SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Selecting A topic/Job Position
For the first part of the project, you will select a topic position
for either a Release of Information Associate
For the position you have selected, address the following in 2
paragraphs
· Brief description of the position (2 -3 sentences)
· Why did you select the position?
· What are the 3 major duties of the position that are most
essential to the role?
Article
Corresponding author:
John Heilmann, Department of Communication Sciences and
Disorders, Mail Stop #668, East Carolina
University, Greenville, NC 27858, USA
E-mail: [email protected]
Language Testing
27(4) 603–626
© The Author(s) 2010
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermission.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0265532209355669
http://ltj.sagepub.com
Sensitivity of narrative
organization measures using
narrative retells produced by
young school-age children
John Heilmann
East Carolina University, USA
Jon F. Miller
University of Wisconsin – Madison, USA
Ann Nockerts
University of Wisconsin – Madison, USA
Abstract
Analysis of children’s productions of oral narratives provides a
rich description of children’s oral
language skills. However, measures of narrative organization
can be directly affected by both
developmental and task-based performance constraints which
can make a measure insensitive
and inappropriate for a particular population and/or sampling
method. This study critically
reviewed four methods of evaluating children’s narrative
organization skills and revealed that the
Narrative Scoring Scheme (NSS) was the most developmentally
sensitive measure for a group
of 129 5–7-year-old children who completed a narrative retell.
Upon comparing the methods
of assessing narrative organization skills, the NSS was unique
in its incorporation of higher-level
narrative features and its scoring rules, which required
examiners to make subjective judgments
across seven aspects of the narrative process. The discussion
surrounded issues of measuring
children’s narrative organization skills and, more broadly,
issues surrounding sensitivity of criterion
referenced assessment measures.
Keywords
assessment, ceiling effects, narrative, language development,
oral language, psychometrics
604 Language Testing 27(4)
Introduction
Oral narrative skills in children
Assessment of children’s oral narratives is of significant
interest to researchers and
practitioners, as being a proficient narrator is an important skill
in the life of young
children. Oral narrative skills are a key component of most
school curricula. In the
USA, each state is required to develop guidelines describing
skills that need to be incor-
porated into the general curriculum. While oral language skills
are often not formally
assessed in the general classroom, they are a key component in
each state’s guidelines.
For example, the state of North Carolina requires teachers to
facilitate mastery of nar-
rative comprehension as well as effective ability to produce
narratives and the complex
language associated with a literate speaking style (North
Carolina Department of Public
Instruction, 2004). Oral narrative skills have been incorporated
into children’s curricula
for good reasons. Being a proficient narrator is a skill needed to
express one’s intentions
and effectively participate in classroom activities. In addition,
several decades of
research have documented the strong link between children’s
oral narrative skills and
broader curricular requirements.
Research on monolingual English-speaking children has
documented that chil-
dren’s oral narrative skills are predictive of later reading
outcomes (Bishop &
Edmundson, 1987; Dickinson & McCabe, 2001; Griffin,
Hemphill, Camp, & Wolf,
2004; Hemphill & Snow, 1996; Roth, Speece, Cooper, & de la
Paz, 1996; Snow, 1983;
Snow, Dickinson, Jennings, & Purves, 1991; Tabors, Snow, &
Dickinson, 2001).
Additional studies have documented that children’s early
narrative competence is
related to broader academic outcomes (e.g., Fazio, Naremore, &
Connell, 1996;
O’Neill, Pearce, & Pick, 2004). Fazio and colleagues found that
oral narrative skills
were one of the strongest predictors for whether or not a child
later required academic
remediation, while O’Neill et al. identified a strong relationship
between young
children’s oral narrative skills and later mathematical ability.
While these studies
documenting the relationship between oral narratives and
broader reading and aca-
demic outcomes are correlational and a causal relationship
cannot be assumed, there
is a general consensus in the field that oral narrative skills may
play a key role in
developing the foundation for higher level academic tasks.
It is only natural that oral narrative skills are of interest to
those working with
children who have language impairments (LI). Approximately
7% of monolingual
English-speaking children experience significant deficits in
their oral language skills
despite normal cognitive skills (Tomblin, Records, Buckwalter,
Zhang, Smith, &
O’Brien, 1997). Children with LI have substantial difficulty
producing fully coherent
oral narratives (Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Fey, Catts, Proctor-
Williams, Tomblin, &
Zhang, 2004; Gillam & Johnston, 1992; Newman & McGregor,
2006; Pearce, McCormack,
& James, 2003; Reilly, Losh, Bellugi, & Wulfeck, 2004).
Assessment of children’s
narratives can be an effective method of identifying the
presence of LI (Allen, Kertoy,
Sherblom, & Petit, 1994; Paul & Smith, 1993) and provides a
functional description of
children’s performance, which may assist in the development of
treatment goals (Miller,
Gillam, & Peña, 2001).
Heilmann et al. 605
Oral narratives in bilingual children
Clinicians, teachers, and researchers are also interested in better
understanding the oral
narrative skills of children learning a second language. Just as
with monolingual chil-
dren, there is a significant predictive relationship between oral
narrative skills and read-
ing outcomes in children learning a second language (August &
Shanahan, 2006; Miller,
Heilmann, Nockerts, Iglesias, Fabiano, & Francis, 2006; Oller
& Pearson, 2002). Miller
et al. identified that measures from oral narratives predicted
reading scores within and
across languages in young school-age English language learners
(ELL). Given the strong
relationship between oral narratives and reading, it is important
to ensure that young
ELLs have sufficient oral narrative skills. Because a
disproportionately high number of
ELLs have poor reading outcomes when compared to their
monolingual counterparts
(August & Hakuta, 1997), there is a need for a better
understanding of the relationship
between oral narrative skills and reading outcomes.
Numerous speech and language scholars have recommended
using oral narratives in
clinical assessments of bilingual children, given the naturalness
of the task (e.g., Fiestas
& Peña, 2004; Munoz, Gillam, Peña, & Gulley-Faehnle, 2003;
Uccelli & Páez, 2007).
The act of telling stories is universal across cultures and is an
important instrument for
transmitting information (Mandler, Scribner,Cole, & DeForest,
1980); the format of tell-
ing a story may be more familiar to children from different
cultures than formal language
testing. Analysis of oral narratives across multiple languages
can provide an estimate of
relative proficiency in each language spoken and may assist in
differentiating language
differences, where a children may have limited proficiency in
one of their languages
spoken, from global language disorders, where children
demonstrate marked deficits in
all languages spoken (Gutiérrez-Clellen, 2002; Rojas & Iglesias,
2009).
Criterion referenced assessment
Oral narrative analysis falls under the broader umbrella of
criterion referenced (CR)
assessment. The goal of CR assessment is to generate a detailed
description of a child’s
performance in a target domain (e.g., narrative organization)
and to identify if the child’s
performance meets developmental expectations (Linn &
Gronlund, 2000). While the
purpose of standardized tests is to maximize differences
between individuals, CR assess-
ments afford clinicians the opportunity to obtain more detailed
data on specific skill
areas (Bachman, 2000). While standardized language tests
commonly evaluate limited
sets of skills that have been stripped of their communicative
context (Marquardt &
Gillam, 1999), most CR oral language assessments, including
narrative language sample
analysis, examine real-life communication situations and do not
rely on assessment of
decontextualized language skills.
Because CR assessments can generate rich descriptive data, they
are particularly use-
ful when describing the profile of a child exhibiting language
learning difficulties. While
children with LI share the same underlying characteristic (i.e.,
substantial difficulty with
language), there is considerable heterogeneity in the patterns of
difficulties experienced
by these children (Rapin & Allen, 1983; Tomblin, Zhang,
Weiss, Catts, & Ellis Weismer,
606 Language Testing 27(4)
2004). Most standardized language tests provide subtest scores
that would appear to be
useful for describing children’s profiles (e.g., receptive
vocabulary; expressive syntax).
However, the measurement properties of subtests prohibit use
for providing accurate
descriptions of performance patterns; subtest scores are
generated from a relatively small
number of items, making them unreliable for interpretation on
their own (McCauley &
Swisher, 1984). In order to fully identify a child’s pattern of
language performance,
evaluations should use detailed CR assessments to identify
children’s strength and weak-
ness. For example, comprehensive assessment of children’s oral
narrative skills may
reveal relative strengths and weaknesses in using appropriately
complex vocabulary and
syntax, correctly referencing the characters, providing sufficient
description of the major
events, organizing the events of the story, and making the story
interesting to the listener.
Such assessment can provide a clearer understanding of the
nature of a child’s language
difficulties and may assist in the development of treatment
goals.
While CR assessment tasks have many admirable properties,
they can be difficult to
implement, given the unstandardized nature of the task. With
the increased call for use of
naturalistic and CR assessment tasks, there is a need to
understand better the measure-
ment properties of these assessment tasks.
Difficulties associated with developing CR assessments
As stated by Laing and Kamhi (2003, p. 46), ‘criterion-
referenced measures are only as
good as the developmental data on which they are based.’ In the
field of language devel-
opment, decades of research have provided a rich and expansive
literature documenting
the complexities associated with acquiring human language.
Language use is complex
and difficult to measure, as it encompasses multiple aspects of
form, content, and use
(Bloom & Lahey, 1978). At any stage of development, different
aspects of children’s
vocabulary, grammar, and pragmatic skills are at very different
stages of development.
To complicate matters further, language use is highly influenced
by the broader speaking
context; a behavior that appears to be immature in one situation
can appear to be mas-
tered in another situation (Elman, 1995). When identifying
appropriate CR assessment
measures, clinicians and researchers must critically evaluate the
assessment tasks and
sampling procedures to determine whether they are
developmentally appropriate and
will generate sensitive measures for the target population.
Ollendick, Grills, and King
(2001) stated that developmentally appropriate measures should
measure meaningful
behaviors that are sensitive to the children’s developmental
levels and be responsive to
changes in the context that may affect the sensitivity of the
measure.
Measures from children’s productions of oral narratives are
highly sensitive to
changes in the sampling context and discourse demands of the
task. Peña et al. (2006)
described how increased amounts of support provided to the
child, or scaffolding, facili-
tates children’s production of oral narratives. Factors affecting
the complexity and over-
all quality of a child’s narrative production include whether or
not the child had heard the
story before (Ripich & Griffith, 1988; Schneider & Dubé,
2005), how many times the
child had heard the story (Goodsitt, Raitan, & Perlmutter, 1988;
Martinez & Roser,
1985), familiarity with the events depicted in the story (Fivush,
1984; Hudson & Shapiro,
Heilmann et al. 607
1991), complexity of the story (Heilmann, Miller, Iglesias, &
Francis, 2009), and the
types of directions and expectations provided to the child (de
Temple, Wu, & Snow,
1991). For example, if a child is unfamiliar with a story, he or
she may have substantial
difficulty in organizing the complex events and including more
nuanced points of view.
If, however, a child is familiar with a story and is asked to
relate the story in a complex
manner, he or she will likely produce a longer, more detailed
story. Such variations can
present a problem for measurement of children’s narrative
organization skills.
Measuring children’s narrative organization skills
The ability to produce a coherent narrative is a complex
linguistic task that requires nar-
rators to plan and execute their production of the story’s
plotline by using appropriate
vocabulary, grammar, and syntax. Studies examining the
development of narrative have
identified that all stories possess the same underlying
components, or story grammar
(Stein & Glenn, 1979). The story grammar literature has
proposed that all stories contain
a setting and episode system, which includes an initiating event
or problem, a reaction to
that problem, various attempts at resolving the problem, a
conclusion, and resolution. All
stories use some kind of combination of these story grammar
components. Developmental
studies have revealed that the acquisition of narrative
proficiency is a slow process, which
emerges in the preschool years and is not fully developed until
adulthood, with some
adults never becoming fully proficient narrators (Berman &
Slobin, 1994). Analysis of
children’s narrative organization skills has been described in
several clinical texts (Hughes,
McGillivray, & Schmidek, 1997; Strong, 1998), but the
measurement properties of the
various scoring procedures have not been empirically tested
with large samples.
Upon developing a series of narrative databases, the Language
Analysis Lab at the
University of Wisconsin-Madison was interested in identifying
a developmentally sensi-
tive narrative organization measure that that could be used on a
corpus of narrative retells
produced by children using the wordless picture book Frog,
Where are You? (Mayer,
1969). We first looked to the literature for sensitive narrative
organization measures for
this corpus and identified studies measuring narrative
organization in children who pro-
duced Frog, Where are You? The review was limited to this
specific story because it has
been extensively reported in the literature, follows a
prototypical story sequence, and
was to be used for several projects completed in the Language
Analysis Lab, including
the data summarized in this study.
The first major method for measuring children’s narrative
organization skills identi-
fied whether or not children included specific plotlines and
themes (i.e., plot and theme
analysis). When applying plot and theme analyses, examiners
developed a coding scheme
using the key story grammar elements for the target story. This
approach allowed exam-
iners to develop binary decision schemes that identified the
presence or absence of spe-
cific story grammar components. Children who produced a
greater number of plotlines
and themes were thought to have more advanced narrative skills
(e.g., Berman, 1988;
Botting, 2002; Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Miles & Chapman,
2002; Norbury & Bishop,
2003; Reilly, Losh, Bellugi, & Wulfeck, 2004). An example of a
plot and theme analysis
is provided in Appendix A.
608 Language Testing 27(4)
The second major class of narrative organization measures
relied on holistic judgments
of children’s narrative proficiency (Applebee, 1978; Hedberg &
Westby, 1993; Stein,
1988). While the text-level analyses also documented children’s
production of story gram-
mar components, they were not measured by counting the
presence or absence of specific
plotlines and themes. Rather, these measures required holistic
judgments by the examiner
to rate the quality and developmental level of the narrative. Two
contemporary studies
used the text-level measures of narrative organization for
analysis of Frog, Where are
You? Manhardt and Rescorla (2002) converted Applebee’s
categorical levels to an ordinal
scale to assess narratives produced by 8–9-year-old children
with histories of language
delay, while Pearce, McCormack, and James (2003) used Stein’s
scoring scheme to assess
narratives produced by children of 5 ½ years of age. A summary
and brief description of
these two holistic narrative scoring procedures is provided in
Appendix A.
Several of the reported studies compared performance on the
story grammar measures
across clinical groups with varying results. Reilly et al. (2004)
documented that their
story grammar analysis was sensitive to both age and group
differences in four groups of
children of between 7 and 9 years of age (children with specific
language impairment,
early focal brain injury, Williams syndrome, and typical
developmental histories). Pearce
et al. (2003) found significant differences in performance
between children with lan-
guage impairment and typically developing children at 5 ½
years of age. Boudreau and
Hedberg (1999), on the other hand, did not find any differences
between typically devel-
oping children and children with specific language impairment
on their narrative organi-
zation measure despite significant differences on each
additional measure collected from
the children’s narrative productions. McCabe and Rollins (1994)
also noted that the nar-
rative organization skills of children with language impairment
varied widely across
studies and attributed this variability to ‘the use of insensitive
means of scoring narra-
tives’ (p. 47). Further examination of the literature revealed that
these existing story
grammar measures may be too easy and potentially insensitive
for preschool and young
school-age children. For the plot and theme measures, Reilly et
al. documented that typi-
cally developing 7–9-year-old children, on average, produced
95% (11.4/12) of the plot-
lines and themes, while Boudreau and Hedberg documented that
their group of 5-year-old
children produced 78% (4.7/6) of the plotlines and themes. On
average, high success
rates were also noted for the text-level measures, with a group
of 8-year-old children
scoring 3.3/5 on the Applebee measure (Manhardt & Rescorla,
2002) and 5–6-year-old
children scoring 9/11 on the Stein measure (Pearce et al., 2003).
In identifying the best story grammar measure for analyzing
retells of a wordless
picture book, the potential sensitivity issue was compounded by
the sampling context.
Our corpus of narratives was collected using the retell
procedure, whereas most studies
from the literature did not provide an initial model of the target
story. Having children
retell a story provides a model story and assists children in
developing an understanding
of the story’s structure. Narrative retells have been shown to
contain more information
and incorporate a greater number of episodes than productions
of spontaneous narratives
(Ripich & Griffith, 1988; Schneider & Dubé, 2005). Our goal in
using the retell procedure
was to ensure that the samples represented the children’s best
narrative productions.
However, we anticipated that a simple story grammar analysis
would be ineffective for this
corpus of retells given that children include a greater amount of
information in their oral
Heilmann et al. 609
productions in the retell condition. Thus, we decided that
existing narrative organization
measures may not be appropriate and developed a new scale to
allow for more sensitive
measurement of narrative proficiency.
The Narrative Scoring Scheme: A comprehensive measure
of narrative organization skills
The Narrative Scoring Scheme (NSS) was developed by the
Language Analysis Lab at the
University of Wisconsin – Madison as a sensitive measure of
children’s overall narrative
organization skills. To make the NSS more sensitive for a wider
range of ages and for sam-
pling contexts that provide scaffolding (i.e., the narrative retell
procedure), two modifica-
tions were made from the other narrative organization measures.
First, the literature was
reviewed to identify later developing narrative organization
features that go beyond simple
story grammar analyses. Second, scoring procedures were
critiqued to identify methods
that may be more sensitive when evaluating more advanced
narrative productions.
In addition to incorporating basic story grammar features, fully
proficient narrators use
specific types of language features that define a literate style of
speaking (Bamberg &
Damrad-Frye, 1991). One way narrators use a literate style of
speaking is through the use of
metacognitive verbs, which include verbs used to describe the
characters’ thoughts and men-
tal states (e.g., think and know) and metalinguistic verbs, which
include words used to
describe characters’ speech and dialogue (e.g., say and talk; see
Nippold, 2007 and Westby,
2005 for a review). In their analysis of oral productions of Frog,
Where are You?, Bamberg
& Damrad-Frye contrasted productions from novice narrators
(i.e., young children) to those
from experienced narrators (i.e., older children and adults) and
found that use of abstract
language, including metacognitive and metalinguistic verbs,
were the key features that char-
acterized the more sophisticated narrative productions and
assisted the older narrators in
organizing the hierarchical relationships between the events in
the story. They found that
these high-level language skills were first seen around five
years of age, were not consistently
used until the later school-age years, and continued to develop
through adulthood. Additional
studies have documented that these abstract language features,
collectively termed literate
language, emerged during the preschool years (Curenton &
Justice, 2004), were more consis-
tently used as children progressed through adolescence
(Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001;
Nippold, 2007; Pelligrini, Galda, Bartini, & Charak, 1998), and
were not consistently or
appropriately used by children with language impairment
(Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001).
Another high-level skill that is positively related to children’s
narrative organization
skills is the effective use of cohesive devices. Halliday and
Hasan (1976) characterized the
various ways that children maintain concepts across utterances
through use of sophisticated
linguistic procedures, including referential cohesion, which is
the way that speakers main-
tain appropriate reference throughout the story with correct use
of nouns and pronouns,
conjunctive cohesion, which is the way that speakers combine
phrases and sentences with
conjunctions and conjunctive phrases, and lexical cohesion,
which is the way that speakers
choose appropriate words to link concepts across phrases and
sentences. Children with
language impairment have much more difficulty producing
cohesive features than their
typically developing peers (Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Liles,
1985; Strong & Shaver, 1991).
610 Language Testing 27(4)
In addition to incorporating higher level narrative skills into the
NSS, we attempted to
increase the sensitivity of the measure by critically reviewing
the scoring procedures and
scaling of the measure. McFadden and Gillam (1996) compared
two methods of scoring
children’s narrative organization skills: discrete coding schemes
based on presence or
absence of story features and holistic ratings of children’s
narrative proficiency. They
found that the holistic ratings were superior to the discrete
coding schemes in discrimi-
nating between children who were typically developing and
children with language
impairment. These children were between the ages of 9;0 and
11;7. McFadden and
Gillam’s study demonstrated that holistic ratings can effectively
assess the inter-
utterance concepts and qualitative aspects of the story,
including the story’s sparkle (Peterson
& McCabe, 1983). To incorporate holistic ratings into a detailed
and descriptive rating
scale, the NSS requires examiners to make broad judgments
across the seven aspects of
narrative organization included in the scale.
The goal of this paper was to compare the measurement
properties of the NSS with
other methods of operationalizing narrative organization from
the literature. This study
provided a direct comparison of four separate scoring
techniques using the same pool of
transcripts and identified the measures that were most
developmentally appropriate. The
literature review revealed that many of the existing narrative
organization measures may
focus too much on early developing narrative skills, such as the
inclusion of key story
grammar components. We felt that incorporating higher level
narrative skills would
make the scale developmentally appropriate for young school-
age children who pro-
duced narratives with scaffolding from the retell procedure. In
addition, the NSS required
examiners to provide a level of holistic judgment to assess the
qualitative aspects of the
story. To assess the sensitivity of the measures, the distribution
of scores was evaluated
for each narrative scoring technique to determine which
measures were developmentally
appropriate for the participants and sampling context used in the
present study. This
study addressed the following question: Is the distribution of
scores from the NSS less
skewed than scores from three traditional narrative organization
measures when applied
to narrative retells produced by 5–7-year-old children?
Method
Narrative language samples were collected from 129 typically
developing children
between 5;0 and 7;0 years of age. The children were recruited
from public schools in the
San Diego County and El Cajon County school districts. The
school speech-language
pathologist (SLP) and the children’s classroom teachers
confirmed that the children were
typically developing by reviewing all academic records and
summative assessments. To
be included in the study, children had to be performing at grade
level and could not be
receiving special education or speech/language services. See
Table 1 for a summary of
the participants’ demographic data. The narrative samples were
collected by a practicing
SLP working in the schools under the direction of a project
coordinator. The SLPs met
with the project coordinator for three sessions to learn the
protocol, where they had the
opportunity to develop an understanding of the protocol, ask for
clarification, and prac-
tice administering the assessment.
Heilmann et al. 611
The children completed the narrative task in the retell
condition, where the examiner
read the target story aloud to the child, cued the child to follow
along with the pictures in
a wordless picture book (Frog, Where are You?; Mayer, 1969),
and then asked the child to
retell the story to the examiner. This procedure was adapted
from the Strong Narrative
Assessment Procedure (Strong, 1998). The children’s language
samples were digitally
recorded and later transcribed by research assistants (RA) at the
University of Wisconsin
– Madison. Each RA had at least 10 hours of training in
completing transcription of chil-
dren’s oral language samples. The narrative samples were first
transcribed using the
Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT; Miller &
Iglesias, 2008). After tran-
scribing the sample, the RA reviewed the written transcript and
completed the NSS scor-
ing (see Appendix B for scoring rules).
The NSS is divided into seven sections that assess seven
different aspects of narrative
organization. Three of the sections were modeled after the
original story grammar pro-
posals: introduction, conflict resolution, and conclusion. Use of
literate language skills
were evaluated with the mental states and character
development sections. The final two
sections evaluated children’s cohesion skills and included
referencing and cohesion.
Within each section, scores of 1 were administered if the child
demonstrated immature
performance, scores of 3 if the child demonstrated emerging
skills, and scores of 5 if
proficient performance was noted. After completing each
section of the NSS, the scores
were added together to generate a total NSS score, which could
range from 0 to 35.
To compare performance on the different narrative organization
measures, the NSS
scores were compared to scores using three additional narrative
organization measures
from the literature. For the plot and theme approach, the
protocol described in Reilly
et al. (2004) was chosen because it provided a balanced
combination of the core plot
components and key embedded episodes. For the text-level
measures, we used Manhardt
and Rescorla’s (2002) ordinal adaptation of Applebee’s (1978)
narrative levels and
Pearce et al.’s (2003) ordinal adaptation of Stein’s (1988)
narrative levels. See Appendix
A for a description of the scoring procedures.
Each of the narrative organization analyses was scored by a
trained research assistant.
To document inter-rater agreement, 20% of the narrative
organization analyses were inde-
pendently coded by the first author. Krippendorff alpha values
(Krippendorff, 1980) were
calculated with ordinal scaling to determine the level of
agreement between transcribers.
Table 1. Demographic information for the 129 children
completing the narrative retell task
Grade (n) Gender (n) Maternal education (in years)
Race/ethnicity (n)
Preschool: 3 Female: 69 M = 14.4 White: 87
Kindergarten: 79 Male: 60 SD = 2.5 Hispanic: 16
1st Grade: 47 Range = 9–20 Othera: 15
African American: 7
No data: 4
Note: Ethnicity data collected for children who were Hispanic
or Latino. Race data provided for all children
who were non-Hispanic or Latino.
aOther races/ethnicities include Arabic (2), Chinese (3),
Japanese (2), Korean (1), Filipino (5), Portuguese (1),
and Samoan (1).
612 Language Testing 27(4)
Krippendorff established the following benchmarks for alpha
values: ≥0.80 is adequate and
values between 0.67 and 0.80 are acceptable for exploratory
research and drawing tentative
conclusions. The following alpha values were calculated from
samples used in the present
study: NSS = 0.79, Plot & Theme = 0.79, Applebee = 0.61, and
Stein = 0.69.
Results
The normality of the distributions was compared using skewness
and kurtosis statistics
(see Coolican, 2004, for a review). The skewness statistic
identifies if there is an unequal
distribution that goes towards the floor or ceiling. Skewness
measures of zero indicate a
perfectly normal distribution while skewness values exceeding
|0.8| have been described
as ‘noticeably skewed’ (Bourque & Clark, 1992, p. 69). If the
distribution of scores is
negatively skewed, then most of the scores are approaching
mastery levels, reflecting
that the measure was too easy for the child. The kurtosis
statistic identifies how closely
variables are bunched together; platykurtic distributions have
scores that are spread out
and leptokurtic distributions have scores that are bunched
closely together (Coolican,
2004). Skewness and kurtosis statistics were generated for all
four scoring procedures
using SPSS. The traditional kurtosis statistics were converted so
that they were on the
same scale as the skewness statistics, with zero indicating a
perfectly normal distribu-
tion. While these analyses do not generate tests of statistical
significance, they do allow
comparisons to be drawn regarding the relative skewness and
kurtosis across different
measures. A final method for documenting the relative
distribution of scores was to
establish a score representing near-mastery performance and
identify the percentage of
the participants that scored at or above that criteria, similar to a
procedure employed by
Helms et al. (2004). For the present study, achieving at or above
90% correct was con-
sidered near-mastery performance and therefore a ceiling effect.
Table 2 summarizes the scores acquired from each scoring
scheme, showing the full
range of possible scores, the range acquired from this initial
sample, the sample means
and standard deviations, the skewness and kurtosis statistics,
and the percentage of the
sample who scored greater than 90% correct on the measure.
The skewness statistics for
all of the narrative measures were negative, demonstrating that
the distribution of scores
were more concentrated towards the ceiling. Skewness for the
NSS was considerably
lower than the additional measures from the literature.
Furthermore, skewness statistics
exceeded |0.8| for the plot and theme, Applebee, and Stein
measures, demonstrating that
these measures were ‘noticeably skewed’ (Bourque & Clark,
1992). The kurtosis values
for each of the narrative measures were greater than zero,
documenting that the narrative
measures were generating leptokurtic distributions that had a
relatively large proportion
of scores surrounding the mean. Again, the kurtosis values were
noticeably greater for
the three measures from the literature (0.9–1.1) than scores
from the NSS (0.5), showing
that there was a more restricted distribution for the plot and
theme, Applebee, and Stein
measures. The final analysis identified the number of
participants who scored close to
ceiling, with the criteria set at a score equal to or greater than
90% correct. None of the
children scored above 90% on the NSS, while 30–35% of the
children scored above 90%
on the three measures from the literature. Histograms were
generated for each of the
Heilmann et al. 613
narrative measures to visualize the distribution for each
narrative organization measure
(see Figure 1). Examination of Figure 1 illustrates the heavy
weighting of scores near
ceiling and around the mean for the three measures from the
literature and the relatively
normal distribution of the NSS.
Sample Narratives
To further illustrate differences across the scoring techniques,
three samples were
selected and are available in Appendix C. The samples were
modified from their original
coded format to assist with readability. All codes required for
SALT analyses were
Table 2. Children’s performance across four separate narrative
organization measures
Possible range Sample range Sample Mean (SD) Skewness
Kurtosis > 90%
Plot & Theme 0–12 4–12 8.8 (2.0) −1.0 1.1 35%
Applebee 0–5 2–6 4.0 (1.0) −1.1 0.9 30%
Stein 0–11 2–10 6.8 (2.4) −1.0 0.9 30%
NSS 0–35 11–26 20.1 (3.2) −0.5 0.5 0%
Note: > 90% signifies the proportion of the sample who scored
above 90% correct on the measure.
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
Plot & Theme
F
re
q
u
e
n
c
y
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
0 1 2 3 4 5
Applebee
F
re
q
u
e
n
c
y
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Stein
F
re
q
u
e
n
c
y
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
0 3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 33
NSS
F
re
q
u
e
n
c
y
Figure 1. Histograms depicting the distribution of scores using
four narrative structure coding
schemes
614 Language Testing 27(4)
removed, including word errors, utterance errors, mazes (which
include reduplications,
reformulations, and false starts), and slashes used to identify
bound morphemes and
contractions. While these codes are required for syntactic and
semantic analysis of the
narrative samples, they are not necessary to accurately analyze
narrative structure.
Samples 1, 2, and 3 reflect a range of performance on the
narrative retell task.
By simply reading the transcripts, even an untrained reader with
no experience in
analyzing children’s oral language can identify that sample 3 is
much poorer than the
other two samples. It is considerably shorter and more difficult
to follow than samples 1
and 2. Many readers will also note that sample 1 is more
advanced than sample 2. In
story 1, the narrator provided more detail, better described each
of the events, and used
complex language to make the story interesting for the listener.
Upon comparing the dif-
ferent narrative organization measures, each measure was
effective in identifying that
story 3 was less complete and more poorly organized than
stories 1 and 2. However, the
plot and theme and two holistic narrative organization measures
did not distinguish sto-
ries 1 and 2 from each other. Rather, the NSS was the only
measure sensitive enough to
reveal differences between the productions.
