 60. 362 (1972). 4. Knack, S. & Keefer, P. Does social capital have an economic payoff? A cross- country investigation. Q. J. Econ. 112, 1251–-1288 (1997). 5. Zak, P. J. & Knack, S. Trust and growth. Econ. J. 111, 295–- 321 (2001). 6. Carter, C. S. Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779–-818 (1998). 7. Uvnas-Moberg, K. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 819–- 835 (1998). 8. Insel, T. R. & Young, L. J. The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2, 129–-136 (2001). 9. Landgraf, R. & Neumann, I. D. Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: a dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25, 150–-176 (2004). 10. Huber, D., Pierre, V. & Ron, S. Vasopressin and oxytocin excite distinct neuronal populations in the central amygdala. Science 308, 245– -248 (2005). 11. Carter, C. S., Altemus, M. & Chrousos, G. P. Neuroendocrine and emotional changes in the post-partum period. Prog. Brain Res. 133, 241–- 249 (2001).
  • 61. 12. Young, L. J., Lim, M. M., Gingrich, B. & Insel, T. R. Cellular mechanisms of social attachment. Horm. Behav. 40, 133–-138 (2001). 13. Pedersen, C. A. Oxytocin control of maternal behavior. Regulation by sex steroids and offspring stimuli. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 807, 126–- 145 (1997). 14. Heinrichs, M., Neumann, I. & Ehlert, U. Lactation and stress: protective effects of breast-feeding in humans. Stress 5, 195–-203 (2002). 15. Insel, T. R. & Shapiro, L. E. Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 89, 5981–-5985 (1992). 16. Born, J. et al. Sniffing neuropeptides: a transnasal approach to the human brain. Nature Neurosci. 5, 514–-516 (2002). 17. Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C. & Ehlert, U. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 1389–-1398 (2003). 18. Heinrichs, M., Meinlschmidt, G., Wippich, W., Ehlert, U. & Hellhammer, D. H. Selective amnesic effects of oxytocin on human memory. Physiol. Behav. 83, 31–-38 (2004).
  • 62. 19. Camerer, C. & Weigelt, K. Experimental tests of a sequential equilibrium reputation model. Econometrica 56, 1–-36 (1988). 20. Fehr, E., Kirchsteiger, G. & Riedl, A. Does fairness prevent market clearing? An experimental investigation. Q. J. Econ. 108, 437–-459 (1993). 21. Berg, J., Dickhaut, J. & McCabe, K. Trust, reciprocity and social history. Games Econ. Behav. 10, 122–-142 (1995). 22. Bohnet, I. & Zeckhauser, R. Trust, risk and betrayal. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 55, 467–-484 (2004). 23. Holt, C. & Laury, S. Risk aversion and incentive effects. Am. Econ. Rev. 92, 1644–-1655 (2002). 24. Fehr, E. & Schmidt, K. M. A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Q. J. Econ. 114, 817–-868 (1999). 25. Hong, K. & Bohnet, I. Status and Distrust: the Relevance of Inequality and Betrayal Aversion (Working Paper RW04-041, Kennedy School, Harvard Univ., Cambridge, 2004). 26. Steyer, R., Schwenkmezger, P., Notz, P. & Eid, M. Der Mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF) [Multidimensional mood questionnaire] (Hogrefe, Göttingen, 1997).
  • 63. 27. Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R. & Fehr, E. Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 153–-172 (2003). 28. Engelmann, D. & Strobel, M. Inequality aversion, efficiency, and maximin preferences in simple distribution experiments. Am. Econ. Rev. 94, 857–-869 (2004). 29. Kendrick, K. M. et al. Neural control of maternal behaviour and olfactory recognition of offspring. Brain Res. Bull. 44, 383–-395 (1997). 30. Fischbacher, U. z-Tree. Zurich Toolbox for Readymade Economic Experiments (Working Paper No. 21, Institute for Empirical Research in Economics, Univ., Zurich, 1999). Acknowledgements This paper is part of the Research Priority Program ‘Foundations of Human Social Behaviour — Altruism versus Egoism’ at the University of Zurich. Financial support from the MacArthur Foundation (Network on Economic Environments and the Evolution of Individual Preferences and Social Norms) and the Cogito Foundation is gratefully acknowledged. M.H. also acknowledges support from the Swiss National Science Foundation. We thank F. Heusi, A. Enzler, S. Gilomen, E. Götz, M. Koenigsberg, K. Korsunsky, S. Krammer, S. Lauber, P. Obrist-Rybar, M. Schellhammer, B. Studer, R. Stünzi
  • 64. and M. Trottmann for research assistance. Author Information Reprints and permissions information is available at npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions. The authors declare no competing financial interests. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to E.F. ([email protected]) and M.H. ([email protected]). LETTERS NATURE|Vol 435|2 June 2005 676 © 2005 Nature Publishing Group