Discussion
Sensitivity analyses
This study compared the measurement properties of the NSS to
three other narrative
organization measures from the literature. Each of the 129
transcripts was scored using
the four scoring schemes summarized in Appendix A. Twenty
percent of the transcripts
were recoded by a second member of the research team to
document the level of agree-
ment between transcribers. Krippendorff alpha coefficients were
equivalent for the NSS
and plot and theme measures (α = 0.79), while agreement values
for the Applebee and
Stein measures (α = 0.61 and 0.69, respectively) were notably
lower. We anticipated that
the highest level of inter-rater agreement would occur with the
plot and theme measures,
as the coding task was limited to identification of specific story
components. We were
impressed that the coding procedures of the NSS facilitated
comparable agreement val-
ues when compared to the plot and theme measures and
strikingly higher agreement
levels than the two text-level scoring scales.
Upon comparing the distribution of scores across the four
scoring schemes, scores from
the plot and theme and text-level narrative organization
measures were noticeably more
skewed and leptokurtic than scores from the NSS,
demonstrating that the majority of the
scores were closely bunched together near the ceiling.
Furthermore, approximately one
third of the children demonstrated near-mastery performance
and were approaching ceiling
on the measures from the literature, while none of the children
approached ceiling on the
NSS. Upon scoring the narratives with the NSS, we observed a
wider distribution of scores,
a relatively normal distribution across participants, and ample
room for measurement of
children with higher and lower narrative skills. We hypothesize
that there were two reasons
for the increased sensitivity of the NSS. First, by incorporating
children’s use of literate
language and cohesion, the NSS measured skills that were later
developing and/or were
Heilmann et al. 615
present when children receive scaffolding. Second, by utilizing
examiner judgment, the
NSS was able to tap into the perceptual aspects of the narrative
process (i.e., ‘sparkle’) that
are missed by discrete scoring schemes. Use of a more sensitive
scale, such as the NSS,
may address the sensitivity issue described by McCabe and
Rollins (1994) and provide a
more sensitive measure that can distinguish between typically
developing children and
children with language impairment (cf., Boudreau and Hedberg,
1999).
Because children’s narrative skills continue to develop through
the school years, a
more sensitive measure that assesses higher level narrative
features allows clinicians and
researchers to document narrative organization skills in older
school-age children. In the
present study, the child with the strongest narrative
organization skills received a score
of 26 on the NSS, while a perfect score would be 35. Based on
these data, there are an
additional 9 points that can be earned on the NSS for even the
best narrators. We predict
that NSS scores continue to increase in older children with
stronger language skills and
continue to develop additional reference databases to evaluate a
wider range of ages.
It is important to note that the data from the present study were
potentially constrained
by the limited sampling context. The data were acquired from a
relatively short and sim-
ple story. The extensive use of Frog, Where are You? in the
literature has greatly increased
our understanding of how children develop narrative
proficiency. However, additional
work is needed to document the effect of different stories on
measures of narrative orga-
nization. The plot and theme and text-level narrative
organization measures may be more
sensitive when children have the opportunity to produce longer
and more complex narra-
tives. From a clinical standpoint, however, increasing the story
length and complexity
increases the time requirements for elicitation and analysis of
the language samples. Our
goal was to use a story that could be collected and analyzed
quickly and efficiently.
Use of the NSS in research, educational, and clinical contexts
Research examining the mastery of oral narrative skills in
children can assist in better
understanding of the development of broader cognitive skills.
Studies of children’s
developing narrative have improved our understanding of the
development of cognitive
schemas; Berman and Slobin (1994) completed the most
comprehensive description of
the relationship between linguistic form and function. The study
of narrative develop-
ment has also shaped theories about the relationship between
socio-linguistic factors and
developing narrative competence (Eaton, Collis, & Lewis, 1995;
McCabe, 1997; Peterson
& McCabe, 2004; Quasthoff, 1997) as well as the relationship
between oral narratives
and general cognitive skills, such as working memory (e.g., van
den Broek, 1997).
Having a sensitive measure of children’s narrative organization
skills, such as the NSS,
is essential for documenting children’s narrative organization
skills and will facilitate
further advancements of our understanding of human cognition.
A sensitive and feasible measure of narrative organization, such
as the NSS, has
important educational policy implications. While oral language
skills are a key part of
most schools’ curricula, curriculum based assessment in the
general classroom has pri-
marily focused on documentation of children’s reading skills
(see Reschly, Busch, Betts,
Deno, & Long, 2009). Continuing to recognize the important
relationship between
616 Language Testing 27(4)
oral narrative skills and broader reading and academic
achievements will encourage
greater advocacy for regular assessment of children’s oral
narrative skills in the general
curriculum. However, for this to occur, teachers and
practitioners need to have sensitive
and efficient measures, such as the NSS.
The NSS can also be an important tool for clinicians working
with children with lan-
guage impairment and for those working with second language
learners. The composite
NSS score provides a single estimate of children’s overall
narrative competence that is
sensitive for young children producing narrative retells. Given
the flexibility of the mea-
sure, it is likely also appropriate to use with children producing
more complex and less
complex narratives than those produced by the children in this
study. NSS composite
scores may also be used to assist with the identification of
children experiencing lan-
guage learning difficulties. Several databases are available with
the SALT software that
allows clinicians to compare their clients’ performance to a
sample of typically develop-
ing speakers. One database summarizes NSS scores collected
from monolingual English-
speaking children producing narrative retells; the samples used
in this study are included
in that database. The second major database with NSS scores
was collected on a large
group of English language learners who produced narrative
retells in both English and
Spanish. In addition to a composite narrative score, the NSS
provides examiners with
estimates of children’s performance within seven different
aspects of the narrative pro-
cess. Examiners can examine the profile of performance across
each aspect of the NSS
to identify areas of relative strength and relative weakness,
which may assist with further
determining the nature of a child’s language impairment and
assist with the development
of treatment goals. Upon initiating treatment, the NSS can be
used to monitor children’s
progress during the intervention program.
Conclusion
Decades of research and clinical practice literature have
identified the limitations of norm
referenced testing and have identified CR assessment as a viable
alternative for overcom-
ing many of these shortcomings. CR assessments provide a
detailed description of chil-
dren’s performance when completing naturalistic and
meaningful tasks. With the call for
increased use of CR assessments, more rigorous testing of the
properties of these tasks
must be completed. When evaluating assessment measures,
factors influencing the devel-
opmental sensitivity of the measure should be considered,
including the linguistic features
under study and the scaling of the assessment measure. In this
paper, we reviewed four
different methods of measuring children’s narrative
organization skills and found that the
NSS was more sensitive than the three other measures for young
school-age children
completing a narrative retell procedure. These analyses
identified features that allowed for
more sensitive analysis of narrative organization skills,
including assessment of higher
level narrative concepts and incorporation of examiner
judgment across multiple narrative
features. These analyses also provided an example of methods
that can be used to evaluate
the measurement properties of additional CR assessment
procedures. Such analyses and
discussion are increasingly important as more clinicians are
implementing the CR assess-
ment into clinical practice.
Heilmann et al. 617
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Claudia Dunaway and the SLPs in the
San Diego and El Cajon school
districts for their assistance in collecting the narrative retell
data. We would also like to thank
Karen Andriacchi and all of the past and present members of the
language analysis lab who have
been dedicated to accurately transcribing and efficiently
organizing all of the language samples.
References
Allen, M. S., Kertoy, M. K., Sherblom, J. C., & Pettit, J. M.
(1994). Children’s narrative productions:
A comparison of personal event and fictional stories. Applied
Psycholinguistics, 15(2), 149–176.
Applebee, A. (1978). The child’s concept of story: Ages two to
seven. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
August, D., & Hakuta, K. (1997). Improving schooling for
language-minority children. Washing-
ton, DC: National Academy Press.
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.) (2006). Developing literacy
in second-language learners:
Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority
Children and Youth. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bachman, L. (2000). Modern language testing at the turn of the
century: Assuring that what we
count counts. Language Testing, 17(1), 1–42.
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to
provide evaluative comments: Further
explorations of children’s narrative competencies. Journal of
Child Language, 18(3), 689–710.
Berman, R. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative.
Discourse Processes, 11(4), 469–497.
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (Eds.) (1994). Relating events in
narrative: A crosslinguistic develop-
mental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bishop, D., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4-
year-olds: Distinguishing transient
from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech & Hearing
Disorders, 52(2), 156–173.
Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and
language disorders. New York: Wiley.
Botting, N. (2002). Narrative as a tool for the assessment of
linguistic and pragmatic impairments.
Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 18(1), 1–22.
Boudreau, D. M., & Hedberg, N. (1999). A comparison of early
literacy skills in children with
specific language impairment and typically developing peers.
American Journal of Speech-
Language Pathology, 8(3), 249–260.
Boudreau, D., & Hedberg, N. (1999). A comparison of early
literacy skills in children with specific
language impairment and their typically developing peers.
American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 8(3), 249–260.
Bourque, L., & Clark, V. (1992). Processing data: The survey
example. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Coolican, H. (2004). Research Methods and Statistics in
Psychology. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Curenton, S., & Justice, L. (2004). African American and
Caucasian preschoolers’ use of decon-
textualized language: Literate language features in oral
narratives. Language, Speech, and
Hearing Services in the Schools, 35(3), 240–253.
de Temple, J. M., Wu, H.-f., & Snow, C. E. (1991). Papa Pig
just left for Pigtown: Children’s oral and
written picture descriptions under varying instructions.
Discourse Processes, 14(4), 469–495.
618 Language Testing 27(4)
Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all
together: The multiple origins, skills,
and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning
Disabilities Research & Practice,
16(4), 186–202.
Eaton, J., Collis, G., & Lewis, V. (1999). Evaluative
explanations in children’s narratives of a video
sequence without dialogue. Journal of Child Language, 26(3),
699–720.
Elman, J. (1995). Language as a dynamical system. In R. Port &
T. van Gelder (Eds.), Mind as
motion: Exploring the dynamics of cognition (pp. 195–223).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fazio, B., Naremore, R., & Connell, P. (1996). Tracking
children from poverty at risk for specific
language impairment: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research,
39(3), 52–63.
Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B.,
& Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and
written story composition skills of children with language
impairment. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1301–1318.
Fiestas, C. E., & Peña, E. D. (2004). Narrative discourse in
bilingual children: Language and task
effects. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
35(2), 155–168.
Fivush, R. (1984). Learning about school: The development of
kindergartners’ school scripts.
Child Development, 55(5), 1697–1709.
Gillam, R. B., & Johnston, J. R. (1992). Spoken and written
language relationships in language/
learning-impaired and normally achieving school-age children.
Journal of Speech & Hearing
Research, 35(6), 1303–1315.
Goodsitt, J., Raitan, J. G., & Perlmutter, M. (1988). Interaction
between mothers and preschool chil-
dren when reading a novel and familiar book. International
Journal of Behavioral Development,
11(4), 489–505.
Greenhalgh, K., & Strong, C. (2001). Literate language features
in spoken narratives of children
with typical language and children with language impairment.
Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in Schools, 32(2), 114–125.
Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004).
Oral discourse in the preschool years
and later literacy skills. First Language, 24(71), 123–147.
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. (2002). Narratives in two languages:
Assessing performance of bilingual
children. Linguistics and Education, 13(2), 175–197.
Halliday, M., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London:
Longman.
Hedberg, N., and Westby, C. (1993). Analyzing storytelling
skills: Theory to practice. Tucson,
AZ: Communication Skill Builders.
Heilmann, J., Miller, J., Iglesias, A., & Francis, D. (2009,
June). Differences in oral narratives as a
function of story in bilingual children. Paper presented at the
Symposium on Research in Child
Language Disorders, Madison, WI.
Helms, J., Weichbold, V., Baumann, U., von Specht, H., Schon,
F., Muller, J., et al. (2004). Analysis
of ceiling effects occurring with speech recognition tests in
adult cochlear-implant patients.
Journal for Oto-Rhino-Laryngology and Its Related Specialties,
66(3), 130–135.
Hemphill, L., Snow, C., Olson, D. R., & Torrance, N. (1996).
Language and literacy development:
Discontinuities and differences. In D. R. Olson, and N.
Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of
education and human development: New models of learning,
teaching and schooling
(pp. 173–201). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R., McCabe, A., & Peterson, C.
(1991). From knowing to telling: The
development of children’s scripts, stories, and personal
narratives. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson
(Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 89–136). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Heilmann et al. 619
Hughes, D., McGillivray, L., & Schmidek, M. (1997). Guide to
narrative language. Austin, TX:
PRO-ED.
Krippendorff, K. (1980). Content analysis: An introduction to
its methodology. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage Publications.
Laing, S., & Kamhi, A. (2003). Alternative assessment of
language and literacy in culturally and linguis-
tically diverse populations. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in the Schools, 34(1), 44–55.
Liles, B. (1985). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and
language-disordered children. Journal
of Speech & Hearing Research, 28(1), 123–133.
Linn, R. L., & Gronlund, N. E. (2000). Measurement and
assessment in teaching (8th ed.). Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Mandler, J. M., Scribner, S., Cole, M., & DeForest, M. (1980).
Cross-cultural invariance in story
recall. Child Development, 51(1), 19–26.
Manhardt, J., & Rescorla, L. (2002). Oral narrative skills of late
talkers at ages 8 and 9. Applied
Psycholinguistics, 23(1), 1–21.
Marquardt, T., & Gillam, R. (1999). Assessment in
communication disorders: Some observations
on current issues. Language Testing, 16(3), 249–269.
Martinez, M., & Roser, N. (1985). Read it again: The value of
repeated readings during storytime.
The Reading Teacher, 38(8), 782–786.
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press.
McCabe, A. (1997). Developmental and cross-cultural aspects
of children’s narration. In G. Bamberg
(Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp. 137–174).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
McCabe, A., & Rollins, P. (1994). Assessment of preschool
narrative skills. American Journal of
Speech-Language Pathology, 3(1), 45–56.
McCauley, R., & Swisher, L. (1984). Use and misuse of norm-
referenced tests in clinical assess-
ment: A hypothetical case. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, 49(4), 34–42.
McFadden, T., & Gillam, R. (1996). An examination of the
quality of narratives produced by children
with language disorders. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in Schools, 27(1), 48–56.
Miles, S., & Chapman, R. S. (2002). Narrative content as
described by individuals with Down
syndrome and typically developing children. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing
Research, 45(1), 175–189.
Miller, L., Gillam, R., & Peña, E. (2001). Dynamic assessment
and intervention: Improving chil-
dren’s narrative skills. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.
Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano,
L., & Francis, D. (2006). Oral language
and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities
Research and Practice, 21(1), 30–43.
Miller, J. F. & Iglesias, A. (2008). Systematic Analysis of
Language Transcripts (Research Version
9.1) [Computer software]. Madison, WI: Language Analysis
Lab.
Muñoz, M. L., Gillam, R. B., Peña, E. D., & Gulley-Faehnle, A.
(2003). Measures of language
development in fictional narratives of Latino children.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Ser-
vices in Schools, 34(4), 332–342.
Newman, R. M., & McGregor, K. K. (2006). Teachers and
laypersons discern quality differences
between narratives produced by children with or without SLI.
Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research, 49(5), 1022–1036.
Nippold, M. (2007). Later language development: School-age
children, adolescents, and young
adults (3rd ed.). Austin, Texas: Pro-ed.
Norbury, C., & Bishop, D. (2003). Narrative skills of children
with communication impairments.
International Journal of Communication Disorders, 38(3), 287–
313.
620 Language Testing 27(4)
North Carolina Department of Public Instruction (2004).
English language arts: Standard course
of study and grade level competencies, K – 12. Raleigh, NC:
North Carolina Department of
Public Instruction (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
ED496004).
Oller, D. & Pearson, B. (2002). Assessing the effects of
bilingualism: A background. In D. Oller &
R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy development in bilingual
children (pp. 3–21). Tonawanda,
NY: Multilingual Matters.
Ollendick, T., Grills, A., & King, N. (2001). Applying
developmental theory to the assessment and
treatment of childhood disorders: Does it make a difference?
Clinical Psychology & Psycho-
therapy, 8, 304–315.
O’Neill, D. K., Pearce, M. J., & Pick, J. L. (2004). Preschool
children’s narratives and performance
on the Peabody Individualized Achievement Test–Revised:
Evidence of a relation between
early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language,
24(71), 149–183.
Paul, R. & Smith, R. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds with
normal, impaired, and late-developing
language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(3), 592–
598.
Pearce, W., McCormack, P, & James, D. (2003). Exploring the
boundaries of SLI: Findings from mor-
phosyntactic and story grammar analyses. Clinical Linguistics &
Phonetics, 17(4–5), 325–334.
Pelligrini, A., Galda, L., Bartini, M., & Charak, D. (1998). Oral
language and literacy learning in
content: The role of social relationships. Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly, 44(1), 38–54.
Peña, E., Gillam, R., Malek, M., Ruiz-Felter, R., Resendiz, M.,
Fiestas, C., & Sabel, T. (2006). Dynamic
assessment of school-age children’s narrative ability: An
experimental investigation of classifica-
tion accuracy. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Research, 49(5), 1037–1057.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (2004). Echoing our parents:
Parental influences on children’s nar-
ration. In Pratt, M. & Fiese, B. (Eds.), Family stories and the
life course: Across time and
generations (pp. 27–54) Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Quasthoff, U. (1997). An interactive approach to narrative
development. In Bamberg, M. (Ed.),
Narrative development: Six approaches (pp. 51–83). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rapin, I., & Allen, D. (1983). Developmental language
disorders: Nosological considerations. In
U. Kirk (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language, reading and
spelling (pp. 155–184). New York:
Academic Press.
Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). Frog,
Where are you? Narratives in children
with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury and
Williams Syndrome. In
B. Wulfeck & J. Reilly (Eds.), Plasticity and development:
Language in atypical children. Spe-
cial issue, Brain & Language, 88(2), 229–247.
Reschly, A., Busch, T., Betts, J., Deno, S., & Long, J. (2009).
Curriculum-Based Measurement
Oral Reading as an indicator of reading achievement: A meta-
analysis of the correlational
evidence. Journal of School Psychology 47(6), 427–469.
Ripich, D. N., & Griffith, P. L. (1988). Narrative abilities of
children with learning disabilities and
nondisabled children: Story structure, cohesion, and
propositions. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
21(3), 165–173.
Rojas, R., & Iglesias, A. (2009, March). Making a case for
language sampling. The ASHA Leader,
14, 10–11.
Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A
longitudinal analysis of the connection
between oral language and early reading. Journal of Educational
Research, 95(5), 259–272.
Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., Cooper, D. H., & De La Paz, S.
(1996). Unresolved mysteries: How do
metalinguistic and narrative skills connect with early reading?
The Journal of Special Education,
30(3), 257–277.
Heilmann et al. 621
Schneider, P., & Dubé, R. (2005). Story presentation effects on
children’s retell content. American
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(1), 52–60.
Snow, C. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during
the preschool years. Harvard Edu-
cational Review, 53(2), 165–189.
Snow, C. E., Dickinson, D. K., Jennings, E. M., & Purves, A. C.
(1991). Skills that aren’t basic in a
new conception of literacy. In E. Jennings & A. Purves (Eds.),
Literate systems and individual
lives: Perspectives on literacy and schooling (pp. 179–191).
Albany, NY: State University of
New York Press.
Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of storytelling skill. In M.
Franklin & S. Barten (Eds.), Child
language: A reader. New York: Oxford University Press.
Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis for story
comprehension in elementary school. In R. Free-
dle (Eds.), New directions in discourse processing, Vol. 2. (pp.
53–119). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Strong, C. (1998). The Strong Narrative Assessment Procedure.
Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.
Strong, C., & Shaver, J. (1991). Stability of cohesion in the
spoken narratives of language-impaired and
normally developing school-aged children. Journal of Speech &
Hearing Research, 34(1), 95–111.
Tabors, P., Snow, C., & Dickinson, D. (2001). Homes and
schools together: Supporting language
and literacy development. In D. Dickinson & P. Tabors (Eds.),
Beginning literacy with lan-
guage (pp. 313–334). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Weiss, A., Catts, H., & Ellis
Weismer, S. (2004). Dimensions of indi-
vidual differences in communication skills among primary grade
children. In M. Rice & S.
Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From
phenotpyes to etiologies (pp. 53–76).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Tomblin, J., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E.,
& O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence
of specific language impairment in kindergarten children.
Journal of Speech, Language, and
Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260.
Uccelli, P., & Páez, M. M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary
development of bilingual children
from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and
associations among English and
Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools, 38(3), 225–236.
van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cement of the
universe: The development of event
comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In P. van den
Broek, P. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.),
Developmental spans in event comprehension: Bridging
fictional and actual events (pp. 321–342).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Westby, C. (2005). Assessing and facilitating text
comprehension problems. In H. Catts & A. Kamhi
(Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (pp. 157–232).
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Appendix A: Narrative Organization Scoring Procedures
1. Plot and Theme (Reilly et al., 2004)
Plotline (8 points possible):
Setting – 1 point
Instantiation (frog escapes) – 1 point
5 main search episodes – 0–5 points
Resolution – 1 point
622 Language Testing 27(4)
Theme (4 points possible):
Mention that the frog was missing and the boy was looking for
him – 0–2 points
Search theme mentioned through the story: 0 = no additional
mentions, 1 = one
or two mentions, 2 = three or more mentions
2. Ordinal adaptation of Applebee’s Narrative Maturity Scale
(adapted from Manhardt & Rescorla, 2002 and Hughes et al.,
1997)
1. Heap: events described with no central theme
2. Sequence: events related to a single theme; no causal links
between story
concepts
3. Primitive narrative: stories have a concrete core
4. Focused chain: story revolves around a central character
going through a series
of events, story well connected
5. True narrative: well-developed story that has a central theme
or moral
3. Ordinal adaptation of Stein’s story levels (adapted from
Pearce et al.,
2003 and Hughes et al., 1997)
1. Isolated description: description of random characters and
actions
2. Descriptive sequence: describe characters and actions, but no
causal
relationships
3. Action sequence: actions described in correct chronological
order, but no causal
relationships
4. Reactive sequence: series of actions with some causal
relationships, no goal-
directed descriptions
5. Abbreviated episode: story is goal-directed, but characters’
intent not explicitly
stated
6. Incomplete episode: characters’ intent is explicitly stated but
one of the following
episode components is missing: initiating event, attempt, or
consequence
7. Complete episode: story contains all three aspects of a
complete episode: initiating
event, attempt, and consequence
8. Complex episode: full episode is elaborated by including an
obstacle to obtaining
the goal
9. Multiple episode: story contains more than one episode
(either complete or
incomplete)
10. Embedded episodes: one episode embedded within another
11. Interactive episodes: use multiple perspectives to describe
events; multiple charac-
ters and multiple goals mutually influence each other
Heilmann et al. 623
A
p
p
e
n
d
ix
B
:
T
he
N
ar
ra
ti
ve
S
co
ri
ng
S
ch
em
e
C
ha
ra
ct
er
is
ti
c:
Pr
o
fic
ie
nt
Em
er
gi
ng
M
in
im
al
/I
m
m
at
ur
e
In
tr
o
du
ct
io
n
1)
S
et
ti
ng
:
1)
S
et
ti
ng
:
•
L
au
nc
he
s
in
to
s
to
ry
w
it
h
•
S
ta
te
s
ge
ne
ra
l p
la
ce
a
nd
p
ro
vi
de
s
so
m
e
•
S
ta
te
s
ge
ne
ra
l s
et
ti
ng
b
ut
p
ro
vi
de
s
n
o
a
tt
em
pt
t
o
p
ro
vi
de
d
et
ai
l a
bo
ut
t
he
s
et
ti
ng
(
e.
g.
, r
ef
er
en
ce
n
o
d
et
ai
l.
t
he
s
et
ti
ng
.
t
o
t
he
t
im
e
o
f
th
e
se
tt
in
g,
d
ay
ti
m
e,
•
D
es
cr
ip
ti
o
n
o
r
el
em
en
ts
o
f
st
o
ry
a
re
b
ed
ti
m
e,
s
ea
so
n)
.
g
iv
en
in
te
rm
it
te
nt
ly
t
hr
o
ug
h
st
o
ry
.
•
S
et
ti
ng
e
le
m
en
ts
a
re
s
ta
te
d
at
•
M
ay
p
ro
vi
de
d
es
cr
ip
tio
n
o
f s
pe
ci
fic
e
le
m
en
t
a
pp
ro
pr
ia
te
p
la
ce
in
s
to
ry
.
o
f
se
tt
in
g
(e
.g
.,
th
e
fr
o
g
is
in
t
he
ja
r)
.
O
R
2)
C
ha
ra
ct
er
s:
2)
C
ha
ra
ct
er
s:
•
M
ai
n
ch
ar
ac
te
rs
a
re
in
tr
o
du
ce
d
w
it
h
•
C
ha
ra
ct
er
s
o
f
st
o
ry
a
re
m
en
ti
o
ne
d
w
it
h
s
o
m
e
de
sc
ri
pt
io
n
o
r
de
ta
il
pr
o
vi
de
d.
n
o
d
et
ai
l o
r
de
sc
ri
pt
io
n.
C
ha
ra
ct
er
•
M
ai
n
ch
ar
ac
te
r(
s)
a
nd
a
ll
su
pp
o
rt
in
g
•
B
o
th
m
ai
n
an
d
ac
ti
ve
s
up
po
rt
in
g
•
I
nc
o
ns
is
te
nt
m
en
ti
o
n
o
f
de
ve
lo
pm
en
t
c
ha
ra
ct
er
(s
)
ar
e
m
en
ti
o
ne
d.
c
ha
ra
ct
er
s
ar
e
m
en
ti
o
ne
d.
i
nv
o
lv
ed
o
r
ac
ti
ve
•
T
hr
o
ug
ho
ut
s
to
ry
it
is
c
le
ar
c
hi
ld
c
an
•
M
ai
n
ch
ar
ac
te
rs
a
re
n
o
t
cl
ea
rl
y
c
ha
ra
ct
er
s.
d
is
cr
im
in
at
e
be
tw
ee
n
m
ai
n
an
d
d
is
ti
ng
ui
sh
ed
f
ro
m
s
up
po
rt
in
g
ch
ar
ac
te
rs
.
•
C
ha
ra
ct
er
(s
)
ne
ce
ss
ar
y
s
up
po
rt
in
g
ch
ar
ac
te
rs
(
e.
g.
, m
o
re
f
o
r
ad
va
nc
in
g
th
e
pl
o
t
d
es
cr
ip
ti
o
n
o
f,
em
ph
as
is
u
po
n
a
re
n
o
t
pr
es
en
t.
m
ai
n
ch
ar
ac
te
r(
s)
).
•
C
hi
ld
n
ar
ra
te
s
in
f
ir
st
p
er
so
n
us
in
g
c
ha
ra
ct
er
v
o
ic
e
(e
.g
., ‘
Yo
u
ge
t
o
ut
o
f
m
y
t
re
e’
, s
ai
d
th
e
o
w
l.)
.
M
en
ta
l s
ta
te
s
•
M
en
ta
l s
ta
te
s
o
f
m
ai
n
an
d
su
pp
o
rt
in
g
•
S
o
m
e
us
e
o
f
ev
id
en
t
m
en
ta
l s
ta
te
•
N
o
u
se
o
f
m
en
ta
l s
ta
te
c
ha
ra
ct
er
s
ar
e
ex
pr
es
se
d
w
he
n
ne
ce
ss
ar
y
w
o
rd
s
to
d
ev
el
o
p
ch
ar
ac
te
r(
s)
.
w
o
rd
s
to
d
ev
el
o
p
f
o
r
pl
o
t
de
ve
lo
pm
en
t
an
d
ad
va
nc
em
en
t.
c
ha
ra
ct
er
(s
).
•
A
v
ar
ie
ty
o
f
m
en
ta
l s
ta
te
w
o
rd
s
ar
e
us
ed
.
R
ef
er
en
ci
ng
•
P
ro
vi
de
s
ne
ce
ss
ar
y
an
te
ce
de
nt
s
•
I
nc
o
ns
is
te
nt
u
se
o
f
re
fe
re
nt
s/
an
te
ce
de
nt
s.
•
E
xc
es
si
ve
u
se
o
f
t
o
p
ro
no
un
s.
p
ro
no
un
s.
•
R
ef
er
en
ce
s
ar
e
cl
ea
r
th
ro
ug
ho
ut
s
to
ry
.
•
N
o
v
er
ba
l c
la
ri
fie
rs
u
se
d.
•
C
hi
ld
is
u
na
w
ar
e
l
is
te
ne
r
is
c
o
nf
us
ed
.
(C
on
tin
ue
d)
624 Language Testing 27(4)
A
p
p
e
n
d
ix
B
:
(C
o
nt
in
ue
d)
C
ha
ra
ct
er
is
ti
c:
Pr
o
fic
ie
nt
Em
er
gi
ng
M
in
im
al
/I
m
m
at
ur
e
C
o
nf
lic
t
•
C
le
ar
ly
s
ta
te
s
al
l c
o
nf
lic
ts
a
nd
•
U
nd
er
d
ev
el
o
pe
d
de
sc
ri
pt
io
n
o
f
co
nf
lic
ts
•
R
an
do
m
r
es
o
lu
ti
o
n(
s)
re
so
lu
ti
o
n
r
es
o
lu
ti
o
ns
c
ri
ti
ca
l t
o
a
dv
an
ci
ng
t
he
a
nd
r
es
o
lu
ti
o
ns
c
ri
ti
ca
l t
o
a
dv
an
ci
ng
t
he
s
ta
te
d
w
it
h
no
m
en
ti
o
n
p
lo
t
o
f
th
e
st
o
ry
.
p
lo
t
o
f
th
e
st
o
ry
.
o
f
ca
us
e
o
r
co
nf
lic
t.
O
R
O
R
•
N
o
t
al
l c
o
nf
lic
ts
a
nd
r
es
o
lu
ti
o
ns
c
ri
ti
ca
l
•
C
o
nf
lic
t
m
en
ti
o
ne
d
t
o
a
dv
an
ci
ng
t
he
p
lo
t
ar
e
pr
es
en
t.
w
it
ho
ut
r
es
o
lu
ti
o
n.
O
R
•
M
an
y
co
nf
lic
ts
a
nd
r
es
o
lu
ti
o
ns
c
ri
ti
ca
l t
o
a
dv
an
ci
ng
t
he
p
lo
t
ar
e
n
o
t
pr
es
en
t.
C
o
he
si
o
n
•
E
ve
nt
s
fo
llo
w
a
lo
gi
ca
l o
rd
er
.
•
E
ve
nt
s
fo
llo
w
a
lo
gi
ca
l o
rd
er
.
•
N
o
u
se
o
f
sm
o
o
th
•
C
ri
ti
ca
l e
ve
nt
s
ar
e
in
cl
ud
ed
w
hi
le
le
ss
•
E
xc
es
si
ve
d
et
ai
l o
r
em
ph
as
is
p
ro
vi
de
d
o
n
t
ra
ns
it
io
ns
.
e
m
ph
as
is
is
p
la
ce
d
o
n
m
in
o
r
ev
en
ts
.
m
in
o
r
ev
en
ts
le
ad
in
g
th
e
lis
te
ne
r
as
tr
ay
.
•
S
m
o
o
th
t
ra
ns
it
io
ns
a
re
p
ro
vi
de
d
O
R
b
et
w
ee
n
ev
en
ts
.
•
T
ra
ns
it
io
ns
t
o
n
ex
t
ev
en
t
un
cl
ea
r.
O
R
•
M
in
im
al
d
et
ai
l g
iv
en
fo
r
cr
it
ic
al
e
ve
nt
s.
O
R
•
E
qu
al
e
m
ph
as
is
o
n
al
l e
ve
nt
s.
C
o
nc
lu
si
o
n
•
S
to
ry
is
c
le
ar
ly
w
ra
pp
ed
u
p
us
in
g
ge
ne
ra
l
•
S
pe
ci
fic
e
ve
nt
is
c
o
nc
lu
de
d,
b
ut
n
o
•
S
to
ps
n
ar
ra
ti
ng
a
nd
c
o
nc
lu
di
ng
s
ta
te
m
en
ts
s
uc
h
as
‘a
nd
t
he
y
g
en
er
al
s
ta
te
m
en
t
m
ad
e
as
t
o
t
he
l
is
te
ne
r
m
ay
n
ee
d
to
a
sk
w
er
e
to
ge
th
er
a
ga
in
h
ap
py
a
s
co
ul
d
be
’.
c
o
nc
lu
si
o
n
o
f
th
e
w
ho
le
s
to
ry
.
i
f
th
at
is
t
he
e
nd
.
N
ot
es
:
Sc
o
ri
ng
: E
ac
h
ch
ar
ac
te
ri
st
ic
r
ec
ei
ve
s
a
sc
al
ed
s
co
re
0
–5
. P
ro
fic
ie
nt
c
ha
ra
ct
er
is
ti
cs
=
5
, E
m
er
gi
ng
=
3
, M
in
im
al
/
Im
m
at
ur
e
=
1
. S
co
re
s
in
b
et
w
ee
n
(e
.g
.,
2,
4
)
ar
e
un
de
fin
ed
, u
se
ju
dg
m
en
t.
Sc
o
re
s
o
f 0
, N
A
a
re
d
ef
in
ed
b
el
o
w
. A
c
o
m
po
si
te
is
s
co
re
d
by
a
dd
in
g
th
e
to
ta
l o
f t
he
c
ha
ra
ct
er
is
ti
c
sc
o
re
s.
H
ig
he
st
s
co
re
=
3
5.
A
s
co
re
o
f 0
is
g
iv
en
fo
r
C
hi
ld
E
rr
o
rs
(
i.e
.,
te
lli
ng
t
he
w
ro
ng
s
to
ry
, c
o
nv
er
si
ng
w
it
h
ex
am
in
er
, n
o
t
co
m
pl
et
in
g/
re
fu
si
ng
t
as
k,
u
si
ng
w
ro
ng
la
ng
ua
ge
c
re
at
in
g
in
ab
ili
ty
o
f s
co
re
r
to
c
o
m
pr
eh
en
d
st
o
ry
in
t
ar
ge
t
la
ng
ua
ge
, a
ba
nd
o
ne
d
ut
te
ra
nc
es
, u
ni
nt
el
lig
ib
ili
ty
, p
o
o
r
pe
rf
o
rm
an
ce
, c
o
m
po
ne
nt
s
o
f r
ub
ri
c
ar
e
in
im
it
at
io
n-
o
nl
y)
.
A
s
co
re
o
f N
A
(
no
n-
ap
pl
ic
ab
le
)
is
g
iv
en
fo
r
M
ec
ha
ni
ca
l/E
xa
m
in
er
/O
pe
ra
to
r
Er
ro
rs
(
i.e
.,
in
te
rf
er
en
ce
fr
o
m
b
ac
kg
ro
un
d
no
is
e,
is
su
es
w
it
h
re
co
rd
in
g
(c
ut
-o
ffs
, i
nt
er
ru
pt
io
ns
),
ex
am
in
er
q
ui
tt
in
g
be
fo
re
c
hi
ld
d
o
es
, e
xa
m
in
er
n
o
t
fo
llo
w
in
g
pr
o
to
co
l,
ex
am
in
er
a
sk
in
g
o
ve
rl
y
sp
ec
ifi
c
o
r
le
ad
in
g
qu
es
ti
o
ns
r
at
he
r
th
an
o
pe
n-
en
de
d
qu
es
ti
o
ns
o
r
pr
o
m
pt
s)
.
Heilmann et al. 625
Appendix C: Sample Narratives
Sample 1:
A little boy went out one day and caught a frog. He put the frog
in a jar and stared at him
when he got in his room again. The dog looked in the jar and
saw the frog too. While the
boy was sleeping the frog jumped down and ran out the window,
while he was sleeping.
The next morning the boy woke up and looked at the jar. The
frog was not inside the jar.
The boy looked everywhere. The dog put his head inside the jar.
His head got stuck. Then
they looked out the window and called for the frog to come
back. The dog fell off the thing
and the jar broke. The boy picked him up to see if he was OK.
And the dog licked him for
that. The dog and the boy went out in the backyard and went in
the forest. They searched
and searched and searched. But they still couldn’t find the frog.
The little boy crawled in a
gopher’s hole. The gopher popped out and started running away.
The dog was barking at a
beehive. And the beehive fell down. And a swarm of bees came
out that minute while the
boy was looking inside a tree. The bees swarmed and chased the
dog. The owl creeped out
and scared the boy away. After that he climbed up a tall rock
and leaned on some branches.
They started moving upwards and turning. This was a deer and
not real branches. The dog
ran with the deer and started barking at the deer. The deer
stopped in a sudden moment.
And the boy and the dog fell down. They landed in a warm pond
and heard the sound of a
croak. The boy told the dog to be quiet because if he wasn’t
then the frog would hop away
because of the babyies over a dead log. They found his old frog
with a mama frog. And they
had eight tiny babies. One jumped up to his hand to greet the
boy. The boy liked him. And
the frog liked him also. So he took that frog for a new pet and
left the other frogs together
so the family would be safe. And they had seven tiny babies to
take care of, not one.
Plot & Theme: 11 (search theme reiterated only twice)
Applebee: 5
Stein: 10
NSS: 26
• Introduction:4
• CharacterDev:4
• MentalStates:2
• Referencing:4
• ConflictRes:4
• Cohesion:4
• Conclusion:4
Sample 2:
Once there was a little boy named Tom. He had a little frog and
a dog. One night, when Tom
and his dog were sleeping, the frog crept away. When Tom and
his dog leaned over the next
morning, the frog was gone. He looked everywhere for the frog.
He called out the window.
When he looked into the jar, his head got stuck in the jar. And
when he leaned over, he fell out
the window. And Tom jumped out and picked him up to see if
he was OK. He gave him a lick.
And then Tom spent the rest of the day looking for his frog. He
called down the hole but there
626 Language Testing 27(4)
was a gopher. The dog barked at a tree and some bees scared
them. Tom looked into a tree hole.
There’s an owl. He climbed up a big rock. And he leaned onto
some big branches. But they
weren’t branches. They were a deer’s antlers. And the deer ran
with Tom on his head. He put
the dog and Tom in the water. And they snuck up very quietly.
And they found his frog and lots
of other frogs. He took the baby frog as his new pet. And he
waved bye to his old frogs.
Plot & Theme: 11 (search theme reiterated only once)
Applebee: 5
Stein: 10
NSS: 21
• Introduction:3
• CharacterDev:3
• MentalStates:2
• Referencing:3
• ConflictRes:3
• Cohesion:4
• Conclusion:3
Sample 3:
The boy was looking for his frog. All day he looked for the frog
and couldn’t find him.
Finally a beehive, the dog barked at a beehive in the tree. And
the dog got in trouble. And
so did the boy because the gopher and the owl. And ran away.
And then he chased the
dog. And he looked out from it. And then he climbed on the
branch. But it wasn’t
branches. It was a deer. And the deer shoved him off of the
cliff. And then he went to the
frog. And he had a family. And then a frog jumped out to get
him. And then he took that
frog home and left his old frog where his old frog was.
Plot & Theme: 5 (Plot: 2 search episodes (beehive and deer
scenes), resolution; Theme: initial
mention that the boy was looking for the frog, one additional
mention of searching for the frog)
Applebee: 3
Stein: 5 (Abbreviated Episode): Story is goal-directed (i.e.,
searching for the frog). But,
there are no complete episodes in the story.
NSS: 14
• Introduction: 1
• CharacterDev: 3
• MentalStates: 1
• Referencing: 3
• ConflictRes: 1
• Cohesion: 2
• Conclusion: 3
Copyright of Language Testing is the property of Sage
Publications, Ltd. and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.
The Development of Narrative Identity in Late Adolescence and
Emergent
Adulthood: The Continued Importance of Listeners
Monisha Pasupathi
University of Utah
Timothy Hoyt
University of New Mexico
Research on narrative identity in late adolescence and early
adulthood has not extensively examined how
conversational storytelling affects the development of narrative
identity. This is a major gap, given the
importance of this age period for narrative identity development
and the clear importance of parent– child
conversations in the development of narrative identity. The
authors present a series of 3 studies (n � 220)
examining how late adolescents and early adults construct
narrative identity in ways that are shaped by
their listeners. The findings suggest that late adolescents and
early adults construct more meaning-laden,
interpretive accounts of their everyday experiences when they
converse with responsive friends. Further,
even within this sample’s abbreviated age range, the authors
found evidence for age-related increases in
the factual content of personal memories. Such findings
illuminate the importance of friends in the
construction of narrative identity during this key developmental
period.
Keywords: narrative identity development, autobiographical
memory development, parent– child remi-
niscing, social construction
Late adolescence and early adulthood are accorded special sig-
nificance for the development of self and identity (Arnett, 2000;
Dusek & McIntyre, 2003; Erikson & Erikson, 1997; Harter,
1998;
Kroger, 2003), narrative identity (Habermas & Bluck, 2000;
McLean, 2005), and autobiographical memory (Holmes & Con-
way, 1999; Rubin & Schulkind, 1997). In fact, work on the
reminiscence bump suggests that adolescence and early
adulthood
are a privileged developmental period for the encoding of auto-
biographical memories, which in narrative terms constitute the
stuff of which selves are made.
But despite the widespread acknowledgment of the importance
of late adolescence and early adulthood for identity
development,
there are some important gaps in existing research. Perhaps
most
notable among these is a relative lack of attention to the micro-
processes by which identity in general and narrative identity in
particular develop during this period. In the present article, our
focus is particularly on the microprocess of conversational
story-
telling as a critical process by which narrative identity more
broadly develops. Further, we show, across three studies, that
having responsive, attentive friends as listeners for
conversational
storytelling helps further narrative identity development in late
adolescence and early adulthood. Next, we outline the
unaddressed
issues in identity development work that can be better examined
with the narrative and, more specifically, conversational
storytell-
ing frameworks.
Identity Development in Adolescence
and Early Adulthood
Erikson and Erikson (1997) originally defined ego identity as
entailing a sense of uniqueness or individuality, an emerging
commitment to a place in society, and a sense of continuity over
time. Much of the extant work on identity development, as well
as work on self-development, has emphasized the uniqueness or
individuality aspect of self and identity by focusing on explicit
self-descriptive statements that participants do or do not en-
dorse. Research in this arena has suggested that identity devel-
opment in terms of exploring alternative identities and commit-
ting to some rather than others is a nonlinear process that
appears to occur primarily in late adolescence and early adult-
hood (Constantinople, 1969; Meilman, 1979; Waterman, 1982).
Over adolescence and early adulthood, self-descriptions be-
come increasingly multifaceted and complex, and changes in
the content of self-descriptions also occur into early adulthood
(Dusek & McIntyre, 2003; Harter, 1998; Sutin & Robins, 2005).
The ability to recognize contradictions in the self emerges by
midadolescence, but the capacity for resolving them is a phe-
nomenon of later adolescence (Harter & Monsour, 1992), con-
sistent with increasing cognitive and epistemic abilities across
this period (e.g., Kitchener, Lynch, Fischer, & Wood, 1993).
Contextual factors such as parent– child relationships, peer re-
lationships, and exposure to college and larger sociopolitical
contexts are linked to identity development over this age range
(Dusek & McIntyre, 2003; Hair, Moore, Garrett, Ling, & Cleve-
land, 2008; McNulty & Swann, 1994; Waterman, 1982).
This literature underscores the importance of late adolescence
and early adulthood for identity development but is relatively
Monisha Pasupathi, Department of Psychology, University of
Utah;
Timothy Hoyt, Department of Psychology, University of New
Mexico.
These studies were supported by National Institute of Mental
Health
Grant 1R03MH64462 awarded to Monisha Pasupathi. Our
gratitude is due
to Benjamin Rich, for his extensive efforts in data collection, as
well as to
Martin Cryer and Leslie Rheinhold, who assisted with data
coding. In
addition, Carol Sansone, Kate McLean, and Frank Drews
provided valu-
able comments on earlier drafts of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Monisha
Pasupathi, Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 390
South 1530
East, Beh-502, Salt Lake City, UT 84112. E-mail:
[email protected]
Developmental Psychology © 2009 American Psychological
Association
2009, Vol. 45, No. 2, 558 –574 0012-1649/09/$12.00 DOI:
10.1037/a0014431
558
uninformative about the microprocesses by which identity is
con-
structed in that age period and does not capture the way in
which
people construct a sense of themselves as continuous across
time.
As an alternative, narrative approaches to identity development
capture both individuality and the continuity aspects of identity,
and their focus on the construction of identity is one step closer
to
an examination of the processes of identity formation.
Narrative Identity in Late Adolescence and Early
Adulthood: Closer to Process
Narrative approaches to identity development examine how
people construct meaning in relation to their experiences and
thus
further their sense of self and identity (McAdams, 1996). The
construction of narratives about past experiences also involves
the
creation of a sense of continuity over time—as the past self is
represented and interpreted by the present self. Thus, narrative
conceptualizations of identity hold the promise of integrating
Erikson and Erikson’s (1997) aspects of identity as well as illu-
minating the processes by which individuals develop identity.
For
example, narratives construct personal continuity over time by
linking past to the present and future. They also highlight
individ-
ual uniqueness and, at the same time, make use of cultural
scripts
and schemas for organizing one’s experience in narrative form.
In
fact, proponents of narrative identity research (McLean,
Pasupathi,
& Pals, 2007; Pasupathi, 2001; Thorne, 2000) have argued that
the
process of constructing narratives about the personal past is the
paramount process by which narrative identity in particular, and
self and identity more broadly, develop. In making this case,
they
specifically suggest that narrative identity develops via the
micro-
process of constructing specific narratives in specific situations.
That is, narrative identity emerges out of countless actions of
narrative construction. The study of narrative identity
development
consequently requires the exploration of processes that
influence
narrative identity creation in the moment.
It is important to note that narrative and narrative identity are
not synonymous. Although all stories must provide an account
of
what happened—the setting and actions that occurred— each
sto-
ryteller may have a unique sense of how the actions were con-
nected, which were important, and what the broader
implications
and associations of the experience may entail. These
interpretive,
meaning-laden features of a narrative render a set of facts
uniquely
reflective of the individual whose story they compose, and it is
these features that both reflect and construct narrative identity.
Moreover, it is these features that across various operationaliza-
tions are related to macrolevel development of self and identity
in
early childhood (e.g., Bird & Reese, 2006; Bohanek, Marin, &
Fivush, 2006; Fivush, 1991; Fivush, Bohanek, Robertson, &
Duke,
2004; Harley & Reese, 1999; Howe, Courage, & Peterson, 1994)
and in adolescence and early adulthood (McLean & Pratt, 2006;
see also Sutin & Robins, 2005), and to well-being and maturity
throughout adulthood (King & Patterson, 2000; McAdams,
Reyn-
olds, Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001; Pals, 2006a, 2006b).
The overall elaboration of narratives, including both factual and
interpretive information, increases across childhood (e.g., P. J.
Bauer, 2006; Fivush & Nelson, 2004). Little is known about
changes in general elaboration thereafter. Researchers focusing
on
adolescence and adulthood have emphasized broader, global
types
of meanings; these are conceptually connected to interpretive
elaboration. The prevalence of those larger identity-related ele-
ments in narratives increases with age into middle adulthood (J.
J.
Bauer & McAdams, 2004a, 2004b, 2005; Bluck & Glueck, 2004;
McAdams et al., 2006; McLean, 2005; McLean & Thorne, 2003;
Pasupathi & Mansour, 2006), and they become more positive
and
more nuanced (McAdams et al., 2006). This work suggests, but
does not explicitly demonstrate, increases in the prevalence and
sophistication of interpretive elaboration in narratives from ado-
lescence into middle adulthood. Moreover, this work is largely
mute about just how such macrolevel increases might come
about;
that is, it does not necessarily investigate the processes by
which
people come to vary in their construction of narrative identity.
Conversational Storytelling: The Process of Constructing
Narrative Identity
Conversations about the personal past with caregivers are a
major force in shaping young children’s ways of constructing
narratives and developing narrative identity (see, e.g., P. J.
Bauer,
2006; Fivush & Nelson, 2004). Some parents engage children in
highly responsive, elaborative conversation about the past,
whereas others are more focused and repetitive (Cleveland &
Reese, 2005; Fivush & Nelson, 2004; Harley & Reese, 1999;
Reese & Fivush, 1993; Wang, 2004). Children with more elabo-
rative mothers subsequently engage in more elaborative remem-
bering with other listeners and about other events. Maternal lis-
tening, then, influences the story created within that
conversation
but over time exerts cumulative, long-term effects on children’s
style of remembering. Converging findings from experimental
(Peterson, Jesso, & McCabe, 1999) and prospective longitudinal
designs (e.g., Haden, Haine, & Fivush, 1997) provide strong
evidence for a causal role for parent– child remembering. For
very
young children like those studied in this work, parental respon-
siveness is important for both factual and interpretive
elaboration
in narratives. Further, the development of basic capacities for
narration and the development of narrative identity are tightly
intertwined during early childhood.
Within a burgeoning literature on narrative identity in late
adolescence and early adulthood (e.g., McLean et al., 2007),
however, we know surprisingly little about the role played by
conversational storytelling and the listeners for such stories.
This
is in spite of the acknowledged importance of identity develop-
ment in this age range and the fact that this period coincides
with
a shift from parents as the primary audience for personal story-
telling to friends as an additional, increasingly important
audience
for such stories (McLean, 2005; see also Buhrmeister, 1996;
Cooper, 1999; Daddis, 2008; Hartup, 1996; Hazan & Zeifman,
1994; Updegraff, McHale, & Whiteman, 2006).
Some tiny pieces of evidence from studies of adolescents and
emergent adults suggest that listeners are important in the con-
struction of narrative identity in this age range. For example,
conversational interactions with parents do matter. More open
and
free-ranging conversations with parents are associated with
greater
autonomy and identity development in adolescents (Cooper &
Grotevant, 1985). By early adulthood, friends and parents
appear
to serve similar purposes as listeners for personal events,
according
to retrospective reports (McLean, 2005). Further, conversations
with friends during adolescence are quite strikingly concerned
with identity (Korobov & Bamberg, 2004; Parker & Gottman,
559NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
1989), and work with late adolescents suggests that listeners
prefer
narratives that entail more sophisticated identity-related
insights—
indicative of a listener preference for more interpretive
elaboration
(Thorne, McLean, & Lawrence, 2004). Some work with young
adults has linked the action of telling a personal story to
changes
in more traditionally assessed self-conceptions or event percep-
tions and has shown that variations in the way listeners respond
to
that story, or variations in the goals with which the story is told,
correspond to immediate self-perception changes (e.g.,
McGregor
& Holmes, 1999; McLean, 2005; Pasupathi, Alderman, & Shaw,
2007; Pasupathi & Rich, 2005; Thoman, Sansone, & Pasupathi,
2007).
A handful of studies have explicitly looked at variations in
listener behavior and storytelling among young adults (Bavelas,
Coates, & Johnson, 2000; Dickinson & Givon, 1995; Pasupathi,
Stallworth, & Murdoch, 1998). These studies examined story-
telling by young adults to a stranger and experimentally varied
the stranger’s behavior during storytelling. That variation cre-
ated either distracted or disagreeable listeners; the comparison
conditions always involved a responsive, agreeable listener.
Generally, responsive and agreeable listeners concurrently
elicit more elaborative stories than do distracted or disagreeable
listeners, whether about brief film stimuli (Dickinson & Givon,
1995; Pasupathi et al., 1998) or about personal experiences
(Bavelas et al., 2000). In fact, listeners in the responsive and
agreeable conditions behave more elaboratively than do listen-
ers in the distracted conditions, in these studies. That is, they
ask questions, make sympathetic exclamations, demonstrate
that they have understood the speaker’s point, and follow
the speaker’s interests rather than impose their own agenda.
These behaviors are quite similar to those observed among
more elaborative mothers in work on parent– child reminiscing.
One goal of the present studies was to demonstrate that the
development of narrative identity is affected by the process of
storytelling, by showing that the extent to which late adoles-
cents and early adults construct elaborative, richly detailed
personal stories is connected to the behavior of their listening
friends.
But as we noted earlier, not all elaborations are equally
relevant for narrative identity. Interpretive information in par-
ticular makes memory into narrative identity by tying recalled
experiences to the individual’s goals, feelings, thoughts, and
beliefs. In childhood, when memory conversations involve
learning how to remember, responsive listening is clearly linked
to elaboration of all types of information, although for self-
development the elaboration of interpretive or evaluative con-
tent is more important. By contrast, in adulthood, responsive
listening may be of particular importance for interpretive infor-
mation. In fact, Bavelas et al. (2000) showed that distracted
(and therefore unresponsive) listeners were selectively impaired
in their capacity to respond to the meaning of what storytellers
were saying, although they did not examine whether this cor-
responded to a selective suppression of storytellers’ inclusion of
such meanings. Thus, our second aim was to examine whether
responsive friends were especially important in facilitating the
construction of richly interpretive accounts of personal experi-
ences, that is, in helping late adolescents infuse memory with
narrative identity.
Overview of the Present Studies
The major goal of the present studies was to provide laboratory-
based evidence for the microprocess. Critically, these studies
were
not designed to address the macrodevelopmental process of nar-
rative identity process but, rather, to address the critical
micropro-
cess building block for narrative identity—the process of
conver-
sational storytelling. Other work suggests that if responsive
listening has immediate importance for narrative identity in the
short term, it will also have long-term implications for
macrolevel
self-development, an issue to which we return in the discussion
of
our findings.
The present studies examine in vivo storytelling about personal
experiences to close, same-gender friends by late adolescents
and
early adults (defined as the age range from 18 to 35 years). We
chose to examine storytelling to same-gender close friends
because
narrative identity suggests that close friends and family are the
primary audience for personal storytelling in adulthood and be-
cause close friends and parents appear to serve similar roles as
listeners among late adolescents and early adults (McLean,
2005).
Restricting the focus to same-gender friendships allowed us to
reduce the possible impact of different relationships between
lis-
teners and speakers (i.e., romantic vs. primarily platonic
relations).
We compared responsive and unresponsive (distracted) friends
during storytelling across three studies, examining how
variations
in responsiveness influenced the elaboration of factual and
inter-
pretive aspects of storytelling.
We chose experimental methods because exploratory work in
our laboratory suggested that under normal conditions, most
friends are fairly responsive listeners in a laboratory setting in
which there are few distractions and their defined task is to
listen.
Experimentally creating an unresponsive listener via distraction
ensured that some of our participants would not function like
highly elaborative mothers by asking questions or expressing
strong interest. Experimental methods also permit stronger con-
clusions about causality and link the present studies to work on
young adults.
In addition to examining our major goals, we also examined two
other issues in the present studies. First, although age
differences,
particularly in this restricted age range, were not a primary
focus
of our study, we examined whether age was associated with
increases in the elaboration of factual or interpretive
information,
given other findings on autobiographical memory development
during this age period. Second, by late adolescence and beyond,
people also have the capacity to engage in deliberate, strategic
self-presentation (e.g., Baumeister, Stilman, & Wotman, 1990;
Tice, 1992). Such deliberate self-presentation would suggest
that
people strategically change their stories, and strategic changes
in
narration imply a different type of identity-related implication
than
do changes in narrative identity construction that occur without
a
great deal of self-awareness on the part of the individual.
Study 1
Study 1 provides a first examination of how late adolescents
and
young adults construct narrative identity around positive experi-
ences, in collaboration with listeners who vary in their
responsive-
ness. We expected that late adolescents and emergent adults
talk-
ing with distracted friends would construct less elaborated
stories
560 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
in general but would be especially likely to construct less inter-
pretive accounts.
Method
Participants
Participants were 40 same-gender pairs of friends (50% men,
50% women) recruited from the introductory psychology subject
pool at the University of Utah. To recruit pairs of friends, we
first
recruited target participants (assigned to the speaker role), and
these participants were asked to bring a close, same-gender
friend
to the experiment. The average age of speakers was 21.6 years
(SD � 3.2). Participating friends had known each other an
average
of 3.2 years (SD � 3.8).1 The majority of participants were
European American (90%). Pairs were randomly assigned to an
attentive or distracted listener condition; conditions did not
differ
in the length of time friends had known one another or in
ratings
of friendship quality. Equipment failure caused loss of
transcripts
in two cases. Sample sizes for analyses reported below vary
accordingly.
Procedure
After arriving, participants completed several measures, includ-
ing personality, relationship quality, and background measures.
Speakers identified recent undisclosed events they considered to
be positive, selected one event to describe to the friend, and
provided some preliminary ratings of the events. Meanwhile,
friends were separated from speakers for instruction. Attentive
friends were told, “Listen to your friend the way you typically
do
when you’re being a good listener.” Distracted friends were
told,
“We are interested in how conversations go when one person is
distracted.” They were asked to count all words beginning with
the
letters th while listening to the speaker and practiced this task
on
10 prepared stories read aloud by the experimenter, who
provided
feedback about task accuracy after each story. Listeners were
offered an incentive of $5 if they were accurate in counting
words
beginning with th, with accuracy defined as coming within a
4-point margin of error during the subsequent conversation with
the speaker. This target was chosen on the basis of listener per-
formance in the training session, because listeners were rarely
especially accurate but achieved this standard of performance at
a
rate of about 50% of the time. Pilot work revealed that
participants
proved to have difficulty ignoring their friend’s storytelling in
favor of the th task, and the incentive improved their level of
distraction. The dyad was then reunited and videotaped while
the
speaker described the chosen experience to the listener.
Following
the conversation, the speaker completed additional ratings of
lis-
tener responsiveness and agreeableness, and rated the extent to
which the story was detailed and coherently told. Participants
were
fully debriefed and given information about both listener condi-
tions, and were told about our interest in how listeners
influence
storytelling.
Measures
Manipulation check. As a manipulation check, we obtained
speaker ratings of the extent to which the listener agreed with
his
or her version of events (a single item) and of the listener’s
responsiveness (an average across five items, including, for
exam-
ple, “The listener was very responsive”; Cronbach’s � � .84).
Agreement and responsiveness often covary, but they are
concep-
tually distinct (e.g., Pasupathi, Carstensen, Levenson, &
Gottman,
1999). Both were assessed because manipulations of listener re-
sponsiveness sometimes influence both.
Speaker’s perceptions of story quality. To assess deliberate
changes in storytelling, we obtained speakers’ ratings of the
qual-
ity of the story they told across nine items, using 7-point Likert-
type scales in which 1 indicated lower quality and 7 higher
quality.
Example items include “My story used few details/many
details”
and “My story was very incoherent/very coherent.” These nine
items were averaged to create a single index of story quality
(Cronbach’s � � .90).
Coding of conversational data. Each recorded conversation
was transcribed by undergraduate research assistants.
Transcripts
were then divided into “idea units” that roughly corresponded to
verb phrases. Idea units were then coded according to their con-
tent. We examined two main types of narrative content. The
first
main category was event factual information, that is,
information
available to the perceptual capabilities of any bystander.
Examples
of this category include such as statements “Mom and I had a
talk
while you were gone,” “We talked for almost two hours,” and
“She
started crying and everything.” The second category was
interpre-
tive information, or information that emphasizes the subjective,
meaning-laden, and internal aspects of experience—in short,
nar-
rative identity. Examples of this category include “It was just a
good talk,” “She’s always sappy like that,” “We have become
closer and everything,” and “It makes it feel like things have
really
changed since I left for school.” Additional categories included
statements aimed at regulating the conversation (establishing
un-
derstanding, providing minimal back channels, etc.), off-topic
statements (references to the experimental setting), and
uncodable
statements. The Appendix presents an entire conversation, in
units
and coded.
To establish reliability between coders, the two authors first
independently coded several conversations. These independently
coded sets were then compared by means of Cohen’s kappa.
Once
the authors established substantial independent agreement (� �
.91) and resolved remaining discrepancies through discussion,
the
second author then trained a team of two additional
undergraduate
coders. After learning the coding scheme, the coding team
applied
the coding scheme to the 38 conversations in the study. The
coding
team held weekly meetings in which problematic coding units
were discussed and agreement on content was sought. Fifteen
percent of the conversation pairs were coded independently by
two
coders to quantify reliability across the full set of content
catego-
ries in the coding scheme. Across 373 idea units, coders agreed
77% of the time on the content category (� � .68). For each
pair,
we computed the number of speaker-generated facts and evalua-
tions. These content code variables served as the primary
depen-
dent measures for our analyses.
1 The length of time partners had known one another was
unrelated to
any of the results reported here; nor did relationship quality
have any
impact on any of the findings.
561NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
Results
Manipulation Check
A general linear model examining the effects of experimental
condition and gender on perceived listener attentiveness and
agree-
ableness revealed only an overall effect of condition, F(2, 37) �
26.8, p � .01, �p
2 � .59. Univariate tests showed that condition
effects were evident for perceptions of listener attentiveness,
F(1,
38) � 6.3, p � .02, �p
2 � .14, and for perceptions of listener
agreeableness, F(1, 38) � 54.9, p � .01, �p
2 � .59. Speakers
perceived distracted listeners to be less responsive (attentive: M
�
5.4, SD � 1.0; distracted: M � 3.1, SD � 1.0) and less
agreeable
(attentive: M � 6.1, SD � 1.4; distracted: M � 4.9, SD � 1.7).
Impact of Listener Responsiveness on Amount and Type
of Information Recalled
We expected that when faced with distracted listeners, our
partic-
ipants would construct shorter stories in general, and especially
for
interpretive, as opposed to factual, content. To test this
hypothesis, we
computed a general linear model (multivariate analysis of
variance)
with the number of speaker-generated facts and evaluations as
depen-
dent variables, listener condition and gender as between-
subjects
variables, and type of information (facts or evaluations) as a
within-
subjects variable. The results revealed significant main effects
of
condition, F(1, 34) � 5.2, p � .03, �p
2 � .13, and type of information,
F(1, 34) � 6.0, p � .01, �p
2 � .15. We also computed simple effects
tests examining the effect of listener condition within each type
of
information separately. These contrasts revealed that distracted
listen-
ers elicited significantly fewer facts, F(1, 34) � 5.2, p � .03,
�p
2 �
.14, and tended to elicit fewer interpretations, F(1, 34) � 2.9, p
� .10,
�p
2 � .08 (see Table 1). These effects are shown in Figure 1. As
seen
here, factual information predominated. Distracted listeners
elicit less
information overall, and given the absence of a significant
interaction
between information type and listener condition, the
information
decrement appears consistent across information type, contrary
to our
hypotheses.
Are Speakers Aware of the Impact of Listeners
on Their Stories?
To evaluate whether participants were aware of the impact of
listeners on their stories, we computed an analysis of variance
examining perceived story quality as a function of listener
condi-
tion and gender. There were no differences in self-reported
story
quality as a function of listener condition, F(1, 36) � 1, ps �
.40
(attentive: M � 3.8, SD � 1.4; distracted: M � 3.8, SD � 1.0).
There were also no significant effects involving gender.
Cross-Sectional Age Differences in Story Content
To examine age differences in the content of participants’ sto-
ries, we computed a partial correlation between age and factual
and
interpretive units, controlling for the perceived attentiveness of
listeners. Older participants tended to report more facts than
younger participants, r(35) � .31, p � .07, but they did not
differ
significantly in their inclusion of interpretive information, r(35)
�
.18, p � .20. These correlations are not significantly different
from
each other, however, and with greater power both types of
infor-
mation may in fact increase with age.
Study 2
The results of Study 1 suggested that (a) late adolescents and
emergent adults constructed less rich and elaborated stories
when
they spoke to distracted friends; (b) these same participants’
rat-
ings of their own stories did not differ as a function of their
friends’ behavior; and (c) across even this restricted age range
and
with limited power, there were possible age differences in the
elaboration of factual information in conversational narration.
Our initial expectation, that distracted listeners would be
especially
problematic in relation to interpretive content, was not
confirmed.
However, we had relatively little statistical power given our
small
sample size and, in particular, given that the stories told in this
study
had more factual than interpretive content. Further, we had
examined
only positive events in this study, whereas earlier work on
narrative
identity emphasized the potential for negative events to involve
self-
construction in childhood and, to some extent, in adulthood
(Bird &
Reese, 2006; Thorne et al., 2004). Thus, in Study 2, we
expanded our
focus to examine both positive and negative events. Participant
pairs
were randomly assigned to a positive or negative event
condition.
Moreover, the addition of a negative event condition also
increased
our statistical power for examining age differences. Aside from
this
change, Study 2 was an exact replication of Study 1.
Method
Participants
Participants in this study were 81 same-gender friend dyads
(50% men, 50% women) recruited from the introductory
psychol-
ogy subject pool and through flyers posted at the University of
Utah. The average age of speakers was 19.6 years (SD � 1.9),
again, firmly within the age range of late adolescence and early
adulthood. Within dyads, participants had known each other an
average of 4.7 years (SD � 6.0).2 One pair was excluded
because
the speaker was not a native speaker of English, leaving a total
sample of 80 pairs. The sample predominantly comprised Euro-
pean American participants (76.5%), with Hispanic participants
composing an additional 12.3% of the sample. Participants were
randomly assigned to one of two event-valence conditions (posi-
tive or negative) and to one of two listener conditions (attentive
or
distracted). As in Study 1, there were no differences by
condition
in the length of time participants had known one another or in
their
ratings of relationship quality.
2 The length of time that partners had known one another and
ratings of the
quality of their relationship did not influence the results
presented below.
Table 1
Means and Standard Deviations of Dependent Measures in
Study 1
Measure
Attentive Distracted
M SD M SD
Facts 40.1 31.6 21.0 15.0
Evaluations 29.1 23.8 17.7 15.1
562 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
Procedure
The procedure was identical to that in Study 1, except that half
the participants assigned to the speaker role were asked to
identify
recent, undisclosed negative events, and the other half were
asked
to identify recent, undisclosed positive events.
Measures
Manipulation check. Speakers rated their perceptions of the
listeners’ responsiveness across the same five items used in
Study
1 (Cronbach’s � � .83) and rated their perception of the
listeners’
agreement with their views on a 7-point Likert-type scale,
again, as
in Study 1.
Speaker’s perceptions of story quality. Speakers rated their
perceptions of story quality along the same nine items employed
in
Study 1, and we averaged the items to create an index of
perceived
story quality. The internal consistency of this measure was
lower
in this study than in Study 1 (Cronbach’s � � .68).
Coding of conversations. Coding employed the same system
as in Study 1. Twenty-five percent (n � 20) of the conversation
pairs were coded independently to quantify reliability; coders
agreed on about 82% of 1,169 idea units (� � .75). As in Study
1,
we computed the number of speaker-generated event facts and
evaluations as primary dependent measures.
Results
Manipulation Check
A general linear model testing for effects of listener condition
(attentive vs. distracted), event valence (positive or negative),
and
gender on the speaker’s perceptions of the agreeableness and
responsiveness of the listener revealed a main effect of listener
condition, F(2, 72) � 21.4, p � .01, �p
2 � .37, and a main effect
of gender, F(2, 72) � 4.3, p � .02, �p
2 � .11. Examination of the
univariate effects for these two main effects suggested that
listener
condition influenced perceptions of listener responsiveness,
F(1,
73) � 42.9, p � .01, �p
2 � .37, but not perceptions of listener
agreement, F(1, 73) � 1.2, p � .20. As expected, listeners in the
distracted condition were perceived as less responsive (M �
3.6,
SD � 1.2) than listeners in the responsive condition (M � 5.3,
SD � 1.2). Gender was also related to perceptions of listener
responsiveness, F(1, 73) � 4.8, p � .05, �p
2 � .06, but not to
perceptions of listener agreement, F(1, 73) � 1.8, p � .15.
Across
conditions, women rated their listeners (who were also women)
as
more attentive (M � 4.7, SD � 1.6) than did men (M � 4.2, SD
�
1.3). Our manipulation of listener behavior was successful, as
perceived by the speakers, but had a more narrowly focused
impact
on responsiveness than in Study 1. Note that even testing for
effects of listener condition on agreeableness within the
positive
event condition did not result in perceptions that listeners
differed
in agreeableness. Differences in attentiveness by gender did not
interact with the manipulation.
Do Speakers Change Their Story Depending on
Listener Condition and Event Valence?
We conducted a general linear model with information type
(factual, interpretive) as a within-subjects factor and gender,
lis-
tener condition, and event valence as between-subjects factors.
There were main effects of type of information, F(1, 72) � 4.4,
p � .05, �p
2 � .06, and gender, F(1, 72) � 4.0, p � .05, �p
2 � .05,
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
50
snoitaterpretnIstcaF
Type of Information
N
um
be
r
of
Id
ea
U
ni
ts
Attentive
Distracted
Figure 1. Effect of distracted versus attentive friends on
information during conversational remembering in
Study 1. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.
563NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
and a trend toward a main effect of listener condition, F(1, 72)
�
3.8, p � .06, �p
2 � .05. In addition, there was an interaction of
listener condition and type of information, F(1, 72) � 5.6, p �
.03,
�p
2 � .07.
In contrast to the results of Study 1, conversations focused more
on interpretive information (M � 33.8, SD � 48.8) than on facts
(M � 23.9, SD � 23.4), and women produced a greater number
of
units on average, across all types of information (M � 71.6, SD
�
85.1) than did men (M � 43.1, SD � 23.2), which is consistent
with the fact that women in this study rated their listeners as
more
attentive than did men across all conditions. Attentive friends
tended to elicit longer accounts (M � 72.0, SD � 85.2) than did
distracted listeners (M � 43.4, SD � 26.4), but this effect was
qualified by an interaction with information type. To follow up
the
interaction effect, we tested the simple effect of listener
condition
within each type of information. Only one of these simple
effects
tests was significant: that for interpretive information, F(1, 72)
�
5.3, p � .03, �p
2 � .07. Participants told attentive friends nearly
twice as many interpretive units (M � 45.7, SD � 64.9) than
they
told to distracted listeners (M � 21.1, SD � 16.9). Figure 2
displays the amount of information contained in participants’
stories as a function of information type and listener condition.
Thus, there were no significant effects of listeners on factual
content and no effects of valence on memory content. See Table
2.
Are Speakers Aware of the Impact of Listeners
on Their Stories?
As in Study 1, we examined whether speakers’ perception of the
quality of their story differed significantly as a function of
listener
condition, event valence, or gender. There were no significant
main effects or interactions, and speakers in the two listener
conditions rated their stories quite similarly (attentive: M �
4.3,
SD � 1.3; distracted: M � 4.0, SD � 1.0).
Cross-Sectional Age Differences in Story Content
As in Study 1, we computed a partial correlation between age
and factual and interpretive information scores, controlling for
perceptions of listener attentiveness and this time, based on the
results above, for gender. Age was again significantly and posi-
tively correlated with the inclusion of factual information, r(76)
�
.23, p � .05, and was not significantly correlated with the inclu-
sion of interpretive information, r(76) � �.04, p � .50.
To summarize, the results of Study 2 showed that (a) late
adolescents and emergent adults told significantly less
interpretive
stories to distracted friends than to attentive friends, across both
positive and negative events; (b) these same participants were
not
aware of the changes in the stories they told; and (c) once again,
older participants, compared with younger participants, told
stories
that involved more factual content. We also observed main
effects
of gender on both perceptions of listener responsiveness and the
overall length of stories. These main effects were quite
congruent:
Women in this study perceived their listeners as more
responsive
and produced longer stories.
In contrast to the results of Study 1, participants did not reduce
the factual content of their stories when talking to distracted
friends. Notably, distracted friends in this study were perceived
as
less responsive but as equally agreeable. It is possible that when
people perceive their listener to disagree, they repeat and
otherwise
emphasize interpretive content to persuade or convince listeners
to
accept their perspective. Thus, in Study 1, listeners’
unresponsive-
ness and disagreeableness may have had opposing effects on
interpretive information, diminishing the impact of
responsiveness
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
snoitaterpretnIstcaF
Type of Information
N
um
be
r
of
Id
ea
U
ni
ts
Attentive
Distracted
Figure 2. Effect of distracted versus attentive friends on
information during conversational remembering in
Study 2. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.
564 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
on this type of information. Notably, earlier work on film clips,
in
which participants perceived a listening stranger as both
distracted
and disagreeable, obtained findings similar to those of Study 1
(Pasupathi et al., 1998)—significant decrements in both factual
and interpretive information.
Work on children’s personal storytelling typically examines
naturally occurring variability in maternal responsiveness and
elaborativeness; elaborative mothers usually combine warm,
sup-
portive, and generally agreeable attitudes with high levels of
attentiveness to the child’s story. Listening in adulthood
likewise
combines responsiveness with warmth and agreeableness under
most circumstances but not all (see, e.g., Pasupathi et al.,
1999).
Ideally, the ability to disentangle responsiveness and agreement
experimentally would permit us to explore more directly the im-
plications of responsiveness, as distinct from agreement, for the
types of information participants include when constructing a
personal story in conversations. Study 3 was designed to
attempt to
disentangle the implications of agreement from listeners and re-
sponsiveness from listeners for the construction of narrative
iden-
tity and to do so in an experimentally controlled manner. This
disentangling was motivated by the desire to reconcile the con-
flicting findings from Study 1 and Study 2. In addition,
however,
responsiveness combined with disagreement may have some of
its
own particular developmental implications, perhaps especially
in
the context of the dynamics of individuation that are so
important
for adolescents and young adults. Thus, although the primary
purpose of our project was to examine variations in listener re-
sponsiveness, variations in listener agreement with the speaker
may be relevant for the development of narrative identity in a
distinctive way, and Study 3 also provides preliminary evidence
of
that.
Study 3
The major task in Study 3 was to find a way to examine how
unresponsive and disagreeable listening differentially influence
the
construction of identity in conversational narratives. One way to
approach this was to borrow from experimental social
psychology
and to create an experience in our laboratory that permitted
some
conversational narration and that was at least potentially
relevant
to narrative identity construction but also somewhat
standardized
across participants. We did this by asking participants to try a
computer game called Sims (Electronic Arts, Redwood City,
CA)
for the first time. First-time activities, though not of great
impor-
tance in an individual’s life, offer opportunities for thinking
about
new aspects of one’s identity and for construing novel situations
in
terms of enduring identities.
The Sims game was also ideal for our purposes because it
entails
being a simulated individual. Players must “live” in the game by
taking care of basic needs such as eating, bathing, and toileting,
as
well as more elaborate needs for income via work, social
interac-
tion, and self-enhancement. Needs, indicated via bars on the
screen, are addressed by choosing to take particular actions
(e.g.,
clean the apartment, call a friend). Unpredictable, random
events
such as fires, burglaries, and loss of a job occur and must be
dealt
with by the player. The game has relatively high appeal for
women, in contrast to many computer games, and permits some
degree of identity investment on the part of players.
Participants all played the game for the first time, and were then
asked to talk about this experience with a same-gender friend,
following procedures similar to those employed in Studies 1 and
2.
However, because pilot work showed that such conversations
nearly always revolved around evaluating whether the game was
fun, we were able to create a condition in which listeners could
disagree with speakers’ views of the game. This was done by
asking players to evaluate the game just after playing. The
players’
evaluation was then employed to instruct their friends to
convince
them of the opposite view: If players thought the game was fun,
as
most participants did, then friends were asked to express the
view
that the game was boring.
This procedure raised three methodological issues. First, are
descriptions of playing a computer game for the first time
analo-
gous to stories about real personal experiences? Second, in
those
descriptions, how does one address information about how the
game is played, which is neither factual nor, in the usual sense
of
our coding scheme, interpretive? And third, does disagreement
about the evaluation of the game change the storytelling task to
one involving persuasion in ways that would undermine compar-
isons with the previous two studies?
With respect to the first issue, first-time experiences with an
activity, of which the present experiment can be seen as an
example, are in fact among the personal experiences that we
frequently disclose to others. Recently collected data in our
labo-
ratory showed that of 59 people asked to report on a recent
(within
the last 2 weeks) novel experience, 53 had already told others
about that event, and across different types of events (self-
discrepant, self-typical, repeated, and novel), novel events were
the most likely to have been disclosed to others. So storytelling
about new experiences is an ecologically valid subcategory of
the
more general category of personal storytelling. Moreover, it is
likely that storytelling about first-time experiences is a very im-
portant aspect of the construction of narrative identity, in the
sense
that such storytelling may revolve around the potential role that
the
Table 2
Means and Standard Deviations of Dependent Measures in
Study 2
Measure
Attentive Distracted
Positive events Negative events Positive events Negative events
M SD M SD M SD M SD
Facts 26.3 23.0 24.8 26.0 20.5 19.2 24.2 14.1
Evaluations 44.0 52.6 49.0 76.5 22.6 17.3 19.5 16.8
565NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
new experience or activity may or may not play in a person’s
life
(see Thoman et al., 2007).
To address the second issue, we expanded our coding scheme to
include a category called schematic inferences. This category
included idea units that referred to information about how the
game could be played, such as the rules, goals, and
opportunities
within the game. Examples of this category included such state-
ments as “You gotta spend all this time beefing up your dude,”
“You’re just watching her go through her daily routine,” and
“You
have to make friends.” Such information was not, strictly
speaking,
factual in the sense of reporting on what happened during game
play. Like interpretive information, it required inferences based
on
the participants’ experience, but unlike interpretive information,
those inferences were about how to succeed at the game, rather
than about the broader category of interpretations related to the
participants’ experiences and evaluations. As a consequence, we
were able to examine how variations in listener behavior influ-
enced three types of information: factual details about events
that
occurred during the participants’ game experience, schematic
in-
ferences about how the game works, and interpretive
information
about the overall game experience, including the extent to
which
the game was fun or interesting to play. Note that schematic
information is also an aspect of personal remembering that has
been extensively examined in early childhood, and when young
children narrate novel experiences, they often focus extensively
on
this type of information (e.g., P. J. Bauer, 2006). Thus, this
addition also permitted Study 3 to explore the construction of
schematic knowledge in the context of responsive and unrespon-
sive listening in a sample of early adults.
The third issue, which is whether disagreement about the eval-
uation of the game changes a storytelling task to a persuasion
task,
is crucial. However, most remembering to friends or family re-
volves around making some type of interpretive claim and
backing
it up with selected facts (Hyman, 1994). In other words, the
storytelling in Studies 1 and 2 also reflected attempts to tell a
story
that made one or another point, but the specific points at stake
were
heterogeneous given the varied events participants chose.
Method
Participants
Participants in this study were 104 same-gender friend dyads
(49% men, 51% women) recruited from the introductory
psychol-
ogy subject pool at the University of Utah. The average age of
speakers was 20.8 years (SD � 3.4), again, consistent with the
targeted age range. Within dyads, participants had known one
another an average of 4.6 years (SD � 5.5).3 This sample
predom-
inantly comprised European American participants (88.5%).
Par-
ticipants were randomly assigned to one of five conditions:
atten-
tive agreeable (n � 20), attentive disagreeable (n � 22),
distracted
(n � 21), distracted agreeable (n � 22), and distracted disagree-
able (n � 20). On the basis of initial analyses of our
manipulation
check measures, we collapsed these categories into attentive–
agreeable (n � 20), attentive– disagreeable (n � 22), and dis-
tracted (n � 63) conditions.
Procedure
Upon arrival, participants were assigned to speaker and listener
roles. As in previous studies, the pairs were separated to
complete
background measures not included in the present study. In
contrast
to what participants did in previous studies, rather than generate
a
list of recent experiences, speakers played the Sims game for 15
min. The experimenter first explained the video game and pro-
vided brief instructions. All participants began the game at an
identical starting condition, in terms of simulated money, skills,
and status; women played a female character and men a male
character. After playing the game, participants completed ques-
tionnaires assessing interest in the game.
During this game play time, listeners were given separate in-
structions. Agreeable and attentive friends and distracted
listeners
were given instructions as in Studies 1 and 2. Disagreeable and
attentive friends were told that the speaker liked or disliked the
game, according to the speaker’s ratings of the game postplay,
and
were asked to convince the speaker of the opposite evaluation.
Distracted–agreeable and distracted– disagreeable listeners were
asked to practice either mild confirmatory responses (e.g.,
“yeah”)
or mild disconfirming responses (e.g., “hmm”) while engaging
in
the same practice of counting th words as the distracted
listeners.
Measures
Manipulation check. As in Studies 1 and 2, speakers rated
listeners’ responsiveness along the same 7-point Likert-type
items
indexing indicators of listener responsiveness (Cronbach’s � �
.82). Because agreement from listeners was being manipulated
in
this study, we added two items assessing listener concordance
(“Your friend supported your opinion of Sims” and “Your friend
agreed with your view of Sims”), again rated on 7-point Likert-
type scales in which low ratings indicated no concordance and
high ratings indicated higher concordance. These items were av-
eraged to provide a measure of perceived listener concordance
(Cronbach’s � � .84).
Speaker’s perceptions of story quality. The same nine items
used in Studies 1 and 2 were averaged to create an assessment
of
overall story quality (Cronbach’s � � .87).
Coding of transcripts. Each transcription of a conversation
was transcribed and divided into idea units by undergraduate
research assistants. These idea units were coded for content
through the same coding scheme used in Studies 1 and 2, with
the
addition of the schematic inference category. Weekly meetings
were held in which problematic coding units were discussed and
agreement on content was sought. Twenty percent of the conver-
sation pairs (n � 20) were coded independently to quantify reli-
ability. Intercoder reliability for Study 3 across the detailed
coding
scheme was good (83% agreement; � � .79).
Results
Manipulation Check and Preliminary Analyses
As noted, participants were assigned into three conditions: an
attentive and agreeable condition (n � 20 pairs), an attentive
but
disagreeable condition (n � 22 pairs), and a distracted condition
(n � 63 pairs). Reanalyzing the manipulation checks revealed
that
the two attentive conditions differed from the distracted
condition
3 Again, the amount of time known and the quality of the
relationships
did not affect the results presented for this study.
566 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
on attentiveness alone, that the distracted and disagreeable
condi-
tions together differed from the attentive–agreeable condition
on
agreement alone. In other words, the distracted condition in this
study, as in Study 1, is better described as a distracted–
disagreeable condition. Although the full disentangling of
agree-
ableness and attentiveness that we attempted was not achieved,
the
present study did succeed in providing a partial disentangling of
agreement and attentiveness. Specifically, the disagreeable
condi-
tion combined disagreement with high responsiveness, whereas
the
distracted condition reflected perceptions of disagreement with
low responsiveness. The attentive and agreeable condition
resulted
in listeners perceived as highly responsive and highly agreeable.
Do Speakers Change Their Story Depending on Listener
Behavior?
We conducted a general linear model with information type
(factual, game schematic, and interpretive) as a within-subjects
factor and listener condition (attentive–agreeable, attentive–
disagreeable, and distracted) and gender as between-subjects
fac-
tors. The results suggested a significant main effect of
condition,
F(2, 90) � 8.4, p � .01, �p
2 � .16, as well as interactions involving
listener condition and type of information, F(4, 180) � 5.7, p �
.01, �p
2 � .11, and listener condition, type of information, and
gender, F(4, 180) � 2.8, p � .03, �p
2 � .06.
The main effect of listener condition was reflected by the fact
that across all types of information, participants constructed
stories
with more information of all types when they had an attentive–
agreeable (M � 31.3, SEM � 3.6) or attentive– disagreeable (M
�
31.0, SEM � 3.6) listener, in contrast to stories constructed for
a
distracted– disagreeable friend (M � 17.7, SEM � 2.1). The
two
attentive conditions did not differ from each other. The Condi-
tion � Type of Information interaction, however, suggested that
this difference was not consistent across the different types of
information.
Post hoc comparisons examining the effect of listener condition
separately for each type of information suggested that listener
condition significantly affected all three types of information,
3.4 � F(2, 90) � 13.5, ps � .05, �p
2 � .07. For facts about the
game, participants constructed more elaborated stories for
attentive–agreeable friends (M � 45.1, SEM � 6.9), compared
with attentive– disagreeable friends (M � 22.3, SEM � 6.9) and
distracted– disagreeable friends (M � 26.5, SEM � 4.0), who
had
similar effects. Differences between the attentive–agreeable
con-
dition and the other two conditions were marginally significant
( ps � .07) based on pairwise comparisons.
For schematic inferences about the game, participants con-
structed stories with more schematic inferences for attentive–
disagreeable listeners (M � 35.7, SEM � 3.8), followed by
attentive–agreeable listeners (M � 23.9, SEM � 3.8) and
distracted– disagreeable listeners (M � 14.8, SEM � 2.2).
Pairwise
comparisons showed that the difference between the attentive–
disagreeable and distracted– disagreeable condition was
significant
( p � .01), whereas the difference between the attentive–
agreeable
and attentive– disagreeable condition was marginal ( p � .09),
and
the difference between the attentive–agreeable and distracted–
disagreeable condition was not significant ( p � .12).
Interpretive information followed a highly similar pattern, with
attentive– disagreeable listeners eliciting the highest number of
such statements (M � 34.5, SEM � 4.0), followed by attentive–
agreeable listeners (M � 25.1, SEM � 4.0) and distracted
listeners
(M � 11.8, SEM � 2.3). In this case, pairwise comparisons
showed no significant differences between the attentive–
agreeable
and attentive– disagreeable conditions ( p � .27), but the
distracted
condition differed significantly from both of the other
conditions
( ps � .02).
Finally, to explore the three-way interaction with gender, we
again examined the effect of listener condition separately for
each
type of information, but this time we also did this separately for
men and women. The resulting contrasts showed effects of con-
dition on schematic information for men, F(2, 90) � 3.8, p �
.03,
�p
2 � .08, and women, F(2, 90) � 8.6, p � .01, �p
2 � .16, and on
interpretive information for both men, F(2, 90) � 5.2, p � .01,
�p
2
� .10, and women, F(2, 90) � 10.3, p � .01, �p
2 � .19. However,
for factual information, whereas listener condition had a signifi-
cant impact on men, F(2, 90) � 5.1, p � .01, �p
2 � .10, there was
no comparable effect for women, F(2, 90) � 1. Pairwise
compar-
isons of men and women across all other cells of the design
revealed one marginal difference and one significant difference
between men and women. Both differences were evident in the
attentive condition. Men in the attentive condition tended to
pro-
duce more factual accounts than women ( p � .07) and produced
significantly less interpretive accounts ( p � .02). This
difference
in the baseline, in which women produced relatively nonfactual
accounts, is the likely reason that listener condition did not sig-
nificantly influence factual content for women. These results
are
shown in Figure 3. Table 3 presents means and standard
deviations
for all types of information by gender and listener condition.
Are Speakers Aware of the Impact of Listeners on Their
Stories?
As in Study 1, speakers rated their stories along the same
indicators. An analysis of variance of overall story quality as a
function of gender and listener condition revealed no significant
differences in perceived story quality as a function of listener
condition, F(2, 96) � 2.6, p � .08, �p
2 � .05. There were also no
effects involving gender. Notably, this effect is a trend.
Examina-
tion of the estimated marginal means suggests that the overall
trend is due to the fact that stories told to attentive and
agreeable
listeners were rated more highly (M � 4.4, SEM � 0.2) than
stories told to distracted listeners (M � 3.8, SEM � 0.1). Exam-
ining the items that make up the composite story quality rating
suggested that only a single item was rated differently by
partic-
ipants with distracted listeners. Specifically, stories told to
distracted– disagreeable listeners were rated as significantly
less
emotional than stories told to attentive–agreeable listeners, al-
though not differently from stories told to attentive–
disagreeable
friends. In sum, as in Studies 1 and 2, participants did not
report
awareness of telling different stories to friends whose listening
behavior differed.
Cross-Sectional Age Differences in Story Content
As in Studies 1 and 2, we computed a partial correlation be-
tween age and factual, interpretive, and schematic units,
control-
ling for perceptions of listener attentiveness and gender based
on
the analyses above. Again, age was positively and significantly
567NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
correlated with factual content, r(92) � .30, p � .05, but not
with
interpretive content, r(92) � .03, p � .70, or schematic content,
r(92) � �.07, p � .40.
General Discussion
The present studies demonstrate that the development of narra-
tive identity during late adolescence and early adulthood, in the
microcontext of conversational storytelling, is shaped by the re-
sponsiveness, and perhaps the agreeableness, of listening
friends.
Specifically, by early adulthood, responsive listening is critical
for
the inclusion of interpretive information in storytelling. Given
that
interpretive information is important for long-term effects on
self
and identity assessed in a variety of ways (Bird & Reese, 2006;
McLean & Pratt, 2006; Pasupathi, 2007), the findings both
confirm
the role of relationships in identity development and point to a
process by which relationships influence identity development.
In Study 3, the effects of distracted listening were similar for
interpretive information and schematic inferences. The
literature
on the early childhood emergence of autobiographical
remember-
ing has addressed not only the emergence of self (e.g., Bird &
Reese, 2006) but also the emergence of scripts and schemas
(e.g.,
P. J. Bauer & Fivush, 1992). Conversations about a novel
activity
involve both scripts and interpretive content, and faced with un-
responsive friends, our late adolescent participants were
impaired
in both. Tentatively, this suggests that distracted listeners more
broadly suppress meanings, both those that revolve around
“what
I’m like” and those that refer to “how things get done.” Because
both of these kinds of meanings are important for
developmental
trajectories, this finding warrants further investigation across a
broader range of age groups, listeners, and activities.
The role of responsiveness on the construction of narrative
identity in the moment was distinguishable from agreement,
which
was related to whether participants constructed elaborated
factual
narratives. This has some important developmental implications
to
which we return below. Finally, these effects of listeners were
not
reducible to deliberate self-presentation, as our participants
were
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
Facts Schematic Interpretations Facts Schematic Interpretations
Males
Females
N
um
be
r
of
Id
ea
U
ni
ts
Attentive/Agreeable
Attentive/Disagreeable
Distracted
Figure 3. Effect of distracted versus attentive and attentive–
disagreeable listeners on information during
conversational remembering in Study 3. Error bars represent
standard errors of the mean.
Table 3
Means and Standard Deviations of Dependent Measures in
Study 3
Measure
Attentive Disagreeable Distracted
Men Women Men Women Men Women
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Facts 57.7 50.0 32.4 22.8 19.4 15.8 25.2 26.5 25.8 29.8 27.2
28.4
Schematic inferences 21.7 15.8 26.0 12.6 33.0 17.2 38.4 27.1
16.3 17.6 13.3 11.4
Evaluations 15.3 15.6 34.9 26.5 32.3 23.0 36.8 32.3 11.6 9.7
12.0 11.8
Note. Gender is included because this study revealed
differences between male and female conversations.
568 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
not aware of constructing different kinds of stories for different
listeners. Finally, consistent with the idea that this age period is
a
privileged one for autobiographical encoding and recall
(Holmes
& Conway, 1999; Rubin & Schulkind, 1997), we found consis-
tently small but reliable age-related increases in the factual
content
of memories.
Below we consider these findings in the context of their impli-
cations for narrative approaches to the development of memory
and identity (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Fivush &
Nelson,
2004; Hooker & McAdams, 2003; McLean et al., 2007;
Pasupathi,
Mansour, & Brubaker, 2007; Pasupathi & Rich, 2005). Through-
out, we discuss specific limitations in the areas where they are
relevant to our interpretations.
Developing Narrative Identity in Late Adolescence and
Early Adulthood
Despite the ubiquity of conversational remembering (e.g.,
Bohanek et al., 2006; Rimé, Finkenauer, Luminet, Zech, &
Philli-
pot, 1998), previous empirical work on narrative identity in
emer-
gent adulthood has focused primarily on written or interview-
based narratives, in which interviewers strive to avoid
influencing
speakers. Although narrative identity work clearly
acknowledges
the constructive nature of memory that has been part of
cognitive
work for decades (Bartlett, 1932/1995; Conway & Pleydell-
Pearce, 2000; Schacter, 1996), extant research emphasizes how
storytellers’ goals shape their remembering, ranging from
endur-
ing, characteristic motivations of rememberers (McAdams,
Hoff-
man, Mansfield, & Day, 1996; Moffitt & Singer, 1994; Woike,
1995; Woike, Gershkovich, Piorkowski, & Polo, 1999) to exper-
imentally induced motivations (Dudukovic, Marsh, & Tversky,
2004; McGregor & Holmes, 1999; Sanitioso, Kunda, & Fong,
1990; Tversky & Marsh, 2000). Although the existing sense of
self
or personality that a storyteller brings to narration is important
and
clearly influences subsequent remembering for adults (viz.
McLean, 2005; Pasupathi, Alderman, et al., 2007; Sutin &
Robins,
2005) and young children (Bird & Reese, 2006; Reese, Haden,
&
Fivush, 1993), the role of listeners has largely been ignored for
emergent adults.
Previous work examining conversational storytelling in this age
group has focused primarily on relationships between more
tradi-
tional self-conceptions and conversational storytelling. Those
find-
ings indicate that such stories are an important medium for the
development of self in social contexts among late adolescents
and
early adults (Pasupathi, 2006; Pasupathi, Alderman, et al., 2007;
Pasupathi & Rich, 2005; Thoman et al., 2007). The present
results
expand these findings to the interpretive aspects of
conversational
narrative and connect to work on early childhood storytelling,
memory, and self-development. Moreover, participants’
consistent
lack of awareness that their stories were changing for listeners
suggests that the effects of listeners on young adults’ stories
were
not a result of deliberate self-presentations; this is important
be-
cause social psychological work suggests that deliberate self-
presentation does not always exert influences on subsequent
self-
perceptions (e.g., see Rhodewalt, 1998, for a review).
From the childhood work, evaluative content, related to what we
have termed interpretive content, is the most important content
of
memory narratives for self-conception development (e.g., Bird
&
Reese, 2006). Interpretive aspects of remembering in early
child-
hood serve, in part, to reflect and create a sense of agency. As
children come to retain memories over longer periods, and to
think
of themselves as agents that endure across time, they begin to
construct an agent in their stories that has more long-lasting
qualities—an agent with beliefs, values, and long-term goals as
well as immediate emotions and desires (see Habermas & Bluck,
2000). From this perspective, listeners can help or hinder the
development of narrative identity in storytelling, in part by
either
supporting or constraining the agency of storytellers, regardless
of
whether the storyteller is a child or a young adult (see also
Cleveland & Reese, 2005). The experience of agency and its
extension in time, of course, change with development. But this
change is likely to be reciprocally interrelated with the process
of
narrating one’s experiences in ways that come to construct and
reflect a more enduring agency.
Agency in storytelling, however, is multilayered. It includes
both the agent within a story, who has feelings, beliefs, desires,
and identities expressed in interpretive content, and the
storyteller
him- or herself, who is giving voice to the story. The
interpretive
content of autobiographical memories is also the aspect of those
memories that is most unique and individuated. Thus, another
way
to think about distracted listening is that it silences the unique
voice of a storyteller. From this perspective, it is not only that
unresponsive listeners do not allow us to talk about what
experi-
ences mean for our self-views— how we as agents think, feel,
and
believe over time. It is that unresponsive listeners do not allow
us
to be who we are and who we are becoming, by telling stories in
ways that express and further that uniqueness.
Generally, these findings suggest continuity of developmental
processes, in that listeners were of great importance for the re-
membering of personal experiences in emergent adulthood. The
process of parent– child remembering is already known to be
critical in the development of narrative identity, memory, and
self
in early childhood (Fivush & Nelson, 2004). As children move
into
late adolescence and begin the transition to adulthood, the role
played by listeners in the development of narrative identity
may, in
fact, become more specialized. That is, in early childhood,
parents
help children learn what and how to recall about the personal
past,
and this means that parents, as listeners, are shaping both
narrative
capacity and narrative identity. Responsive, elaborative mothers
(and fathers) help children learn to tell elaborate, vivid,
detailed,
and interpretive stories about their past, and such children
develop
more advanced self-conceptions and identities in other ways as
well (Reese et al., 1993). By late adolescence, narrative
capacity is
present—all our participants were capable of telling an adequate
story about their personal experiences. However, the
responsive-
ness of listening friends continued to matter for the construction
of
stories that were richly reflective of narrative identity— of the
specific, individual experience of our participants.
Methodologically, however, and conceptually, our findings also
differ from prior work in childhood, and those differences bear
some consideration here. One issue concerns the comparability
of
distracted friends and low-elaborative mothers. In some ways,
these two groups can be conceived of as having similar effects,
in
that both groups’ behaviors may convey a lack of interest in the
construction of an elaborative, highly interpretive story. Other
work from our laboratory demonstrates relationships between
overall memory elaboration and naturally occurring variability
in
friends’ responsiveness but has not examined interpretive and
569NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
factual content separately (Liu, 2005). However, in other
respects,
these two groups are not the same. First, low-elaborative
mothers
are probably more responsive, within a storytelling interaction,
than distracted friends. Distracted friends are unresponsive on
average, to the entire story, whereas low-elaborative mothers
are
selectively responsive to specific aspects of events (often
factual in
published examples) that they construe as important but are
unre-
sponsive to other aspects in which their child may have more
interest. Thus, low-elaborative mothers may convey interest in a
preferred version of a story, whereas distracted listeners convey
no
interest. Both types of listeners may reduce the likelihood that a
person constructs an elaborate sense of narrative identity in the
moment but in somewhat distinct ways. Second, distracted
friends
in these studies were unresponsive on a single occasion but,
given
the generally established nature and high quality of these
friend-
ships, are probably more responsive on other occasions. Low-
elaborative mothers, by contrast, are seen as engaging in a
stylistic
approach to the construction of narrative identity with their
chil-
dren—an approach that has some consistency across various
rem-
iniscing occasions and over developmental time frames. This
brings up the larger issue, which is that the idea that
conversational
storytelling is a microcontext for the development of narrative
identity requires consideration of the ways in which that micro-
context is linked to more macrolevel developmental outcomes.
One possibility is that at least some people have friendships that
routinely and consistently discourage elaboration of interpretive
aspects of remembering. Although this may seem unlikely, con-
sider our gender effects in this context. In terms of storytelling
content, we observed only one gender difference: Women’s
stories
about a computer game differed from men’s, primarily in the
context of attentive friends. In that context, men’s stories were
more factual and women’s were more interpretive, possibly be-
cause of expertise in computer gaming generally, as expertise is
linked to the nature of storytelling about an event (e.g.,
Pasupathi,
Alderman, et al., 2007).
Other differences by gender, however, were more connected to
the idea of stable and consistent differences in the
responsiveness
of friends. Women also perceived their listeners as more
respon-
sive in general (Study 2) and specifically in the distracted
condi-
tion (Study 3). Further, women were more elaborative in general
(across factual and interpretive information) in Study 2,
suggesting
congruence between their perceptions and their construction of
narrative identity. Such findings are congruent with findings
about
parent– child reminiscing practices that differ between male and
female children (e.g., Fivush, 1998) and are potentially due to
real
differences in listener behavior by gender. Past work suggests
both
men and women prefer female listeners (Clark, 1994),
consistent
with the idea that women may be more responsive listeners in
general and perhaps are better able to remain somewhat
responsive
even in the face of dual task demands. This capacity, in turn,
suggests that the gender of friends may be connected to the
extent
to which those friends facilitate the construction of narrative
identity, and generates some predictions about the prevalence of
friendships with women and the construction of narrative
identity
in this age range.
Further, thinking about gender effects in this way suggests the
potential importance of looking at continuity and discontinuity
in
the kinds of listening done by good friends and the kinds of
parent– child reminiscing practices that characterized
someone’s
early childhood, as well as the parent– child reminiscing
practices
that characterize their current experience. Recent qualitative
work
(Weeks & Pasupathi, in press) suggests that at least in some
cases,
there may be interesting continuities between problematic
parent–
child reminiscing and problematic friend–friend reminiscing in
late adolescence, continuities broadly consistent with
attachment
perspectives on development (Hesse, 1999).
In addition to the possibility of some friendships entailing
consistently low elaboration, there is also the almost certain
like-
lihood that friends do engage in unresponsive listening with one
another periodically and that there are systematic features of
that
unresponsive listening. Although in the context of high-quality
relationships like those we examined here, friends are seldom
unresponsive across the board, they are certainly unresponsive
some of the time and to particular kinds of content over time. In
the
long run, repeated unresponsiveness likely serves to silence a
particular aspect of identity within that specific relationship
con-
text, and perhaps to silence that aspect of identity more broadly,
depending on the nature of that relationship for the person in-
volved. This set of implications is more consistent with the be-
havior of low-elaborative mothers, who are in some sense
shaping
a conversational story toward a particular, mother-driven form.
This kind of selective unresponsiveness can vary in its impact
on
young adults, from simply making it more difficult to express
and
explore, in reflection, one’s interest in particular experiences or
activities, as in the case of unpopular majors or hobbies, to a
real
experience of silencing, as in the case of minority sexual
identities.
The extent of the impact will also depend on the individual’s
motivation and capacity to seek out alternative social
relationships
within which particular identities can be elaborated, reflected
upon, and thus further developed—and indeed, on the very
avail-
ability of alternative relationships.
Factual Information: Development and Disagreement
The disagreeableness of listeners tended to suppress factual
content in remembering, and across all three of our studies,
older
participants consistently included more facts in their stories.
The
latter finding is consistent with the idea that memory
development
continues even into early adulthood (P. J. Bauer, 2006; Holmes
&
Conway, 1999; Rubin & Schulkind, 1997). Additionally, it is
broadly consistent with developments in knowledge about life
(e.g., Glueck & Baltes, 2006; Pasupathi, Staudinger, & Baltes,
2001) and in memory for news events, music, and other happen-
ings (e.g., Holmes & Conway, 1999) across this same age range.
This finding clearly suggests the potential for future work,
espe-
cially in looking at a broader age range and at more particular
types of information.
Factual information was suppressed in the context of disagree-
able listening for both men and women in Study 1, for men in
Study 3, and in past work (Pasupathi et al., 1998). Though
tenta-
tive, the suppression of factual information in disagreeable dis-
course is worth noting, because factual information can ground
interpretive information, and interpretations or evaluations
without
sufficient factual basis may be tenuously held, vulnerable to
vari-
ability over time or even simply not truly reflective of a
person’s
mental representation. This is the assumption behind the Adult
Attachment Interview, that broad statements about a “good
mother” are not plausible without being grounded in factual
details
570 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
(Hesse, 1999). Although interpretive information was our major
focus, the facts of our lives also have developmental import.
Further, it is worth noting that disagreement combined with re-
sponsiveness can facilitate narrative identity development very
well—a fact consistent with the earlier work of researchers
inter-
ested in autonomy and identity development (Cooper &
Grotevant,
1985) and an important reminder that responsiveness is not syn-
onymous with positivity or agreeableness.
Taken together, these studies demonstrate that the development
of
narrative identity during late adolescence and early adulthood,
at a
microlevel, is fundamentally intertwined with the extent to
which
listeners are responsive and encourage elaboration. Further
work that
attempts to examine the processes by which identity develops
needs to
take into consideration the context of conversational
storytelling and,
within that context, the collaboration of listeners. From
childhood
through adulthood, responsive listeners are critical for allowing
each
of us to express and further our own unique voice.
References
Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of
development from
the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55,
469 –
480.
Bartlett, F. C. (1995). Remembering: A study in experimental
and social
psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
(Origi-
nal work published in 1932)
Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2004a). Growth goals,
maturity, and
well-being. Developmental Psychology, 40, 114 –127.
Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2004b). Personal growth in
adults’ stories
of life transitions. Journal of Personality, 72, 573– 602.
Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2005). Interpreting the good
life: Growth
memories in the lives of mature, happy people. Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology, 88, 203–216.
Bauer, P. J. (2006). Remembering the times of our lives:
Memory in infancy
and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bauer, P. J., & Fivush, R. (1992). Constructing event
representations:
Building on a foundation of variation and enabling relations.
Cognitive
Development, 7, 381– 401.
Baumeister, R. F., Stilman, A., & Wotman, S. R. (1990). Victim
and
perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical
narra-
tives about anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
59,
994 –1005.
Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T. (2000). Listeners as
co-narrators.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 941–952.
Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the
development
of an autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42,
613– 626.
Bluck, S., & Glueck, J. (2004). Making things better and
learning a lesson:
Experiencing wisdom across the lifespan. Journal of
Personality, 72,
543–572.
Bohanek, J. G., Marin, K. A., & Fivush, R. (2006, March).
Family
narratives and adolescent identity. Paper presented at the
biennial meet-
ing of the Society for Research on Adolescence, San Francisco,
CA.
Buhrmeister, D. (1996). Need fulfillment, interpersonal
competence, and
the developmental contexts of early adolescent friendship. In
W. M.
Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The
company they
keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 158 –185).
Cam-
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, R. A. (1994). Children’s and adolescents’ gender
preferences for
conversational partners for specific communicative objectives.
Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 313–319.
Cleveland, E., & Reese, E. (2005). Maternal structure and
autonomy
support in conversations about the past: Contributions to
children’s
autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychology, 41, 376 –
388.
Constantinople, A. (1969). An Eriksonian measure of
personality devel-
opment in college students. Developmental Psychology, 1, 357–
372.
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The
construction of
autobiographical memories in the self-memory system.
Psychological
Review, 107, 261–288.
Cooper, C. R. (1999). Multiple selves, multiple worlds: Cultural
perspec-
tives on individuality and connectedness in adolescent
development. In
A. Masten (Ed.), Cultural processes in child development (pp.
25–57).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cooper, C. R., & Grotevant, H. D. (1985). Patterns of
interaction in family
relationships and the development of identity exploration in
adolescence.
Child Development, 56, 415– 428.
Daddis, C. (2008). Influence of close friends on the boundaries
of adoles-
cent personal authority. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
18, 75–98.
Dickinson, C., & Givon, T. (1995). Memory and conversation:
Toward an
experimental paradigm. In T. Givon (Ed.), Conversation:
Cognitive,
communicative, and social perspectives (pp. 91–132).
Amsterdam: Ben-
jamins.
Dudukovic, N. M., Marsh, E. J., & Tversky, B. (2004). Telling a
story or
telling it straight: The effects of entertaining versus accurate
retellings
on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 125–143.
Dusek, J. B., & McIntyre, J. G. (2003). Self-concept and self-
esteem
development. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.),
Blackwell
handbook of adolescence (pp. 290 –309). Malden, MA:
Blackwell.
Erikson, E. H., & Erikson, J. M. (1997). The life cycle
completed: Extended
version. New York: Norton.
Fivush, R. (1991). The social construction of personal
narratives. Merrill-
Palmer Quarterly, 37, 59 – 82.
Fivush, R. (1998). Gendered narratives: Elaboration, structure,
and emo-
tion in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. In
C. P.
Thompson, D. J. Herrmann, D. Bruce, J. D. Read, D. G. Payne,
& M. P.
Toglia (Eds.), Autobiographical memory: Theoretical and
applied per-
spectives (pp. 79 –104). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fivush, R., Bohanek, J., Robertson, R., & Duke, M. (2004).
Family
narratives and the development of children’s emotional well-
being. In
M. W. Pratt & B. H. Fiese (Eds.), Family stories and the life
course:
Across time and generations (pp. 55–76). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and language in the
emergence of
autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15, 586 –590.
Glueck, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Using the concept of wisdom
to enhance
the expression of wisdom knowledge: Not the philosopher’s
dream but
differential effects of developmental preparedness. Psychology
and Ag-
ing, 21, 679 – 690.
Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The
development of the
life story in adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 748 –
769.
Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing
narrative
structure in parent– child reminiscing across the preschool
years. Devel-
opmental Psychology, 33, 295–307.
Hair, E. C., Moore, K. A., Garrett, S. B., Ling, T., & Cleveland,
K. (2008).
The continued importance of quality parent-adolescent
relationships
during late adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
18, 187–
199.
Harley, K., & Reese, E. (1999). Origins of autobiographical
memory.
Developmental Psychology, 35, 1338 –1348.
Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In
N. Eisenberg
& W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed.,
Vol. 3, pp.
553– 617). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Harter, S., & Monsour, A. (1992). Developmental analysis of
conflict
caused by opposing attributes in the adolescent self-portrait.
Develop-
mental Psychology, 28, 251–260.
Hartup, W. W. (1996). The company they keep: Friendships and
their
developmental significance. Child Development, 67, 1–13.
Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1994). Sex and the psychological
tether.
Advances in Personal Relationships, 5, 151–177.
571NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
Hesse, E. (1999). The adult attachment interview: Historical and
current
perspectives. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
attachment:
Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 395– 433). New
York:
Guilford Press.
Holmes, A., & Conway, M. A. (1999). Generation identity and
the remi-
niscence bump: Memory for public and private events. Journal
of Adult
Development, 6, 21–34.
Hooker, K., & McAdams, D. P. (2003). Personality
reconsidered: A new
agenda for aging research. Journals of Gerontology: Series B:
Psycho-
logical Sciences and Social Sciences, 58B, P296 –P304.
Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Peterson, C. (1994). How can I
remember
when “I” wasn’t there: Long-term retention of traumatic
experiences and
emergence of the cognitive self. Consciousness and Cognition,
3, 327–
355.
Hyman, I. E. (1994). Conversational remembering: Story recall
with a peer
versus for an experimenter. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8,
49 – 66.
King, L. A., & Patterson, C. (2000). Reconstructing life goals
after the birth
of a child with Down Syndrome: Finding happiness and
growing.
International Journal of Rehabilitation and Health, 5(1), 17–30.
Kitchener, K. S., Lynch, C. L., Fischer, K. W., & Wood, P. K.
(1993).
Developmental range of reflective judgment: The effect of
contextual
support and practice on developmental stage. Developmental
Psychol-
ogy, 29, 893–906.
Korobov, N., & Bamberg, M. (2004). Positioning a “mature”
self in
interactive practices: How adolescent males negotiate “physical
attrac-
tion” in group talk. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 22,
471– 492.
Kroger, J. (2003). Identity development during adolescence. In
G. R.
Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of
adolescence
(pp. 205–226). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Liu, T. (2005). The planning function of imagined conversation.
Unpub-
lished master’s thesis, University of Utah.
McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied
self: A
contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological
Inquiry, 7,
295–321.
McAdams, D. P., Bauer, J. J., Sakaeda, A. R., Anyidoho, N. A.,
Machado,
M. A., Magrino-Failla, K., et al. (2006). Continuity and change
in the
life story: A longitudinal study of autobiographical memories in
emerg-
ing adulthood. Journal of Personality, 74, 1371–1400.
McAdams, D. P., Hoffman, B. J., Mansfield, E. D., & Day, R.
(1996).
Themes of agency and communion in significant
autobiographical
scenes. Journal of Personality, 64, 339 –377.
McAdams, D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A. H., &
Bowman, P. J.
(2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad:
Sequences
of redemption and contamination in life narrative and their
relation to
psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students.
Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 474 – 485.
McGregor, I., & Holmes, J. G. (1999). How storytelling shapes
memory
and impressions of relationship events over time. Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology, 76, 403– 419.
McLean, K. C. (2005). Late adolescent identity development:
Narrative
meaning making and memory telling. Developmental
Psychology, 41,
683– 691.
McLean, K. C., Pasupathi, M., & Pals, J. L. (2007). Selves
creating stories
creating selves: A process model of narrative self-development.
Person-
ality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 262–278.
McLean, K. C., & Pratt, M. W. (2006). Life’s little (and big)
lessons:
Identity statuses and meaning making in the turning point
narratives of
emerging adults. Developmental Psychology, 42, 714 –722.
McLean, K. C., & Thorne, A. (2003). Late adolescents’ self-
defining
memories about relationships. Developmental Psychology, 39,
635– 645.
McNulty, S. E., & Swann, W. B. J. (1994). Identity negotiation
in room-
mate relationships: The self as architect and consequence of
social
reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1012–
1023.
Meilman, P. W. (1979). Cross-sectional age changes in ego
identity status
during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 15, 230 –231.
Moffitt, K. H., & Singer, J. A. (1994). Continuity in the life
story:
Self-defining memories, affect, and approach/avoidance
personal striv-
ings. Journal of Personality, 62, 21– 43.
Pals, J. L. (2006a). Constructing the “springboard effect”:
Causal connec-
tions, self-making, and growth within the life story. In D.
McAdams, R.
Josselson, & A. Lieblich (Eds.), Identity and story: Creating
self in
narrative (pp. 175–199). Washington, DC: American
Psychological
Association.
Pals, J. L. (2006b). Narrative identity processing of difficult
life experi-
ences: Pathways of personality development and positive self-
transformation in adulthood. Journal of Personality, 74, 1079 –
1110.
Parker, J. G., & Gottman, J. M. (1989). Social and emotional
development
in a relational context: Friendship interaction from early
childhood to
adolescence. In T. J. Berndt & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer
relationships in
child development (pp. 95–131). Oxford, England: John Wiley
and Sons.
Pasupathi, M. (2001). The social construction of the personal
past and its
implications for adult development. Psychological Bulletin,
127, 651–
672.
Pasupathi, M. (2006). Silk from sows’ ears: Collaborative
construction of
everyday selves in everyday stories. In D. McAdams, R.
Josselson, & A.
Lieblich (Eds.), Identity and story: Creating self in narrative
(pp.
129 –150). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Pasupathi, M. (2007). Telling and the remembered self:
Linguistic differ-
ences in memories for previously disclosed and undisclosed
events.
Memory, 15, 258 –270.
Pasupathi, M., Alderman, K., & Shaw, D. (2007). Talking the
talk: Col-
laborative remembering and self-perceived expertise. Discourse
Pro-
cesses, 43, 55–77.
Pasupathi, M., Carstensen, L. L., Levenson, R. W., & Gottman,
J. M.
(1999). Responsive listening in long-married couples: A
psycholinguis-
tic perspective. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 173–193.
Pasupathi, M., & Mansour, E. (2006). Adult age differences in
autobio-
graphical narratives: Integrating experiences with the self.
Developmen-
tal Psychology, 42, 798 – 808.
Pasupathi, M., Mansour, E., & Brubaker, J. (2007). Developing
a life story:
Constructing relations between self and experience in
autobiographical
narratives. Human Development, 50, 85–110.
Pasupathi, M., & Rich, B. (2005). Inattentive listening
undermines self-
verification in personal storytelling. Journal of Personality, 73,
1051–
1086.
Pasupathi, M., Stallworth, L. M., & Murdoch, K. (1998). How
what we tell
becomes what we know: Listener effects on speakers’ long-term
mem-
ory for events. Discourse Processes, 26, 1–25.
Pasupathi, M., Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Seeds
of wisdom:
Adolescents’ knowledge and judgment about difficult life
problems.
Developmental Psychology, 37, 351–361.
Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging
narratives in
preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language,
26,
49 – 67.
Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (1993). Parental styles of talking about
the past.
Developmental Psychology, 29, 596 – 606.
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child
conversations
about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time.
Cognitive
Development, 8, 403– 430.
Rhodewalt, F. (1998). Self-presentation and the phenomenal
self: The
“carryover effect” revisited. In J. M. Darley & J. Cooper (Eds.),
Attri-
bution and social interaction (pp. 373–398). Washington, DC:
American
Psychological Association.
Rimé, B., Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Zech, E., & Phillipot, P.
(1998).
Social sharing of emotion: New evidence and new questions.
European
Review of Social Psychology, 9, 145–189.
Rubin, D. C., & Schulkind, M. D. (1997). Distribution of
important and
572 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-
old
adults. Psychology and Aging, 12, 524 –535.
Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z., & Fong, G. T. (1990). Motivated
recruitment of
autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychol-
ogy, 59, 229 –241.
Schacter, D. L. (1996). Searching for memory: The brain, the
mind, and the
past. New York: Basic Books.
Sutin, A. R., & Robins, R. W. (2005). Continuity and correlates
of
emotions and motives in self-defining memories. Journal of
Personality,
73, 793– 824.
Thoman, D. B., Sansone, C., & Pasupathi, M. (2007). Talking
about
interest: Exploring the role of social interaction for regulating
motivation
and the interest experience. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8,
335–370.
Thorne, A. (2000). Personal memory telling and personality
development.
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(1), 45–56.
Thorne, A., McLean, K. C., & Lawrence, A. M. (2004). When
remember-
ing is not enough: Reflecting on self-defining memories in late
adoles-
cence. Journal of Personality, 72, 513–541.
Tice, D. M. (1992). Self-concept change and self-presentation:
The looking
glass self is also a magnifying glass. Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology, 63, 435– 451.
Tversky, B., & Marsh, E. J. (2000). Biased retellings of events
yield biased
memories. Cognitive Psychology, 40(1), 1–38.
Updegraff, K. A., McHale, S. M., & Whiteman, S. D. (2006).
The nature
and correlates of Mexican-American adolescents’ time with
parents and
peers. Child Development, 77, 1470 –1486.
Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs:
Autobio-
graphical memory and self-description in European American
and Chi-
nese Children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 3–15.
Waterman, A. S. (1982). Identity development from adolescence
to adult-
hood: An extension of theory and a review of research.
Developmental
Psychology, 18, 341–358.
Weeks, T. L., & Pasupathi, M. (in press). Autonomy, identity,
and joint
autobiographical remembering in adolescence. In K. C. McLean
& M.
Pasupathi (Eds.), Narrative development in adolescence:
Creating the
storied self. New York: Springer.
Woike, B. A. (1995). Most memorable experiences: Evidence
for a link
between implicit and explicit motives and social cognitive
processes in
everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
1081–
1091.
Woike, B., Gershkovich, I., Piorkowski, R., & Polo, M. (1999).
The role of
motives in the content and structure of autobiographical
memory. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 600 – 612.
(Appendix follows)
573NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
Appendix
Sample Transcript With Coding
Example: Pair 29 Content
S: All right, well my experience was a couple days ago 1
we ran out of toilet paper. And 2
Mary was like, okay, 2
I’ll go get toilet paper, because 2
it’s her turn, right? 3
Yeah. 1
Ten minutes later, she’s like, hey Eileen, 2
can you get some toilet paper? 2
I was so mad. 3
Yeah, like, yesterday all day long she’s like, 2
I don’t have anything to do all day. 2
I’m like, why don’t you go get some toilet paper because 2
we’re out of toilet paper. 2
She’s like, no, 2
I just want to sit here and 2
play on the Internet. 2
L: Oh. 1
S: So mad. 3
Finally today I went and got it. 2
(inaudible) 9
I was like, napkins are good. 2
L: That’s smart. 3
S: Well, then there’s the part where 3
I’m the younger 3
(inaudible), 9
supposedly, because she’s twenty-six. 3
L: Oh. 1
S: And she’s like, no, 2
I don’t want to. 2
Being lazy. 3
She’s like, I’ll pay you back. 2
I’m like, Smith’s is two minutes down the road. 2
Go get some. 2
L: That’s funny. Funny. 3
Roommate troubles. 2
S: Drama, drama, drama 3
We’re done. 1
Note. S � speaker; L � listener; 1 � regulatory; 2 � event; 3 �
interpretive; 9 � uncodable.
Received August 9, 2007
Revision received September 29, 2008
Accepted October 14, 2008 �
574 PASUPATHI AND HOYT
Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme
Using Narrative Retells in Young
School-Age Children
John Heilmann
East Carolina University, Greenville, NC
Jon F. Miller
Ann Nockerts
University of Wisconsin—Madison
Claudia Dunaway
San Diego Unified School District, CA
Purpose: To evaluate the clinical utility of the
narrative scoring scheme (NSS) as an index of
narrative macrostructure for young school-age
children.
Method: Oral retells of a wordless picture book
were elicited from 129 typically developing chil-
dren, ages 5–7. A series of correlations and
hierarchical regression equations were completed
using microstructural measures of vocabulary and
grammar to predict NSS scores.
Results: While the NSS was significantly corre-
lated with age and each of the microstructural
measures, the hierarchical regression analyses
revealed a unique relationship between vocabu-
lary and narrative macrostructure.
Conclusion: The NSS is an efficient and informa-
tive tool for documenting children’s development
of narrative macrostructure. The relationship
between the NSS and microstructural measures
demonstrates that it is a robust measure of
children’s overall oral narrative competence and a
powerful tool for clinicians and researchers. The
unique relationship between lexical diversity and
the NSS confirmed that a special relationship
exists between vocabulary and narrative organi-
zation skills in young school-age children.
Key Words: narrative, language sample
analysis, story grammar, vocabulary
A
nalysis of oral narratives provides a rich source of
data that documents children’s language use in a
naturalistic context. Narrative analysis is a highly
effective clinical and research tool, as it allows examiners to
analyze multiple linguistic features simultaneously using a
single short sample. Examiners have the opportunity to doc-
ument children’s productive vocabulary and grammar using
microstructural analyses as well as children’s broader text-
level narrative organization skills by utilizing macrostructural
analyses (see Westby, 2005).
Microstructural analyses primarily focus on children’s lin-
guistic form and content, which are measured within individ-
ual utterances. Linguistic form is commonly assessed by
analyzing children’s grammatical and syntactic abilities using
mean length of utterance (e.g., Brown, 1973; Miller, 1981)
and various measures of sentence complexity (e.g., Nippold,
Hesketh, Duthie, & Mansfield, 2005; Schuele & Tolbert, 2001;
Scott & Stokes, 1995). Measures of linguistic content are used
to document children’s productive vocabulary skills, which
typically calculate children’s lexical diversity using measures
such as type–token ratio (Templin, 1957) and number of dif-
ferent words (Miller, 1987; Miller & Klee, 1995). While these
measures are the most commonly cited microstructural anal-
yses in the literature, this list is in no way exhaustive. Micro-
structural analyses continue to be reviewed, critiqued, and
reanalyzed (see Justice et al., 2006, for a review and
discussion).
Macrostructural analyses, on the other hand, examine
children’s language skills beyond the utterance level and
document children’s ability to relate concepts that transcend
the individual utterance. Most macrostructural analyses of
children’s narratives are rooted in the story grammar tradi-
tion, which proposes that all stories have a setting and episode
system (Mandler & Johnson, 1977; Rumelhart, 1975; Stein &
Glenn, 1979). The setting provides background information
Research
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19 •
154–166 • May 2010 • A American Speech-Language-Hearing
Association154
on the characters and their environment, while the episode
system includes three main components that occur in all
stories: (a) a problem (initiating event and/or internal response),
(b) attempts at solving the problem, and (c) consequences/
outcomes. To be a complete episode, a narrator must include
all three of these key components (see Strong, 1998, for a
review). These settings and episodes can be combined in an
infinite number of ways to create individual stories.
Relationship Between Microstructural and
Macrostructural Measures
Previous research has demonstrated that lexical and gram-
matical gains play an important role in children’s acqui-
sition of narrative proficiency (Berman & Slobin, 1994;
Bishop & Donlan, 2005). In their seminal work, Berman and
Slobin (1994) documented the role of linguistic forms taking
on new functions that aid in the organization of narratives.
This work described the trade-offs that occurred when differ-
ent microstructural features were used for different functions,
including the organization of narratives. Children’s narrative
organization skills were positively related to advances in use of
microstructural features, including grammatical forms (e.g.,
verb tense, aspect, and voice), lexical forms (e.g., lexical aspect
and manner/cause of verbs of motion), and lexico-grammatical
features (e.g., locative particles, prepositional phrases, and
connectives). This pattern of results is consistent with Slobin’s
long-standing claim that “new forms first express old func-
tions, and new functions are first expressed by old forms”
(Slobin, 1973, p. 184). Bishop and Donlan (2005) observed
that children’s ability to encode and retell a story was more
strongly related to linguistic ability than nonverbal IQ. Chil-
dren’s microstructures, including complex syntax and relating
causal concepts, were more influential in event memory and
story organization than their cognitive skills. These data docu-
menting the unique relationship between microstructures and
macrostructures are contrary to evidence demonstrating that
children’s narrative organization skills emerge from more
general cognitive capabilities, including executive function
(van den Broek, 1997), and sociolinguistic processes (Eaton,
Collis, & Lewis, 1999; McCabe, 1997; Peterson & McCabe,
2004; Quasthoff, 1997a, 1997b).
Narrative Skills of Children With Language Impairment
Given the strong relationship between microstructural and
macrostructural measures, it is not surprising that children
with language impairment have substantial difficulty using
appropriate vocabulary and grammar when telling stories
(Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Gillam & Johnston, 1992; Pearce,
McCormack, & James, 2003; Reilly, Losh, Bellugi, & Wulfeck,
2004) in addition to substantial difficulty with text-level orga-
nization of narratives (Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Manhardt
& Rescorla, 2002; Merritt & Liles, 1987; Pearce et al., 2003;
Reilly et al., 2004). The presence of both microstructural
and macrostructural deficits in children with language impair-
ment counters theories that identify the primary cause of
language impairment as deficiencies with grammatical com-
petence (e.g., Clahsen, 1989; Gopnik & Crago, 1991; Rice,
Wexler, & Cleave, 1995). Alternatively, the global narrative
difficulties of children with language impairment suggest that
their language deficits may be due to broader information-
processing deficits, such as a reduced processing capacity
(Boudreau, 2007; Colozzo, Garcia, Megan, Gillam, & Johnston,
2006). Further study of the relationship between children’s
microstructural and macrostructural language skills will pro-
vide a better understanding of the nature of the impairment.
Better understanding of children’s narrative skills in general,
and macrostructural skills in particular, also has substantial
clinical implications. Difficulty with narrative organization
can have a dramatic impact on children with language impair-
ment, as such discourse-level skills are required to effectively
communicate. While vocabulary and grammar deficits limit
children’s ability to produce fully competent utterances, im-
paired macrostructural skills affect children’s ability to gen-
erate coherent and age-appropriate extended discourse.
Furthermore, narratives are a major component of the school
curriculum, and children are expected to understand and use
appropriate narrative form effectively. Therefore, narrative
macrostructure skills must be efficiently and accurately docu-
mented, and should be considered within an extensive assess-
ment protocol for children with language difficulties.
Methods of Measuring Narrative Macrostructure
While most narrative macrostructure measures are rooted
in the story grammar tradition and share the same underlying
principles, the coding of narrative macrostructure varies
widely throughout the literature. Some narrative macro-
structure coding schemes documented children’s inclusion of
specific story grammar components (e.g., Strong, 1998) or
identified the presence or absence of specific story grammar
components for a given story (e.g., Berman, 1988; Boudreau
& Hedberg, 1999; Miles & Chapman, 2002; Reilly et al.,
2004). In these studies, children who produced more story
grammar components and/or more advanced story grammar
features were thought to have stronger narrative organization
skills. The second major class of macrostructural measures
used text-level judgments of children’s narrative proficiency
(Applebee, 1978; Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Stein, 1988).
Rather than identifying the presence or absence of specific
story grammar components, these measures required holistic
judgments by the examiner to rate the quality and develop-
mental level of the narrative.
The major advantage of the simple story grammar coding
procedures is that it facilitates a relatively high level of ac-
curacy across coders. There is less room for differences across
coders, as the coder is only responsible for identifying the
presence or absence of specific story-related themes. The
major disadvantage of simple story grammar analyses is that
they are potentially limited in their ability to account for the
abstract interutterance concepts and qualitative aspects of the
narrative, or the story’s “sparkle” (Peterson & McCabe, 1983).
McFadden and Gillam (1996) demonstrated that holistic rat-
ings of children’s narratives capture the more refined aspects of
narratives, such as charm and depth, and that these ratings were
better than simple story grammar analyses for documenting
differences between children with language impairment and
their typically developing peers between the ages of 9;0 (years;
months) and 11;7.
Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 155
Additional Skills Required to Tell an Effective Story:
Beyond Story Grammar
The development of narratives in children and adults has
been studied extensively and has revealed additional areas of
advancement beyond inclusion of story grammar features,
including children’s use of literate language and cohesive de-
vices (Bamberg & Damrad-Frye, 1991; Halliday & Hasan,
1976; Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Wigglesworth, 1997). Use of
literate language occurs when children use abstract language
features commonly used by teachers and found in the curri-
culum (Westby, 2005). Some key literate language features
related to narrative competence include use of metacognitive
verbs (e.g., think or know), metalinguistic verbs (e.g., say or
talk), and elaborated noun phrases (e.g., the boy in the restau-
rant with the frog; see Nippold, 2007, and Westby, 2005, for a
review). Bamberg and Damrad-Frye (1991) identified that
more sophisticated and later developing narratives included
abstract language such as metacognitive and metalinguistic
verbs. In their analysis of productions of Frog, Where Are
You? (Mayer, 1969), Bamberg and Damrad-Frye identified
that these abstract language skills emerged at age 5 years,
demonstrated robust development through adulthood, and
were essential for relating the hierarchical relationships be-
tween events in complex narrative productions. Additional
studies have identified that literate language skills were pres-
ent in children’s oral narratives during the preschool years
(Curenton & Justice, 2004), developed through the school
years and into adolescence (Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001;
Nippold, 2007; Pelligrini, Galda, Bartini, & Charak, 1998),
and were used less frequently by children with language im-
pairment (Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001).
Another high-level narrative feature that continues to develop
through the school years is the cohesiveness of children’s nar-
rative productions (Halliday & Hasan, 1976; Wigglesworth,
1997). To tell a story, narrators must effectively use cohesive
devices to carry concepts across individual utterances. Three
major categories of cohesive devices are (a) referential cohe-
sion, which allows a narrator to maintain appropriate refer-
ence to the characters, objects, and locations across utterances
using both noun phrases and pronouns; (b) conjunctive cohe-
sion, which allows a narrator to sustain concepts across phrases
and utterances using conjunctive words and phrases (e.g.,
and, but, besides, on the other hand, finally, in addition); and
(c) lexical cohesion, which allows a narrator to effectively use
vocabulary to link concepts across utterances. Measures of
cohesiveness can be a sensitive index of language use, as
children with language impairment have more difficulty with
correct use of cohesive ties (Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Liles,
1985; Strong & Shaver, 1991). While cohesion is often con-
sidered a microstructural measure, we treated it as a macro-
structural measure because aspects of cohesion transcend
individual utterances and are necessary for producing coher-
ent narratives.
The Narrative Scoring Scheme: A Comprehensive
Measure of Narrative Proficiency
Our goal in developing the narrative scoring scheme (NSS)
was to create a metric that documents the range of skills
required
for school-age children to effectively tell a coherent and in-
teresting story (see Appendix). To extend beyond simple story
grammar analyses, the NSS incorporates multiple aspects of
the narrative process into a single scoring rubric and provides
an overall impression of the child’s narrative ability. This
metric combines both the basic features of the story grammar
approaches as well as the higher level narrative skills that
continue to develop through the school-age years. In addition
to adding higher level narrative skills in the scoring scheme,
the NSS uses a combination of discrete coding criteria and
examiner judgment. The NSS was created to improve on the
simple story grammar measures by requiring the examiners to
make interutterance text-level judgments, which have been
shown to be more effective than discrete coding schemes in
identifying children with language impairment (McFadden &
Gillam, 1996). By breaking the judgments into seven skill
areas, examiners have the opportunity to reflect on each com-
ponent of the narrative process and judge the child’s profi-
ciency in that area. This combination of explicit scoring
guidelines and flexibility to allow for examiner judgment
reflects the hybrid nature of the NSS. The scores from the
seven NSS categories are combined to provide a single com-
posite score, which allows the examiner to generate an index
of children’s overall narrative ability.
The first step in developing the NSS was establishing the
key components from the story grammar literature, which
included the story’s introduction, the major conflicts and res-
olutions (conflict resolution), and a conclusion. To document
children’s use of literate language, the NSS includes catego-
ries that assess metacognitive and metalinguistic verbs. The
mental states component documents children’s abilities to use
metacognitive verbs (e.g., think and know) to describe the
characters’ thoughts and feelings. The characterdevelopment
component of the NSS also documents children’s literate
language skills by measuring the ability to use metalinguistic
verbs (e.g., talk and say), differentiate between main and sup-
porting characters, and talk in the first person to depict the
characters in the story. The NSS evaluates two separate as-
pects of cohesive ties that were adapted from Halliday and
Hasan (1976). The referencing component measures aspects
of referential cohesion, including appropriate use of pronouns
and antecedents. The cohesion component documents the
conjunctive and lexical aspects of cohesion, including appro-
priate ordering, emphasis of critical events, and transitions
between events.
Goals of the Study
Children’s performance on the NSS has been reported in
studies that examined the narrative organization skills of na-
tive Spanish-speaking children who were learning English
as a second language (Miller et al., 2006). The goal of the
present study was to describe the NSS from a clinical perspec-
tive and to further analyze the linguistic properties of the
measure in a group of children who were fluent in English.
Furthermore, the literature has revealed that there is a special
relationship between children’s microstructural and macro-
structural language skills (Berman & Slobin, 1994; Bishop &
Donlan, 2005). To better understand the linguistic properties of
the NSS and to extend our understanding of the relationship
156 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19
• 154–166 • May 2010
between microstructural and macrostructural measures, a
second goal was to document the relationship between chil-
dren’s vocabulary, grammar, and narrative organization skills.
To achieve this, we examined the relationship between mea-
sures of vocabulary, grammar, and the NSS. These analyses
further clarified the key constructs that the NSS is measuring
and also provided additional evidence for the role of vocabu-
lary and grammar in the development of young school-age
children’s narrative organization skills. This study addressed
the following questions:
1. Are age and measures of vocabulary and grammar signifi-
cantly correlated with NSS scores in narrative retells of
young school-age children?
2. Are measures of vocabulary uniquely related to NSS scores
in narrative retells of young school-age children?
3. Are measures of grammar uniquely related to NSS scores
in narrative retells of young school-age children?
Method
Participants
A total of 129 typically developing children age 5–7 years
were recruited for this study. The children were recruited from
public schools in the San Diego (CA) City School and Cajon
Valley School Districts. Administrators from the two districts
assisted with obtaining informed consent from each of the
children’s primary caregivers. The pool of potential partici-
pants was reviewed by the school’s speech-language pathol-
ogists (SLPs) and classroom teachers to identify children who
qualified for the study. To participate in the study, children
were required to have average scores on all summative class-
room, district, and state assessments. The classroom teachers
reviewed the records for each student to identify him or her
as average performing. The child’s academic record was also
reviewed to identify that he or she had no history of language
impairment and/or learning disability. While academic data
were used as inclusionary criteria for the participants, test
scores and descriptions of performance were not recorded and
were not available for further analysis. The majority of the
participants were native English speakers. A small percentage
of the children were Spanish/English bilingual and designated
as “fluent English.” This designation was made by the child’s
respective school district and was based on a passing grade on
an English proficiency test and grade-level academics. The
SLPs and teachers confirmed that each child met the inclu-
sionary criteria and enrolled the eligible children in the study.
The individual children participating in the study provided
verbal assent prior to completing the protocol.
Table 1 summarizes the demographic data for the partici-
pants. Sixty-one percent of the participants were in kinder-
garten, 36% of the children were in first grade, and 2% were
in preschool. The numbers of male and female participants
were roughly equal, with slightly fewer boys than girls. The
school SLPs attempted to recruit students who reflected the
racial and ethnic diversity of their schools. The SLPs first
identified the children’s ethnicity (Hispanic or not Hispanic)
and then documented the non-Hispanic children’s race. A
review of the race and ethnicity data revealed that the sample
is a relatively heterogeneous group. Socioeconomic status
was measured by calculating the highest number of years
of education that the child’s mother completed. On average,
mothers completed 14.4 years of education (SD = 2.5), with
a range of 9–20 years. The majority of the mothers completed
at least some college, while only eight of the children’s
mothers did not complete high school.
Procedure
Each participant completed a narrative retell of the word-
less picture book Frog, Where Are You? (Mayer, 1969). The
purpose of collecting the narratives was to establish a nor-
mative database reflecting typically developing children’s
oral narrative skills and to further our understanding of chil-
dren’s developing narrative competence. The head SLP from
each district was responsible for training 18 school-based
SLPs to elicit the oral narratives. The head SLP and clinicians
met on three separate occasions and had the opportunity to
practice the protocol with each other several times. The clini-
cians read the scripted instructions to the students and cued
them to listen to a taped version of the story while following
along with the pictures in the book. The students then retold the
story using the book as an aid. Examiner prompts were limited
to encouragement to begin the story and open-ended cues to
continue the retell. The scripted instructions and audiotaped
story script were adapted from the Strong Narrative Assess-
ment Procedure (Strong, 1998) and were used to facilitate
high fidelity among the numerous examiners completing the
language sample elicitation.
Transcription and Coding
The children’s narrative productions were digitally recorded
and sent to the Language Analysis Lab at the University of
Wisconsin—Madison, where they were transcribed by trained
research assistants who had at least 10 hr of transcription
TABLE 1. Demographic data for all participants.
Variable n
Grade
Preschool 3
Kindergarten 79
First grade 47
Sex
Female 69
Male 60
Race/ethnicity
White 87
Hispanic 16
Othera 15
African American 7
No data 4
Note. Ethnicity data were collected for children who were
Hispanic or
Latino. Race data are provided for all children who were non-
Hispanic/
non-Latino.
aOther races/ethnicities included Arabic (2), Chinese (3),
Japanese (2),
Korean (1), Filipino (5), Portuguese (1), and Samoan (1).
Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 157
experience using standard coding conventions for Systematic
Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) software (Miller &
Iglesias, 2008). Utterances were segmented into communi-
cation units (C-units; Labov & Waletzky, 1967), which in-
cluded a main clause and all dependent clauses. The transcripts
began and ended with the child’s first and last utterance, re-
spectively. See Miller and Iglesias (2008) for a full review of
the transcription conventions.
After completing the orthographic transcription, the research
assistant reviewed the transcript and completed the NSS. To
score the NSS, the transcriber carefully reviewed the narrative
transcript and assigned a score of 0–5 for each of the seven
categories summarized in the Appendix. Categories that could
not be scored received a score of zero or NA. Scores of zero
were given if the child did something that precluded the ex-
aminer from scoring a section of the NSS, such as skipping
a part of the story or refusing to complete the task. If there was
an error on the part of the examiner (e.g., not following the
protocol or problems with the recording), sections of the NSS
that were affected were not scored, and the examiner coded the
section as not applicable for analysis (i.e., NA). For all other
sections, scores of 1 reflected minimal presence/immature
performance, scores of 3 reflected emerging skills, and scores
of 5 reflected proficient performance. Transcribers also had
the opportunity to assign scores of 2 and 4 if performance was
judged to be between the major anchors. To improve the
accuracy of the scoring procedure, specific guidelines were
provided for scores of 1, 3, and 5 (see the Appendix). These
guidelines assisted the transcriber in assigning an accurate
score that reflected the child’s performance in each compo-
nent of the narrative process and reduced the abstractness of
the narrative macrostructure concepts. The scoring across the
seven categories received equal weighting because the liter-
ature revealed that each of these seven narrative aspects is
necessary for telling a well-developed story. Furthermore,
keeping the scoring rules straightforward and consistent
facilitates simple and accurate scoring. A comprehensive
training procedure is available on the SALT Web site (www.
saltsoftware.com/training/handcoded/) that includes an
overview of the NSS; scoring tips; a description of how to
enter NSS scores into a SALT file; excerpts from samples
demonstrating minimal/immature, emerging, and proficient
performance across each section of the NSS; and a set of
practice transcripts.
In addition to the NSS, the transcribers completed coding
for the subordination index (SI; Scott & Stokes, 1995; Strong,
1998). The transcribers added a code to each C-unit that sum-
marized the number of independent and dependent clauses.
C-units that were incomplete, unintelligible, or nonverbal, or
that had an error at the utterance level, were excluded from
the analysis. Utterance-level errors included incorrect word
order, omission of more than two words in an utterance, and
utterances that simply did not make sense. Elliptical responses
to
examiner questions were also excluded from the SI analysis.
Utterances in which the child inappropriately omitted the sub-
ject or copula were coded and received a score of zero. After
each C-unit was coded, the SI was generated by dividing
the total number of clauses (both main and subordinate) by
the total number of C-units. After all the narratives were
transcribed and coded, the transcripts were analyzed using
SALT (Miller & Iglesias, 2008), which produced a rectan-
gular data file summarizing each dependent measure for each
of the transcripts. This file was formatted for statistical anal-
ysis using SPSS Version 16.0.
Agreement
Accuracy of the transcription and coding process was ex-
amined at three levels. Protocol accuracy was calculated by
the principal investigator, who reviewed 10% of the written
transcripts to identify whether the transcribers were adhering
to the transcription conventions. Percentage agreement be-
tween the transcribers and principal investigator was 98%
for segmentation rules, 99% for word-level codes, and 98%
for coding of reduplications and reformulations (i.e., mazes).
To determine transcription accuracy and coding agreement,
10% of the narratives were independently transcribed and
coded for the NSS and SI by a second research assistant.
Transcription accuracy was calculated by comparing the in-
dependent transcripts at the word and morpheme level (94%
agreement), utterance segmentation decisions (98% agree-
ment), placement of mazes (93% agreement), and utterance
types (100% agreement).
Calculating agreement for the two coding schemes pre-
sented a greater challenge. Simple interrater agreement scores
can be misleading, as small differences between coders (e.g.,
NSS scores of 24 and 25) are treated the same as large dif-
ferences between coders (e.g., NSS scores of 16 and 28).
Therefore, agreement for the NSS and SI coding was calcu-
lated using Krippendorff’s alpha, which accounted for both
chance agreement and the degree of difference between tran-
scribers (Krippendorff, 1980). Alpha values accounting for
differences in ordinal data were calculated using the summed
NSS and SI scores that were calculated by the two independent
transcribers (a = .92 for SI; a = .79 for NSS). Krippendorff
established benchmarks for alpha values, with ≥.80 reflecting
adequate agreement and values between .67 and .80 reflect-
ing acceptable agreement for exploratory research and draw-
ing tentative conclusions. For a review of the accuracy and
agreement process and a further discussion of Krippendorff’s
alpha, see Heilmann et al. (2008).
Language Sample Measures
To test the relationship between microstructural measures
and the NSS, we used the following language sample mea-
sures that repeatedly have been found to be robust and devel-
opmentally sensitive to the population and context used in the
present study (i.e., typically developing 5–7-year-olds who
produced oral narratives):
Length/productivity. Number of total words (NTW) is a
measure of productivity that documents the amount of infor-
mation provided in the story (Allen, Kertoy, Sherblom, &
Pettit, 1994; Paul & Smith, 1993). In addition, NTW was used
to assist with statistically controlling sample length, which has
the potential to affect additional language sample measures.
Vocabulary. Number of different words (NDW) is a mea-
sure of lexical diversity that provides a robust estimate of
children’s productive vocabulary (Klee, 1992; Miller, 1987;
Miller & Klee, 1995) and has been widely used as an index of
158 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19
• 154–166 • May 2010
vocabulary skills in studies examining children’s oral nar-
rative skills (Gazella & Stockman, 2003; Humphries, Cardy,
Worling, & Peets, 2004; Swanson, Fey, Mills, & Hood, 2005;
Uccelli & Páez, 2007).
Grammar. Mean length of C-unit (MLCU) measures the
average number of morphemes that children use per C-unit
and is an index of general grammatical skills that increases with
age through the school-age years, particularly when analyzed
using narratives and expositories (Leadholm & Miller, 1992;
Nippold et al., 2005). Also, the SI, a measure of clausal density,
indicates the average number of subordinate clauses produced
per C-unit. Use of subordinate clauses emerges during the
preschool years (Diessel & Tomasello, 2000) and continues
to develop through the school-age years (Nippold, 2007).
Because the productions of oral narratives were relatively
short, issues of sample length required special consideration.
For conversational language samples, it is common practice to
control sample length across children by using a consistent
number of utterances, words, or elapsed time (e.g., 50 utter-
ances; Miller, 1981). In the present study, children produced
between 12 and 77 utterances. Using a standard transcrip-
tion cut of 50 utterances would limit the analyses to 11 tran-
scripts. If we chose to maintain 75% of the data, 97 transcripts
would remain in the analysis, and each transcript would be
cut at 29 utterances and would ultimately withhold 955 utter-
ances from the data set. Our goal was to maintain the maxi-
mum amount of data available by maintaining the entire sample
and to use statistical procedures to account for differences
in transcript length. Furthermore, using the entire sample for
linguistic analyses, including NDW, is common practice in
contemporary studies of children’s oral narratives (Gazella &
Stockman, 2003; Humphries et al., 2004; Swanson et al.,
2005; Uccelli & Páez, 2007).
Results
The NSS scores were first reviewed to identify how many
child and examiner errors precluded scores to be adminis-
tered. In the present study, a total of 903 scores were completed
using the children’s narrative transcripts (seven categories were
scored across 129 transcripts). Across the 903 sections, only
six sections received scores of zero (four stories lacked an in-
troduction, and two children omitted a conclusion), and only
one of the 903 sections received a score of NA; 99% of the
NSS sections were able to be scored correctly, confirming that
the training and elicitation procedures facilitated a high level
of child compliance and examiner fidelity during the elici-
tation process.
The skewness statistic was calculated for the NSS to de-
termine if there was an unequal distribution of NSS scores
across the sample (see Coolican, 2004, for a review). Skew-
ness measures of zero indicate a perfectly normal distribution,
while skewness values below –0.8 or above 0.8 have been
described as “noticeably skewed” (Bourque & Clark, 1992,
p. 69). The skewness statistic for the NSS was –0.5, indicat-
ing that scores were more concentrated toward the ceiling
but were not noticeably skewed according to the criteria
established by Bourque and Clark.
Descriptive statistics for age, the NSS, and each of the
microstructural measures are presented in Table 2. Table 2
also summarizes the bivariate correlations between the NSS,
children’s age, and each of the microstructures. All correla-
tions were significant and were moderate in strength (Cohen,
1988).
To further explore the covariance structure between the
variables and to identify unique relationships between the
microstructures and the NSS, two separate hierarchical re-
gression equations were completed. Hierarchical regressions
allow examination of variance that is uniquely explained by a
given variable. The first hierarchical regression equation is
summarized in Table 3. Sample length was first controlled by
entering NTW into Model 1. Length was controlled because
measures of lexical diversity are inevitably influenced by the
NTW in the sample (Malvern & Richards, 2002). That is, the
more total words that a child produces, the more opportunity
he or she has to produce different words.
Model 2 identified the unique relationship between vocab-
ulary and NSS after controlling for length. Taken together,
NTWand NDW were significantly correlated with NSS scores
(r = .58). Adding NDW in Model 2 increased the explained
variance from 24% to 33%, a net increase of 9%. A one-way
analysis of variance was completed to determine whether the
increase in explained variance was significant, and an f 2 sta-
tistic was calculated to estimate its effect size. According to
Cohen (1988), effect sizes of 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35 are con-
sidered small, medium, and large, respectively. The 9% increase
in explained variance was significant, F(1, 124) = 16.5,
p < .001, f 2 = 0.12, demonstrating that NDW was uniquely
TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics for language sample measures
and correlations with the narrative scoring scheme (NSS).
Measure M SD Range
Correlation
with NSS
Age 6.0 0.7 5.0–7.0 .30*
Narrative macrostructure
(NSS)
19.0 3.0 11.0–26.0 —
Productivity (NTW) 264.1 77.2 133.0–608.0 .50**
Vocabulary (NDW) 92.5 19.0 47.0–150.0 .58**
Grammar (MLCU) 7.0 0.9 4.8–9.6 .44**
Grammar (SI) 1.1 0.1 1.0–1.3 .35**
Note. NTW = number of total words; NDW = number of
different
words; MLCU = mean length of C-unit; SI = subordination
index.
*p = .001. **p < .001.
TABLE 3. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis with
grammatical measures uniquely predicting NSS scores in
Model 3.
Model Predictors r Adjusted r2 r2 change
1 Productivity (NTW) .50 .24*
2 Productivity (NTW) .58 .33* .09*
Vocabulary (NDW)
3 Productivity (NTW) .61 .35* .02
Vocabulary (NDW)
Grammar (MLCU, SI)
*Significant at p < .01.
Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 159
related to children’s NSS scores above and beyond the vari-
ability explained by NTW.
Model 3 was completed to identify if there was a unique
relationship between the two grammatical measures (MLCU
and SI) and NSS scores, after controlling for NTW and NDW.
The measures in this final model were significantly correlated
with the NSS (r = .61). Adding MLCU and SI to the third
regression equation added an additional 2% prediction to NSS
scores, which was not significant, F(2, 122) = 2.9, p = .060,
f 2 = 0.03. In this first hierarchical regression analysis, NDW
uniquely predicted NSS scores after controlling for length
using NTW. The two grammatical measures, however, did
not add unique prediction of children’s NSS scores. To con-
firm that NDW was the major unique predictor of NSS scores, a
second hierarchical regression equation was completed and is
summarized in Table 4. Again, sample length was controlled
by entering NTW in the first model. The two grammatical
measures were next entered in the second equation, which
resulted in a combined correlation of r = .57. Adding SI and
MLCU to NTW increased the explained variance from .24
to .30, documenting that the grammatical measures explain
6% of the variance in NSS after controlling for sample length.
This increase was significant, F(2, 123) = 6.0, p = .002,
f 2 = 0.08. NDW was added to the third model to test the
unique prediction of vocabulary on NSS scores. Adding NDW
in the third model resulted in a 5% increase in explained
variance between the microstructures and NSS, which was
significant, F(1, 122) = 8.7, p = .003, f 2 = 0.06.
In sum, this series of hierarchical regression equations
documented that children’s use of vocabulary is the major
significant and unique microstructural variable in predicting
their story organization skills as measured by the NSS. Chil-
dren’s productive grammar, while significantly correlated
with NSS scores, did not provide unique prediction of the
children’s narrative macrostructure ability.
Discussion
Upon establishing a set of reference databases for chil-
dren’s narrative retells, the Language Analysis Lab at the
University of Wisconsin—Madison set out to identify a clini-
cally useful measure of children’s narrative organization skills.
The NSS was created to bring together the benefits of con-
crete scoring criteria combined with judgment of text-level
constructs. The NSS also incorporated higher level narrative
components, including cohesive markers and measures of
literate language, to measure a wider range of skills than tradi-
tional story grammar analyses.
To determine whether the NSS was developmentally ap-
propriate for the children in this study, the skewness statistic
was calculated. This analysis revealed that the data were not
noticeably skewed according to Borque and Clark’s (1992)
criteria and that the NSS appeared to be a sensitive measure for
school-age children who produced an oral retell. (We are cur-
rently completing a more thorough investigation of the de-
velopmental sensitivity of the NSS as compared to alternative
methods of documenting narrative macrostructure skills.)
To document the relationship between the microstructural
features of language samples and children’s performance on
the NSS, a series of correlation and hierarchical regression
analyses were completed. These analyses confirmed that a close
relationship existed between children’s productivity, vocab-
ulary, grammar, and narrative macrostructure skills. The
correlation analyses documented that age and each of the
microstructural measures (NTW, NDW, MLCU, and SI)
were significantly correlated with children’s narrative organi-
zation skills. It is noteworthy that the correlation between age
and NSS was the weakest observed correlation. The socio-
cultural theory of narrative macrostructure development pro-
poses that children who have more experience with stories
will have greater narrative competence (e.g., Eaton et al., 1999;
Stein & Glenn, 1979). The children in the present sample
spanned 2 years in age. The older children in this sample likely
had more experience listening to and telling stories. How-
ever, it was the children’s vocabulary and grammar skills that
were most strongly related to their narrative macrostructure
scores. While this study did not directly control for the amount
of experience the children had with narratives, the data pro-
vide some additional evidence for the importance of chil-
dren’s linguistic proficiency in predicting narrative organization
skills (Berman & Slobin, 1994; Bishop & Donlan, 2005).
Two separate hierarchical regression analyses were com-
pleted to identify the unique relationships between each of
the microstructures and NSS scores. After controlling for
length, the unique relationship between the NSS and mea-
sures of vocabulary and grammar was calculated. The anal-
yses showed that children’s productive vocabulary skills were
the only unique predictor of narrative organization skills.
Grammatical measures, on the other hand, provided no unique
prediction of NSS scores. The unique importance of vocab-
ulary in predicting narrative organization skills was a novel
finding. Bishop and Donlan (2005) documented that children’s
use of complex syntax and expression of causal concepts
uniquely predicted children’s ability to organize their oral
narratives. Bishop and Donlan examined children between
7 and 9 years of age, while the present study investigated
children age 5–7 years. The children in the present study were
using minimal complex syntax. As observed in Table 2, SI
values averaged 1.1, showing that children produce approx-
imately one subordinate clause every 10 utterances. The
children’s use of subordination may have been influenced by
the story used in this study; the children’s SI values, on aver-
age, were just slightly lower than the SI value from the story
script (SI = 1.15). These low levels of subordination could
explain, in part, the modest correlations and lack of a unique
relationship between the grammatical measures and the NSS.
TABLE 4. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis with
vocabulary measures uniquely predicting NSS scores in
Model 3.
Model Predictors r Adjusted r2 r2 change
1 Productivity (NTW) .50 .24*
2 Productivity (NTW) .57 .30* .06*
Grammar (MLCU, SI)
3 Productivity (NTW) .61 .35* .05*
Grammar (MLCU, SI)
Vocabulary (NDW)
*Significant at p < .01.
160 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19
• 154–166 • May 2010
Relationship Between Vocabulary and the NSS
The present study revealed that there is a special and im-
portant relationship between narrative organization and vo-
cabulary skills that emerges prior to children becoming fully
literate. This study demonstrated that the development of
story schema and vocabulary acquisition is developing along
a similar path. The importance of vocabulary in narrative
organization skills is not surprising given the literature de-
scribing the development of narrative form. Preschool and
young school-age children typically produce narratives that
simply chain sequences of events in temporal order (Berman,
1988) and provide simple descriptive sequences (Stein &
Glenn, 1979). It is not until the later school-age years that
children hierarchically organize the events in their narrative
productions (Berman, 1988) and take multiple perspectives to
relate the events (Stein & Glenn, 1979). To produce these
more advanced narratives, children must use complex syntax
(Bamberg & Damrad-Frye, 1991; Bishop & Donlan, 2005).
Before children are proficient in using complex syntax, they
likely have to rely on their vocabulary skills to organize their
narrative productions. Furthermore, the emerging literacy
literature has documented the importance of vocabulary in the
development of children’s narrative and comprehension skills.
There is a well-documented relationship between children’s
vocabulary skills and reading comprehension (see Scarborough,
2001). Furthermore, preschool and young school-age chil-
dren acquire new vocabulary through repeated exposures to
narrative form (Robbins & Ehri, 1994; Senechal & Cornell,
1993) and by receiving adult scaffolding that highlights
the story’s structure (Hargrave & Senechal, 2000; Penno,
Wilkinson, & Moore, 2002).
Limitations
The results from the hierarchical regression equations
could have been affected by the sampling context. Because all
of the measures were collected from a single language sam-
ple, high internal validity was achieved. While each of the
language sample measures theoretically and empirically re-
flects its respective linguistic domain, strong intercorrelations
between language sample measures have been observed (Miller
& Klee, 1995). The hierarchical regression equations did af-
ford better understanding of the covariance structure. How-
ever, vocabulary and grammatical measures acquired from
alternate tasks may provide a more informative test of the
relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and narrative
macrostructure.
Clinical Implications
The NSS was created as a clinically useful comprehensive
narrative macrostructure measure. To be clinically feasible,
an assessment tool must be able to be completed in a short
amount of time to accommodate the busy schedule of the SLP.
In our lab, trained transcribers could complete the NSS using
a narrative transcript in approximately 3 min. In addition to
efficient scoring, the NSS was developed to facilitate accurate
scoring both within and between examiners. The Krippen-
dorff alpha analyses revealed that the NSS had lower inter-
rater agreement than we would ideally observe and that the
NSS alpha was not as high as the alpha for the SI. The dif-
ference in alphas between the NSS and SI was not surprising,
however, as the NSS requires much greater individual judg-
ment when compared to the relatively straightforward scoring
rules for the SI. However, recall that these subjective ratings
are often the most sensitive when identifying children with
language impairment (e.g., McFadden & Gillam, 1996). Our
goal is to continue developing new training methods and
complete additional research identifying ways to increase
coding accuracy for the NSS and other macrostructural anal-
yses of children’s natural language use.
Upon scoring the NSS, clinicians are provided with a clini-
cally useful benchmark for children’s overall narrative profi-
ciency. The data described in this study used the composite
NSS score, which was the summed score for all seven sec-
tions of the NSS. We proposed that the NSS composite score
provided an index of children’s overall narrative organization
skills. The NSS data described in this study are available as
part of the SALT Narrative Story Retell database and can be
downloaded free of charge from the SALT Web site. SALT
software, used to access the data, can compare a target child’s
NSS scores with those of age-matched peers. In addition to
comparing NSS scores to the SALT database, composite NSS
scores can be useful for monitoring progress and documenting
treatment outcomes by collecting multiple narrative samples
from a child and documenting changes in NSS scores over
time.
The NSS also provides examiners with the opportunity to
identify specific aspects of the narrative process that are dif-
ficult for a child. Because the NSS separately judges seven
aspects of the narrative process, examiners can evaluate per-
formance on each section of the NSS to identify areas of
strength and areas that require intervention. Compared with
narrative macrostructure measures that make holistic text-
level judgments of narrative proficiency, there is greater spe-
cificity in the NSS scoring procedure. Having a detailed
narrative performance profile facilitates a more accurate de-
scription of the child’s performance and can assist in the
development of treatment goals. For example, a child who
performed poorly on the referencing and cohesion sections of
the NSS but did well on the other sections likely has difficulty
using cohesive devices. Treatment goals could include im-
proving the child’s use of referential, lexical, and conjunctive
cohesion.
Increasing our understanding of the relationships between
vocabulary, grammar, and narrative macrostructure has im-
portant clinical implications for documenting functional out-
comes and identifying treatment goals. One goal of language
intervention programs is for the therapy tasks to generalize
to functional tasks. Telling a well-formed narrative is a func-
tional task that is important to children’s academic success
(Bishop & Edmundson, 1987; Griffin, Hemphill, Camp, &
Wolf, 2004; O’Neill, Pearce, & Pick, 2004). The data from the
present study demonstrated that vocabulary skills were uniquely
related to children’s story organization skills. Therefore, a
treatment procedure that increases a child’s vocabulary skills
would have broader, more functional outcomes if concurrent
increases in his or her narrative macrostructure skills were
documented. Similarly, we may expect that treatments ad-
dressing narrative macrostructure skills could also result in
Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 161
concurrent increases in vocabulary skills. Clearer understand-
ing of the relationship between microstructures and macro-
structures will facilitate selection of appropriate treatment
goals. Understanding these relationships can aid in identifying
the appropriate microstructures to address when implement-
ing interventions that focus on linguistic macrostructures, such
as narrative organization. It is important to note that the data
in this study are purely correlational and that further research
is needed before causal relationships between vocabulary,
grammar, and narrative macrostructure can be identified.
In sum, narrative language assessment is an effective
method for documenting children’s language skills. The NSS
was developed by the Language Analysis Lab as a clinically
useful index of children’s narrative organization skills. Given
its clinical feasibility and its robust relationship with other
linguistic measures, the NSS provides clinicians and re-
searchers with an additional tool to document children’s global
language skills using a functional and curriculum-oriented
task.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Institutes of Health
Grant 5 T32 DC005459 (“Interdisciplinary Research Training in
Speech-Language Disorders”) and by the Language Analysis
Lab at
the University of Wisconsin—Madison. This work was also
made
possible through close collaboration with two public school
districts
in San Diego County: San Diego City Schools and Cajon Valley
Union Schools. The authors express appreciation to Karen
Andriacchi
and the research assistants from the Language Analysis Lab for
their
assistance with transcription and organization of the data.
Portions
of this article were presented at the 2006 American Speech-
Language-
Hearing Association Annual Convention in Miami, FL.
References
Allen, M., Kertoy, M., Sherblom, J., & Pettit, J. (1994). Chil-
dren’s narrative productions: A comparison of personal event
and fictional stories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 149–176.
Applebee, A. (1978). The child’s concept of story: Ages two to
seven. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to pro-
vide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s
narrative competencies. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689–
710.
Berman, R. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative.
Discourse Processes, 11, 469–497.
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (Eds.). (1994). Relating events in nar-
rative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Bishop, D., & Donlan, C. (2005). The role of syntax in encoding
and recall of pictorial narratives: Evidence from specific lan-
guage impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 23, 25–46.
Bishop, D., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4-
year-
olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment.
Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 52, 156–173.
Boudreau, D. (2007). Narrative abilities in children with
language
impairment. In R. Paul (Ed.), Language disorders from a devel-
opmental perspective: Essays in honor of Robin S. Chapman
(pp. 331–356). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Boudreau, D., & Hedberg, N. (1999). A comparison of early
literacy skills in children with specific language impairment and
their typically developing peers. American Journal of Speech-
Language Pathology, 8, 249–260.
Bourque, L., & Clark, V. (1992). Processing data: The survey
example. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages.
Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Clahsen, H. (1989). The grammatical characterization of
develop-
mental dysphasia. Linguistics, 27, 897–920.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Colozzo, P., Garcia, R., Megan, C., Gillam, R., & Johnston, J.
(2006, June). Narrative assessment in SLI: Exploring interac-
tions between content and form. Poster session presented at
the annual meeting of the Symposium on Research in Child
Language Disorders, Madison, WI.
Coolican, H. (2004). Research methods and statistics in
psychol-
ogy. London, England: Hodder & Stoughton.
Curenton, S., & Justice, L. (2004). African American and
Cauca-
sian preschoolers’ use of decontextualized language: Literate
language features in oral narratives. Language, Speech, and
Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 240–253.
Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of
relative
clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics, 11,
131–151.
Eaton, J., Collis, G., & Lewis, V. (1999). Evaluative
explanations
in children’s narratives of a video sequence without dialogue.
Journal of Child Language, 26, 699–720.
Gazella, J., & Stockman, I. (2003). Children’s story retelling
under different modality and task conditions: Implications for
standardizing language sampling procedures. American Journal
of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 61–72.
Gillam, R., & Johnston, J. (1992). Spoken and written language
relationships in language/learning-impaired and normally
achiev-
ing school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Research,
35, 1303–1315.
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. (1991). Familial aggregation of a
devel-
opmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50.
Greenhalgh, K., & Strong, C. (2001). Literate language features
in
spoken narratives of children with typical language and children
with language impairments. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services in Schools, 32, 114–125.
Griffin, T., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. (2004). Oral
discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First
Language, 24, 123–147.
Halliday, M., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London,
England: Longman.
Hargrave, A., & Senechal, M. (2000). A book reading interven-
tion with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: The
benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early Child-
hood Research Quarterly, 15, 75–90.
Hedberg, N., & Westby, C. (1993). Analyzing storytelling
skills:
Theory to practice. Tucson, AZ: Communication Skill Builders.
Heilmann, J., Miller, J., Iglesias, A., Fabiano-Smith, L.,
Nockerts, A., & Andriacchi, K. (2008). Narrative transcription
accuracy and reliability in two languages. Topics in Language
Disorders, 28, 178–188.
Humphries, T., Cardy, J., Worling, D., & Peets, K. (2004).
Narrative comprehension and retelling abilities of children
with nonverbal learning disabilities. Brain and Cognition, 56,
77–88.
Justice, L., Bowles, R., Kaderavek, J., Ukrainetz, T., Eisenberg,
S., & Gillam, R. (2006). The Index of Narrative Microstructure:
A clinical tool for analyzing school-age children’s narrative
performances. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathol-
ogy, 15, 177–191.
162 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19
• 154–166 • May 2010
Klee, T. (1992). Developmental and diagnostic characteristics
of
quantitative measures of children’s language production. Topics
in Language Disorders, 12, 28–41.
Krippendorff, K. (1980). Content analysis: An introduction to
its
methodology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral ver-
sions of personal experience. In J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the
verbal and visual arts (pp. 12–44). Seattle: University of
Washington Press.
Leadholm, B., & Miller, J. F. (1992). Language sample analysis:
The Wisconsin guide. Madison, WI: Department of Public
Instruction.
Liles, B. (1985). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and
language-
disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,
28, 123–133.
Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2002). Investigating accommoda-
tion in language proficiency interviews using a new measure of
lexical diversity. Language Testing, 19, 85–104.
Mandler, J., & Johnson, N. (1977). Remembrance of things
parsed:
Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111–151.
Manhardt, J., & Rescorla, L. (2002). Oral narrative skills of late
talkers at ages 8 and 9. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 1–21.
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York, NY: Dial
Press.
McCabe, A. (1997). Developmental and cross-cultural aspects
of
children’s narration. In G. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative develop-
ment: Six approaches (pp. 137–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McFadden, T., & Gillam, R. (1996). An examination of the
quality
of narratives produced by children with language disorders.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 48–56.
Merritt, D., & Liles, B. (1987). Story grammar ability in
children
with and without language disorder: Story generation, story
retelling, and story comprehension. Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research, 30, 539–552.
Miles,S., & Chapman,R. S.(2002). Narrative content as
described
by individuals with Down syndrome and typically developing
children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
45, 175–189.
Miller, J. F. (1981). Assessing language production in children.
Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
Miller, J. F. (1987). A grammatical characterization of language
disorder. In J. Martin, P. Fletcher, R. Grunwell, & D. Hall
(Eds.),
Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Specific
Speech and Language Disorders in Children (pp. 100–113).
London, England: Association for All Speech Impaired
Children.
Miller, J., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L.,
& Francis, D. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual
children. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21,
30–43.
Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2008). Systematic Analysis of Lan-
guage Transcripts (Research Version 9.1) [Computer software].
Middleton, WI: SALT Software.
Miller, J., & Klee, T. (1995). Computational approaches to the
anal-
ysis of language impairment. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney
(Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 545–572). Oxford,
England: Blackwell.
Nippold, M. (2007). Later language development: School-age
children, adolescents, and young adults (3rd ed.). Austin, TX:
Pro-Ed.
Nippold, M., Hesketh, L., Duthie, J., & Mansfield, T. (2005).
Conversational versus expository discourse: A study of
syntactic
development in children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1048–1064.
O’Neill, D., Pearce, M., & Pick, J. (2004). Preschool children’s
narratives and performance on the Peabody Individualized
Achievement Test—Revised: Evidence of a relation between
early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language,
24, 149–183.
Paul, R., & Smith, R. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds
with
normal, impaired, and late-developing language. Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 592–598.
Pearce, W., McCormack, P., & James, D. (2003). Exploring
the boundaries of SLI: Findings from morphosyntactic and
story grammar analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17,
325–334.
Pelligrini, A., Galda, L., Bartini, M., & Charak, D. (1998). Oral
language and literacy learning in context: The role of social
relationships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 38–54.
Penno, J., Wilkinson, I., & Moore, D. (2002). Vocabulary
acquisi-
tion from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories:
Do they overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educational
Psychology, 94, 23–33.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholin-
guistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New
York,
NY: Plenum Press.
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (2004). Echoing our parents:
Parental
influences on children’s narration. In M. Pratt & B. Fiese
(Eds.),
Family stories and the life course: Across time and generations
(pp. 27–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Quasthoff, U. (1997a). An interactive approach to narrative
devel-
opment. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative development: Six
approaches (pp. 51–83). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Quasthoff, U. (1997b). Were you ever in a situation where you
were in serious danger of being killed? Narrator-listener inter-
action in Labov and Waletzky’s narratives. Journal of Narrative
& Life History, 7, 121–128.
Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). Frog,
Where are you? Narratives in children with specific language
impairment, early focal brain injury and Williams syndrome.
Brain & Language, 88, 229–247.
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific
language
impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal
of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863.
Robbins, C., & Ehri, L. (1994). Reading storybooks to kinder-
garteners helps them learn new vocabulary words. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 86, 56–64.
Rumelhart, D. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. G.
Bobrow & A. M. Collins (Eds.), Representation and under-
standing: Studies in cognitive science (pp. 211–236). New
York, NY: Academic Press.
Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy
to
later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In
S. Neuman & D. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook for research in
early literacy (pp. 97–110). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Schuele, C., & Tolbert, L. (2001). Omissions of obligatory
relative
markers in children with specific language impairment. Clinical
Linguistics & Phonetics, 15, 257–274.
Scott, C., & Stokes, S. (1995). Measures of syntax in school-age
children and adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing
Services
in the Schools, 26, 309–319.
Senechal,M.,&Cornell,E.(1993). Vocabulary acquisition through
shared reading experiences. Reading Research Quarterly, 28,
360–374.
Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development
of
grammar. In C. Ferguson & D. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child
language development (pp. 175–208). New York, NY: Aca-
demic Press.
Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of storytelling skill. In
M. Franklin & S. Barten (Eds.), Child language: A reader
(pp. 282–297). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis for story comprehen-
sion in elementary school. In R. Freedle (Ed.), New directions
Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 163
in discourse processing (Vol. 2, pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ:
Ablex.
Strong, C. (1998). The Strong Narrative Assessment Procedure.
Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.
Strong, C., & Shaver, J. (1991). Stability of cohesion in the
spoken
narratives of language-impaired and normally developing
school-
aged children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34,
95–111.
Swanson, L., Fey, M., Mills, C., & Hood, L. (2005). Use of
narrative-based language intervention with children who have
specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-
Language Pathology, 14, 131–143.
Templin, M. (1957). Certain language skills in children: Their
development and interrelationships. Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota Press.
Uccelli, P., & Páez, M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary devel-
opment of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade:
Developmental changes and associations among English and
Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
Schools, 38, 225–236.
van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cement of the
universe:
The development of event comprehension from childhood to
adulthood. In P. van den Broek, P. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.),
Developmental spans in event comprehension: Bridging fic-
tional and actual events (pp. 321–342). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Westby, C. (2005). Assessing and facilitating text
comprehension
problems. In H. Catts & A. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading
disabilities (pp. 157–232). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Wigglesworth, G. (1997). Children’s individual approaches to
the organization of narrative. Journal of Child Language, 24,
279–309.
Received March 24, 2008
Revision received December 1, 2008
Accepted November 30, 2009
DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0024)
Contact author: John Heilmann, East Carolina University,
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Health
Sciences Building, Room 3310T, Greenville, NC 27858-4353.
E-mail: [email protected]
164 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19
• 154–166 • May 2010
Appendix (p. 1 of 2)
The Narrative Scoring Scheme
Characteristic Proficient Emerging Minimal/immature
Introduction Setting Setting -Child launches into story
with no attempt to provide
the setting.
-Child states general place and provides
some detail about the setting (e.g.,
reference to the time of the setting—
daytime, bedtime, or season).
-Child states general setting but
provides no detail.
-Setting elements are stated at
appropriate place in story.
-Description or elements of story
are given intermittently
through story.
Characters
-Child may provide description
of specific element of setting
(e.g., the frog is in the jar).
-Main characters are introduced
with some description or
detail provided.
OR
Characters
-Characters of story are mentioned
with no detail or description.
Character
development
-Main character(s) and all supporting
character(s) are mentioned.
-Both main and active supporting
characters are mentioned.
-Inconsistent mention is made of
involved or active characters.
-Throughout story it is clear that child
can discriminate between main and
supporting characters (e.g., more
description of and emphasis on
main character[s]).
-Main characters are not clearly
distinguished from supporting
characters.
-Characters necessary for
advancing the plot are
not present.
-Child narrates in first person using
character voice (e.g., “You get out
of my tree,” said the owl).
Mental states -Mental states of main and supporting
characters are expressed when
necessary for plot development
and advancement.
-Some mental state words are
used to develop character(s).
No use is made of mental state
words to develop characters.
-A variety of mental state words are used.
-A limited number of mental state
words are used inconsistently
throughout the story.
Referencing -Child provides necessary antecedents
to pronouns.
-Referents/antecedents are used
inconsistently.
-Pronouns are used excessively.
-References are clear throughout story.
-No verbal clarifiers are used.
-Child is unaware listener is confused.
Conflict
resolution
-Child clearly states all conflicts and
resolutions critical to advancing
the plot of the story.
-Description of conflicts and
resolutions critical to
advancing the plot of the
story is underdeveloped.
-Random resolution is stated with
no mention of cause or conflict.
OR
OR
-Not all conflicts and resolutions
critical to advancing the plot
are present.
-Conflict is mentioned without
resolution.
OR
-Many conflicts and resolutions
critical to advancing the
plot are not present.
Cohesion -Events follow a logical order. -Events follow a
logical order. -No use is made of smooth
transitions.-Critical events are included, while less
emphasis is placed on minor events.
-Excessive detail or emphasis
provided on minor events
leads the listener astray.-Smooth transitions are provided
between events. OR
-Transitions to next event are unclear.
OR
-Minimal detail is given for critical events.
OR
-Equal emphasis is placed on all events.
Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 165
Appendix (p. 1 of 2)? (p. NaN )
The Narrative Scoring Scheme
Characteristic Proficient Emerging Minimal/immature
Conclusion -Story is clearly wrapped up using
general concluding statements
such as “and they were together
again happy as could be.”
-Specific event is concluded, but no
general statement is made as
to the conclusion of the whole story.
-Child stops narrating, and
listener may need to ask if that
is the end.
Scoring: Each characteristic receives a scaled score of 0–5.
Proficient characteristics = 5; Emerging = 3; Minimal/immature
= 1. Scores between
(i.e., 2 and 4) are undefined; use judgment. Scores of zero and
NA are defined below. A composite is scored by adding the
total of the characteristic
scores. Highest score = 35.
A score of zero is given for child errors (such as telling the
wrong story, conversing with examiner, not completing/refusing
task, using wrong
language and creating inability of scorer to comprehend story in
target language, abandoned utterances, unintelligibility, poor
performance, or
components of rubric are in imitation-only).
A score of NA (nonapplicable) is given for
mechanical/examiner/operator errors (such as interference from
background noise, issues with recording
such as cut-offs or interruptions, examiner quitting before child
does, examiner not following protocol, or examiner asking
overly specific or leading
questions rather than open-ended questions or prompts).
Appendix (p. 2 of 2)
The Narrative Scoring Scheme
166 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19
• 154–166 • May 2010
Copyright of American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology
is the property of American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

More Related Content

PDF
Why use SSP and not 'Whole Word' Approach with II Students
PPTX
SCD slide share
PDF
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
PDF
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
PPT
Strickland Ncfl Conference
PPT
Strickland Ncfl Conference
PPT
Family Literacy & The Achievement Gap
DOCX
Apa citation
Why use SSP and not 'Whole Word' Approach with II Students
SCD slide share
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
ACADEMIC-PERFORMANCE-TOWARDS-SPEAKING-ENGLISH.pdf
Strickland Ncfl Conference
Strickland Ncfl Conference
Family Literacy & The Achievement Gap
Apa citation

Similar to Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docx (20)

DOCX
Apa citation
PDF
Self- Efficacy, Word Reading, and Vocabulary Knowledge in English Language Le...
PDF
Using visual phonics as a strategic intervention to increase literacy
PDF
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
PDF
TENSE MARKERS HINDI
DOC
proununciation
PPT
Academic Vocabulary and Reading Online for ELLs
DOCX
JSLHRArticleEffects of Sampling Context on Spontaneous.docx
DOCX
refrences13350209.pdfOral language development and access.docx
PDF
My New Article- Kamalata Lukama
PPTX
Speech and Language Presentation (1).pptx
PDF
Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
PDF
Wong esl teacher candidates’ perceptions of readiness
PPTX
ELT 205 RESEARCH ANALYSIS_GREGORIO, KRISTAL JANE F..pptx
PPT
Oral Language and Literacy Powerpoint
DOCX
Issue Brief
DOCX
ACTION-RESEARCH final for education .docx
PPT
Apptitude As An Individual Difference In Sla
PPT
Apptitude As An Individual Difference In Sla
Apa citation
Self- Efficacy, Word Reading, and Vocabulary Knowledge in English Language Le...
Using visual phonics as a strategic intervention to increase literacy
Remedial Instruction in Language Disfluencies in the Non-Psycho-Expert Lens
TENSE MARKERS HINDI
proununciation
Academic Vocabulary and Reading Online for ELLs
JSLHRArticleEffects of Sampling Context on Spontaneous.docx
refrences13350209.pdfOral language development and access.docx
My New Article- Kamalata Lukama
Speech and Language Presentation (1).pptx
Language-impaired preschoolers: A follow-up into adolescence.
Wong esl teacher candidates’ perceptions of readiness
ELT 205 RESEARCH ANALYSIS_GREGORIO, KRISTAL JANE F..pptx
Oral Language and Literacy Powerpoint
Issue Brief
ACTION-RESEARCH final for education .docx
Apptitude As An Individual Difference In Sla
Apptitude As An Individual Difference In Sla
Ad

More from bagotjesusa (20)

DOCX
Issues Identify at least seven issues you see in the case1..docx
DOCX
Issues and disagreements between management and employees lead.docx
DOCX
ISSN1369 7021 © Elsevier Ltd 2010DECEMBER 2010 VOLUME 13 .docx
DOCX
ISSA Journal September 2008Article Title Article Author.docx
DOCX
ISOL 536Security Architecture and DesignThreat Modeling.docx
DOCX
ISOL 533 Project Part 1OverviewWrite paper in sections.docx
DOCX
Is the United States of America a democracyDetailed Outline.docx
DOCX
Islamic Profession of Faith (There is no God but God and Muhammad is.docx
DOCX
IS-365 Writing Rubric Last updated January 15, 2018 .docx
DOCX
ISAS 600 – Database Project Phase III RubricAs the final ste.docx
DOCX
Is teenage pregnancy a social problem How does this topic reflect.docx
DOCX
Is Texas so conservative- (at least for the time being)- as many pun.docx
DOCX
Irreplaceable Personal Objects and Cultural IdentityThink of .docx
DOCX
IRB is an important step in research. State the required components .docx
DOCX
irem.orgjpm jpm® 47AND REWARDRISK .docx
DOCX
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docx
DOCX
In two paragraphs, respond to the prompt below. Journal entries .docx
DOCX
Investigative Statement AnalysisInitial statement given by Ted K.docx
DOCX
Investigating Happiness at College SNAPSHOT T.docx
DOCX
Investigate Development Case Death with Dignity Physician-Assiste.docx
Issues Identify at least seven issues you see in the case1..docx
Issues and disagreements between management and employees lead.docx
ISSN1369 7021 © Elsevier Ltd 2010DECEMBER 2010 VOLUME 13 .docx
ISSA Journal September 2008Article Title Article Author.docx
ISOL 536Security Architecture and DesignThreat Modeling.docx
ISOL 533 Project Part 1OverviewWrite paper in sections.docx
Is the United States of America a democracyDetailed Outline.docx
Islamic Profession of Faith (There is no God but God and Muhammad is.docx
IS-365 Writing Rubric Last updated January 15, 2018 .docx
ISAS 600 – Database Project Phase III RubricAs the final ste.docx
Is teenage pregnancy a social problem How does this topic reflect.docx
Is Texas so conservative- (at least for the time being)- as many pun.docx
Irreplaceable Personal Objects and Cultural IdentityThink of .docx
IRB is an important step in research. State the required components .docx
irem.orgjpm jpm® 47AND REWARDRISK .docx
IoT Referenceshttpswww.techrepublic.comarticlehow-to-secur.docx
In two paragraphs, respond to the prompt below. Journal entries .docx
Investigative Statement AnalysisInitial statement given by Ted K.docx
Investigating Happiness at College SNAPSHOT T.docx
Investigate Development Case Death with Dignity Physician-Assiste.docx
Ad

Recently uploaded (20)

PDF
My India Quiz Book_20210205121199924.pdf
PDF
احياء السادس العلمي - الفصل الثالث (التكاثر) منهج متميزين/كلية بغداد/موهوبين
PPTX
ELIAS-SEZIURE AND EPilepsy semmioan session.pptx
PDF
Complications of Minimal Access-Surgery.pdf
PPTX
TNA_Presentation-1-Final(SAVE)) (1).pptx
PPTX
Virtual and Augmented Reality in Current Scenario
PDF
FORM 1 BIOLOGY MIND MAPS and their schemes
PDF
BP 704 T. NOVEL DRUG DELIVERY SYSTEMS (UNIT 1)
PPTX
Chinmaya Tiranga Azadi Quiz (Class 7-8 )
PPTX
History, Philosophy and sociology of education (1).pptx
PDF
IGGE1 Understanding the Self1234567891011
PDF
What if we spent less time fighting change, and more time building what’s rig...
PPTX
B.Sc. DS Unit 2 Software Engineering.pptx
PDF
ChatGPT for Dummies - Pam Baker Ccesa007.pdf
PDF
medical_surgical_nursing_10th_edition_ignatavicius_TEST_BANK_pdf.pdf
PDF
Τίμαιος είναι φιλοσοφικός διάλογος του Πλάτωνα
PDF
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
DOCX
Cambridge-Practice-Tests-for-IELTS-12.docx
PDF
International_Financial_Reporting_Standa.pdf
PDF
Vision Prelims GS PYQ Analysis 2011-2022 www.upscpdf.com.pdf
My India Quiz Book_20210205121199924.pdf
احياء السادس العلمي - الفصل الثالث (التكاثر) منهج متميزين/كلية بغداد/موهوبين
ELIAS-SEZIURE AND EPilepsy semmioan session.pptx
Complications of Minimal Access-Surgery.pdf
TNA_Presentation-1-Final(SAVE)) (1).pptx
Virtual and Augmented Reality in Current Scenario
FORM 1 BIOLOGY MIND MAPS and their schemes
BP 704 T. NOVEL DRUG DELIVERY SYSTEMS (UNIT 1)
Chinmaya Tiranga Azadi Quiz (Class 7-8 )
History, Philosophy and sociology of education (1).pptx
IGGE1 Understanding the Self1234567891011
What if we spent less time fighting change, and more time building what’s rig...
B.Sc. DS Unit 2 Software Engineering.pptx
ChatGPT for Dummies - Pam Baker Ccesa007.pdf
medical_surgical_nursing_10th_edition_ignatavicius_TEST_BANK_pdf.pdf
Τίμαιος είναι φιλοσοφικός διάλογος του Πλάτωνα
Weekly quiz Compilation Jan -July 25.pdf
Cambridge-Practice-Tests-for-IELTS-12.docx
International_Financial_Reporting_Standa.pdf
Vision Prelims GS PYQ Analysis 2011-2022 www.upscpdf.com.pdf

Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docx

  • 1. Selecting A topic/Job Position For the first part of the project, you will select a topic position for either a Release of Information Associate For the position you have selected, address the following in 2 paragraphs · Brief description of the position (2 -3 sentences) · Why did you select the position? · What are the 3 major duties of the position that are most essential to the role? Article Corresponding author: John Heilmann, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Mail Stop #668, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC 27858, USA E-mail: [email protected] Language Testing 27(4) 603–626 © The Author(s) 2010 Reprints and permission: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermission.nav DOI: 10.1177/0265532209355669 http://ltj.sagepub.com Sensitivity of narrative organization measures using narrative retells produced by
  • 2. young school-age children John Heilmann East Carolina University, USA Jon F. Miller University of Wisconsin – Madison, USA Ann Nockerts University of Wisconsin – Madison, USA Abstract Analysis of children’s productions of oral narratives provides a rich description of children’s oral language skills. However, measures of narrative organization can be directly affected by both developmental and task-based performance constraints which can make a measure insensitive and inappropriate for a particular population and/or sampling method. This study critically reviewed four methods of evaluating children’s narrative organization skills and revealed that the Narrative Scoring Scheme (NSS) was the most developmentally sensitive measure for a group of 129 5–7-year-old children who completed a narrative retell. Upon comparing the methods of assessing narrative organization skills, the NSS was unique in its incorporation of higher-level narrative features and its scoring rules, which required examiners to make subjective judgments across seven aspects of the narrative process. The discussion surrounded issues of measuring children’s narrative organization skills and, more broadly, issues surrounding sensitivity of criterion referenced assessment measures.
  • 3. Keywords assessment, ceiling effects, narrative, language development, oral language, psychometrics 604 Language Testing 27(4) Introduction Oral narrative skills in children Assessment of children’s oral narratives is of significant interest to researchers and practitioners, as being a proficient narrator is an important skill in the life of young children. Oral narrative skills are a key component of most school curricula. In the USA, each state is required to develop guidelines describing skills that need to be incor- porated into the general curriculum. While oral language skills are often not formally assessed in the general classroom, they are a key component in each state’s guidelines. For example, the state of North Carolina requires teachers to facilitate mastery of nar- rative comprehension as well as effective ability to produce narratives and the complex language associated with a literate speaking style (North Carolina Department of Public Instruction, 2004). Oral narrative skills have been incorporated into children’s curricula for good reasons. Being a proficient narrator is a skill needed to express one’s intentions and effectively participate in classroom activities. In addition, several decades of research have documented the strong link between children’s
  • 4. oral narrative skills and broader curricular requirements. Research on monolingual English-speaking children has documented that chil- dren’s oral narrative skills are predictive of later reading outcomes (Bishop & Edmundson, 1987; Dickinson & McCabe, 2001; Griffin, Hemphill, Camp, & Wolf, 2004; Hemphill & Snow, 1996; Roth, Speece, Cooper, & de la Paz, 1996; Snow, 1983; Snow, Dickinson, Jennings, & Purves, 1991; Tabors, Snow, & Dickinson, 2001). Additional studies have documented that children’s early narrative competence is related to broader academic outcomes (e.g., Fazio, Naremore, & Connell, 1996; O’Neill, Pearce, & Pick, 2004). Fazio and colleagues found that oral narrative skills were one of the strongest predictors for whether or not a child later required academic remediation, while O’Neill et al. identified a strong relationship between young children’s oral narrative skills and later mathematical ability. While these studies documenting the relationship between oral narratives and broader reading and aca- demic outcomes are correlational and a causal relationship cannot be assumed, there is a general consensus in the field that oral narrative skills may play a key role in developing the foundation for higher level academic tasks. It is only natural that oral narrative skills are of interest to those working with children who have language impairments (LI). Approximately
  • 5. 7% of monolingual English-speaking children experience significant deficits in their oral language skills despite normal cognitive skills (Tomblin, Records, Buckwalter, Zhang, Smith, & O’Brien, 1997). Children with LI have substantial difficulty producing fully coherent oral narratives (Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Fey, Catts, Proctor- Williams, Tomblin, & Zhang, 2004; Gillam & Johnston, 1992; Newman & McGregor, 2006; Pearce, McCormack, & James, 2003; Reilly, Losh, Bellugi, & Wulfeck, 2004). Assessment of children’s narratives can be an effective method of identifying the presence of LI (Allen, Kertoy, Sherblom, & Petit, 1994; Paul & Smith, 1993) and provides a functional description of children’s performance, which may assist in the development of treatment goals (Miller, Gillam, & Peña, 2001). Heilmann et al. 605 Oral narratives in bilingual children Clinicians, teachers, and researchers are also interested in better understanding the oral narrative skills of children learning a second language. Just as with monolingual chil- dren, there is a significant predictive relationship between oral narrative skills and read- ing outcomes in children learning a second language (August & Shanahan, 2006; Miller, Heilmann, Nockerts, Iglesias, Fabiano, & Francis, 2006; Oller & Pearson, 2002). Miller
  • 6. et al. identified that measures from oral narratives predicted reading scores within and across languages in young school-age English language learners (ELL). Given the strong relationship between oral narratives and reading, it is important to ensure that young ELLs have sufficient oral narrative skills. Because a disproportionately high number of ELLs have poor reading outcomes when compared to their monolingual counterparts (August & Hakuta, 1997), there is a need for a better understanding of the relationship between oral narrative skills and reading outcomes. Numerous speech and language scholars have recommended using oral narratives in clinical assessments of bilingual children, given the naturalness of the task (e.g., Fiestas & Peña, 2004; Munoz, Gillam, Peña, & Gulley-Faehnle, 2003; Uccelli & Páez, 2007). The act of telling stories is universal across cultures and is an important instrument for transmitting information (Mandler, Scribner,Cole, & DeForest, 1980); the format of tell- ing a story may be more familiar to children from different cultures than formal language testing. Analysis of oral narratives across multiple languages can provide an estimate of relative proficiency in each language spoken and may assist in differentiating language differences, where a children may have limited proficiency in one of their languages spoken, from global language disorders, where children demonstrate marked deficits in all languages spoken (Gutiérrez-Clellen, 2002; Rojas & Iglesias, 2009).
  • 7. Criterion referenced assessment Oral narrative analysis falls under the broader umbrella of criterion referenced (CR) assessment. The goal of CR assessment is to generate a detailed description of a child’s performance in a target domain (e.g., narrative organization) and to identify if the child’s performance meets developmental expectations (Linn & Gronlund, 2000). While the purpose of standardized tests is to maximize differences between individuals, CR assess- ments afford clinicians the opportunity to obtain more detailed data on specific skill areas (Bachman, 2000). While standardized language tests commonly evaluate limited sets of skills that have been stripped of their communicative context (Marquardt & Gillam, 1999), most CR oral language assessments, including narrative language sample analysis, examine real-life communication situations and do not rely on assessment of decontextualized language skills. Because CR assessments can generate rich descriptive data, they are particularly use- ful when describing the profile of a child exhibiting language learning difficulties. While children with LI share the same underlying characteristic (i.e., substantial difficulty with language), there is considerable heterogeneity in the patterns of difficulties experienced by these children (Rapin & Allen, 1983; Tomblin, Zhang, Weiss, Catts, & Ellis Weismer,
  • 8. 606 Language Testing 27(4) 2004). Most standardized language tests provide subtest scores that would appear to be useful for describing children’s profiles (e.g., receptive vocabulary; expressive syntax). However, the measurement properties of subtests prohibit use for providing accurate descriptions of performance patterns; subtest scores are generated from a relatively small number of items, making them unreliable for interpretation on their own (McCauley & Swisher, 1984). In order to fully identify a child’s pattern of language performance, evaluations should use detailed CR assessments to identify children’s strength and weak- ness. For example, comprehensive assessment of children’s oral narrative skills may reveal relative strengths and weaknesses in using appropriately complex vocabulary and syntax, correctly referencing the characters, providing sufficient description of the major events, organizing the events of the story, and making the story interesting to the listener. Such assessment can provide a clearer understanding of the nature of a child’s language difficulties and may assist in the development of treatment goals. While CR assessment tasks have many admirable properties, they can be difficult to implement, given the unstandardized nature of the task. With the increased call for use of naturalistic and CR assessment tasks, there is a need to understand better the measure-
  • 9. ment properties of these assessment tasks. Difficulties associated with developing CR assessments As stated by Laing and Kamhi (2003, p. 46), ‘criterion- referenced measures are only as good as the developmental data on which they are based.’ In the field of language devel- opment, decades of research have provided a rich and expansive literature documenting the complexities associated with acquiring human language. Language use is complex and difficult to measure, as it encompasses multiple aspects of form, content, and use (Bloom & Lahey, 1978). At any stage of development, different aspects of children’s vocabulary, grammar, and pragmatic skills are at very different stages of development. To complicate matters further, language use is highly influenced by the broader speaking context; a behavior that appears to be immature in one situation can appear to be mas- tered in another situation (Elman, 1995). When identifying appropriate CR assessment measures, clinicians and researchers must critically evaluate the assessment tasks and sampling procedures to determine whether they are developmentally appropriate and will generate sensitive measures for the target population. Ollendick, Grills, and King (2001) stated that developmentally appropriate measures should measure meaningful behaviors that are sensitive to the children’s developmental levels and be responsive to changes in the context that may affect the sensitivity of the measure.
  • 10. Measures from children’s productions of oral narratives are highly sensitive to changes in the sampling context and discourse demands of the task. Peña et al. (2006) described how increased amounts of support provided to the child, or scaffolding, facili- tates children’s production of oral narratives. Factors affecting the complexity and over- all quality of a child’s narrative production include whether or not the child had heard the story before (Ripich & Griffith, 1988; Schneider & Dubé, 2005), how many times the child had heard the story (Goodsitt, Raitan, & Perlmutter, 1988; Martinez & Roser, 1985), familiarity with the events depicted in the story (Fivush, 1984; Hudson & Shapiro, Heilmann et al. 607 1991), complexity of the story (Heilmann, Miller, Iglesias, & Francis, 2009), and the types of directions and expectations provided to the child (de Temple, Wu, & Snow, 1991). For example, if a child is unfamiliar with a story, he or she may have substantial difficulty in organizing the complex events and including more nuanced points of view. If, however, a child is familiar with a story and is asked to relate the story in a complex manner, he or she will likely produce a longer, more detailed story. Such variations can present a problem for measurement of children’s narrative organization skills.
  • 11. Measuring children’s narrative organization skills The ability to produce a coherent narrative is a complex linguistic task that requires nar- rators to plan and execute their production of the story’s plotline by using appropriate vocabulary, grammar, and syntax. Studies examining the development of narrative have identified that all stories possess the same underlying components, or story grammar (Stein & Glenn, 1979). The story grammar literature has proposed that all stories contain a setting and episode system, which includes an initiating event or problem, a reaction to that problem, various attempts at resolving the problem, a conclusion, and resolution. All stories use some kind of combination of these story grammar components. Developmental studies have revealed that the acquisition of narrative proficiency is a slow process, which emerges in the preschool years and is not fully developed until adulthood, with some adults never becoming fully proficient narrators (Berman & Slobin, 1994). Analysis of children’s narrative organization skills has been described in several clinical texts (Hughes, McGillivray, & Schmidek, 1997; Strong, 1998), but the measurement properties of the various scoring procedures have not been empirically tested with large samples. Upon developing a series of narrative databases, the Language Analysis Lab at the University of Wisconsin-Madison was interested in identifying a developmentally sensi- tive narrative organization measure that that could be used on a corpus of narrative retells
  • 12. produced by children using the wordless picture book Frog, Where are You? (Mayer, 1969). We first looked to the literature for sensitive narrative organization measures for this corpus and identified studies measuring narrative organization in children who pro- duced Frog, Where are You? The review was limited to this specific story because it has been extensively reported in the literature, follows a prototypical story sequence, and was to be used for several projects completed in the Language Analysis Lab, including the data summarized in this study. The first major method for measuring children’s narrative organization skills identi- fied whether or not children included specific plotlines and themes (i.e., plot and theme analysis). When applying plot and theme analyses, examiners developed a coding scheme using the key story grammar elements for the target story. This approach allowed exam- iners to develop binary decision schemes that identified the presence or absence of spe- cific story grammar components. Children who produced a greater number of plotlines and themes were thought to have more advanced narrative skills (e.g., Berman, 1988; Botting, 2002; Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Miles & Chapman, 2002; Norbury & Bishop, 2003; Reilly, Losh, Bellugi, & Wulfeck, 2004). An example of a plot and theme analysis is provided in Appendix A.
  • 13. 608 Language Testing 27(4) The second major class of narrative organization measures relied on holistic judgments of children’s narrative proficiency (Applebee, 1978; Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Stein, 1988). While the text-level analyses also documented children’s production of story gram- mar components, they were not measured by counting the presence or absence of specific plotlines and themes. Rather, these measures required holistic judgments by the examiner to rate the quality and developmental level of the narrative. Two contemporary studies used the text-level measures of narrative organization for analysis of Frog, Where are You? Manhardt and Rescorla (2002) converted Applebee’s categorical levels to an ordinal scale to assess narratives produced by 8–9-year-old children with histories of language delay, while Pearce, McCormack, and James (2003) used Stein’s scoring scheme to assess narratives produced by children of 5 ½ years of age. A summary and brief description of these two holistic narrative scoring procedures is provided in Appendix A. Several of the reported studies compared performance on the story grammar measures across clinical groups with varying results. Reilly et al. (2004) documented that their story grammar analysis was sensitive to both age and group differences in four groups of children of between 7 and 9 years of age (children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, Williams syndrome, and typical
  • 14. developmental histories). Pearce et al. (2003) found significant differences in performance between children with lan- guage impairment and typically developing children at 5 ½ years of age. Boudreau and Hedberg (1999), on the other hand, did not find any differences between typically devel- oping children and children with specific language impairment on their narrative organi- zation measure despite significant differences on each additional measure collected from the children’s narrative productions. McCabe and Rollins (1994) also noted that the nar- rative organization skills of children with language impairment varied widely across studies and attributed this variability to ‘the use of insensitive means of scoring narra- tives’ (p. 47). Further examination of the literature revealed that these existing story grammar measures may be too easy and potentially insensitive for preschool and young school-age children. For the plot and theme measures, Reilly et al. documented that typi- cally developing 7–9-year-old children, on average, produced 95% (11.4/12) of the plot- lines and themes, while Boudreau and Hedberg documented that their group of 5-year-old children produced 78% (4.7/6) of the plotlines and themes. On average, high success rates were also noted for the text-level measures, with a group of 8-year-old children scoring 3.3/5 on the Applebee measure (Manhardt & Rescorla, 2002) and 5–6-year-old children scoring 9/11 on the Stein measure (Pearce et al., 2003). In identifying the best story grammar measure for analyzing
  • 15. retells of a wordless picture book, the potential sensitivity issue was compounded by the sampling context. Our corpus of narratives was collected using the retell procedure, whereas most studies from the literature did not provide an initial model of the target story. Having children retell a story provides a model story and assists children in developing an understanding of the story’s structure. Narrative retells have been shown to contain more information and incorporate a greater number of episodes than productions of spontaneous narratives (Ripich & Griffith, 1988; Schneider & Dubé, 2005). Our goal in using the retell procedure was to ensure that the samples represented the children’s best narrative productions. However, we anticipated that a simple story grammar analysis would be ineffective for this corpus of retells given that children include a greater amount of information in their oral Heilmann et al. 609 productions in the retell condition. Thus, we decided that existing narrative organization measures may not be appropriate and developed a new scale to allow for more sensitive measurement of narrative proficiency. The Narrative Scoring Scheme: A comprehensive measure of narrative organization skills The Narrative Scoring Scheme (NSS) was developed by the Language Analysis Lab at the
  • 16. University of Wisconsin – Madison as a sensitive measure of children’s overall narrative organization skills. To make the NSS more sensitive for a wider range of ages and for sam- pling contexts that provide scaffolding (i.e., the narrative retell procedure), two modifica- tions were made from the other narrative organization measures. First, the literature was reviewed to identify later developing narrative organization features that go beyond simple story grammar analyses. Second, scoring procedures were critiqued to identify methods that may be more sensitive when evaluating more advanced narrative productions. In addition to incorporating basic story grammar features, fully proficient narrators use specific types of language features that define a literate style of speaking (Bamberg & Damrad-Frye, 1991). One way narrators use a literate style of speaking is through the use of metacognitive verbs, which include verbs used to describe the characters’ thoughts and men- tal states (e.g., think and know) and metalinguistic verbs, which include words used to describe characters’ speech and dialogue (e.g., say and talk; see Nippold, 2007 and Westby, 2005 for a review). In their analysis of oral productions of Frog, Where are You?, Bamberg & Damrad-Frye contrasted productions from novice narrators (i.e., young children) to those from experienced narrators (i.e., older children and adults) and found that use of abstract language, including metacognitive and metalinguistic verbs, were the key features that char- acterized the more sophisticated narrative productions and
  • 17. assisted the older narrators in organizing the hierarchical relationships between the events in the story. They found that these high-level language skills were first seen around five years of age, were not consistently used until the later school-age years, and continued to develop through adulthood. Additional studies have documented that these abstract language features, collectively termed literate language, emerged during the preschool years (Curenton & Justice, 2004), were more consis- tently used as children progressed through adolescence (Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001; Nippold, 2007; Pelligrini, Galda, Bartini, & Charak, 1998), and were not consistently or appropriately used by children with language impairment (Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001). Another high-level skill that is positively related to children’s narrative organization skills is the effective use of cohesive devices. Halliday and Hasan (1976) characterized the various ways that children maintain concepts across utterances through use of sophisticated linguistic procedures, including referential cohesion, which is the way that speakers main- tain appropriate reference throughout the story with correct use of nouns and pronouns, conjunctive cohesion, which is the way that speakers combine phrases and sentences with conjunctions and conjunctive phrases, and lexical cohesion, which is the way that speakers choose appropriate words to link concepts across phrases and sentences. Children with language impairment have much more difficulty producing cohesive features than their
  • 18. typically developing peers (Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Liles, 1985; Strong & Shaver, 1991). 610 Language Testing 27(4) In addition to incorporating higher level narrative skills into the NSS, we attempted to increase the sensitivity of the measure by critically reviewing the scoring procedures and scaling of the measure. McFadden and Gillam (1996) compared two methods of scoring children’s narrative organization skills: discrete coding schemes based on presence or absence of story features and holistic ratings of children’s narrative proficiency. They found that the holistic ratings were superior to the discrete coding schemes in discrimi- nating between children who were typically developing and children with language impairment. These children were between the ages of 9;0 and 11;7. McFadden and Gillam’s study demonstrated that holistic ratings can effectively assess the inter- utterance concepts and qualitative aspects of the story, including the story’s sparkle (Peterson & McCabe, 1983). To incorporate holistic ratings into a detailed and descriptive rating scale, the NSS requires examiners to make broad judgments across the seven aspects of narrative organization included in the scale. The goal of this paper was to compare the measurement properties of the NSS with other methods of operationalizing narrative organization from
  • 19. the literature. This study provided a direct comparison of four separate scoring techniques using the same pool of transcripts and identified the measures that were most developmentally appropriate. The literature review revealed that many of the existing narrative organization measures may focus too much on early developing narrative skills, such as the inclusion of key story grammar components. We felt that incorporating higher level narrative skills would make the scale developmentally appropriate for young school- age children who pro- duced narratives with scaffolding from the retell procedure. In addition, the NSS required examiners to provide a level of holistic judgment to assess the qualitative aspects of the story. To assess the sensitivity of the measures, the distribution of scores was evaluated for each narrative scoring technique to determine which measures were developmentally appropriate for the participants and sampling context used in the present study. This study addressed the following question: Is the distribution of scores from the NSS less skewed than scores from three traditional narrative organization measures when applied to narrative retells produced by 5–7-year-old children? Method Narrative language samples were collected from 129 typically developing children between 5;0 and 7;0 years of age. The children were recruited from public schools in the San Diego County and El Cajon County school districts. The school speech-language
  • 20. pathologist (SLP) and the children’s classroom teachers confirmed that the children were typically developing by reviewing all academic records and summative assessments. To be included in the study, children had to be performing at grade level and could not be receiving special education or speech/language services. See Table 1 for a summary of the participants’ demographic data. The narrative samples were collected by a practicing SLP working in the schools under the direction of a project coordinator. The SLPs met with the project coordinator for three sessions to learn the protocol, where they had the opportunity to develop an understanding of the protocol, ask for clarification, and prac- tice administering the assessment. Heilmann et al. 611 The children completed the narrative task in the retell condition, where the examiner read the target story aloud to the child, cued the child to follow along with the pictures in a wordless picture book (Frog, Where are You?; Mayer, 1969), and then asked the child to retell the story to the examiner. This procedure was adapted from the Strong Narrative Assessment Procedure (Strong, 1998). The children’s language samples were digitally recorded and later transcribed by research assistants (RA) at the University of Wisconsin – Madison. Each RA had at least 10 hours of training in completing transcription of chil-
  • 21. dren’s oral language samples. The narrative samples were first transcribed using the Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT; Miller & Iglesias, 2008). After tran- scribing the sample, the RA reviewed the written transcript and completed the NSS scor- ing (see Appendix B for scoring rules). The NSS is divided into seven sections that assess seven different aspects of narrative organization. Three of the sections were modeled after the original story grammar pro- posals: introduction, conflict resolution, and conclusion. Use of literate language skills were evaluated with the mental states and character development sections. The final two sections evaluated children’s cohesion skills and included referencing and cohesion. Within each section, scores of 1 were administered if the child demonstrated immature performance, scores of 3 if the child demonstrated emerging skills, and scores of 5 if proficient performance was noted. After completing each section of the NSS, the scores were added together to generate a total NSS score, which could range from 0 to 35. To compare performance on the different narrative organization measures, the NSS scores were compared to scores using three additional narrative organization measures from the literature. For the plot and theme approach, the protocol described in Reilly et al. (2004) was chosen because it provided a balanced combination of the core plot components and key embedded episodes. For the text-level
  • 22. measures, we used Manhardt and Rescorla’s (2002) ordinal adaptation of Applebee’s (1978) narrative levels and Pearce et al.’s (2003) ordinal adaptation of Stein’s (1988) narrative levels. See Appendix A for a description of the scoring procedures. Each of the narrative organization analyses was scored by a trained research assistant. To document inter-rater agreement, 20% of the narrative organization analyses were inde- pendently coded by the first author. Krippendorff alpha values (Krippendorff, 1980) were calculated with ordinal scaling to determine the level of agreement between transcribers. Table 1. Demographic information for the 129 children completing the narrative retell task Grade (n) Gender (n) Maternal education (in years) Race/ethnicity (n) Preschool: 3 Female: 69 M = 14.4 White: 87 Kindergarten: 79 Male: 60 SD = 2.5 Hispanic: 16 1st Grade: 47 Range = 9–20 Othera: 15 African American: 7 No data: 4 Note: Ethnicity data collected for children who were Hispanic or Latino. Race data provided for all children who were non-Hispanic or Latino. aOther races/ethnicities include Arabic (2), Chinese (3), Japanese (2), Korean (1), Filipino (5), Portuguese (1), and Samoan (1).
  • 23. 612 Language Testing 27(4) Krippendorff established the following benchmarks for alpha values: ≥0.80 is adequate and values between 0.67 and 0.80 are acceptable for exploratory research and drawing tentative conclusions. The following alpha values were calculated from samples used in the present study: NSS = 0.79, Plot & Theme = 0.79, Applebee = 0.61, and Stein = 0.69. Results The normality of the distributions was compared using skewness and kurtosis statistics (see Coolican, 2004, for a review). The skewness statistic identifies if there is an unequal distribution that goes towards the floor or ceiling. Skewness measures of zero indicate a perfectly normal distribution while skewness values exceeding |0.8| have been described as ‘noticeably skewed’ (Bourque & Clark, 1992, p. 69). If the distribution of scores is negatively skewed, then most of the scores are approaching mastery levels, reflecting that the measure was too easy for the child. The kurtosis statistic identifies how closely variables are bunched together; platykurtic distributions have scores that are spread out and leptokurtic distributions have scores that are bunched closely together (Coolican, 2004). Skewness and kurtosis statistics were generated for all four scoring procedures using SPSS. The traditional kurtosis statistics were converted so that they were on the same scale as the skewness statistics, with zero indicating a
  • 24. perfectly normal distribu- tion. While these analyses do not generate tests of statistical significance, they do allow comparisons to be drawn regarding the relative skewness and kurtosis across different measures. A final method for documenting the relative distribution of scores was to establish a score representing near-mastery performance and identify the percentage of the participants that scored at or above that criteria, similar to a procedure employed by Helms et al. (2004). For the present study, achieving at or above 90% correct was con- sidered near-mastery performance and therefore a ceiling effect. Table 2 summarizes the scores acquired from each scoring scheme, showing the full range of possible scores, the range acquired from this initial sample, the sample means and standard deviations, the skewness and kurtosis statistics, and the percentage of the sample who scored greater than 90% correct on the measure. The skewness statistics for all of the narrative measures were negative, demonstrating that the distribution of scores were more concentrated towards the ceiling. Skewness for the NSS was considerably lower than the additional measures from the literature. Furthermore, skewness statistics exceeded |0.8| for the plot and theme, Applebee, and Stein measures, demonstrating that these measures were ‘noticeably skewed’ (Bourque & Clark, 1992). The kurtosis values for each of the narrative measures were greater than zero, documenting that the narrative measures were generating leptokurtic distributions that had a
  • 25. relatively large proportion of scores surrounding the mean. Again, the kurtosis values were noticeably greater for the three measures from the literature (0.9–1.1) than scores from the NSS (0.5), showing that there was a more restricted distribution for the plot and theme, Applebee, and Stein measures. The final analysis identified the number of participants who scored close to ceiling, with the criteria set at a score equal to or greater than 90% correct. None of the children scored above 90% on the NSS, while 30–35% of the children scored above 90% on the three measures from the literature. Histograms were generated for each of the Heilmann et al. 613 narrative measures to visualize the distribution for each narrative organization measure (see Figure 1). Examination of Figure 1 illustrates the heavy weighting of scores near ceiling and around the mean for the three measures from the literature and the relatively normal distribution of the NSS. Sample Narratives To further illustrate differences across the scoring techniques, three samples were selected and are available in Appendix C. The samples were modified from their original coded format to assist with readability. All codes required for SALT analyses were
  • 26. Table 2. Children’s performance across four separate narrative organization measures Possible range Sample range Sample Mean (SD) Skewness Kurtosis > 90% Plot & Theme 0–12 4–12 8.8 (2.0) −1.0 1.1 35% Applebee 0–5 2–6 4.0 (1.0) −1.1 0.9 30% Stein 0–11 2–10 6.8 (2.4) −1.0 0.9 30% NSS 0–35 11–26 20.1 (3.2) −0.5 0.5 0% Note: > 90% signifies the proportion of the sample who scored above 90% correct on the measure. 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
  • 28. q u e n c y 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Stein F re q u e n
  • 29. c y 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 0 3 6 9 12 15 18 21 24 27 30 33 NSS F re q u e n c y Figure 1. Histograms depicting the distribution of scores using four narrative structure coding schemes
  • 30. 614 Language Testing 27(4) removed, including word errors, utterance errors, mazes (which include reduplications, reformulations, and false starts), and slashes used to identify bound morphemes and contractions. While these codes are required for syntactic and semantic analysis of the narrative samples, they are not necessary to accurately analyze narrative structure. Samples 1, 2, and 3 reflect a range of performance on the narrative retell task. By simply reading the transcripts, even an untrained reader with no experience in analyzing children’s oral language can identify that sample 3 is much poorer than the other two samples. It is considerably shorter and more difficult to follow than samples 1 and 2. Many readers will also note that sample 1 is more advanced than sample 2. In story 1, the narrator provided more detail, better described each of the events, and used complex language to make the story interesting for the listener. Upon comparing the dif- ferent narrative organization measures, each measure was effective in identifying that story 3 was less complete and more poorly organized than stories 1 and 2. However, the plot and theme and two holistic narrative organization measures did not distinguish sto- ries 1 and 2 from each other. Rather, the NSS was the only measure sensitive enough to reveal differences between the productions.
  • 31. Discussion Sensitivity analyses This study compared the measurement properties of the NSS to three other narrative organization measures from the literature. Each of the 129 transcripts was scored using the four scoring schemes summarized in Appendix A. Twenty percent of the transcripts were recoded by a second member of the research team to document the level of agree- ment between transcribers. Krippendorff alpha coefficients were equivalent for the NSS and plot and theme measures (α = 0.79), while agreement values for the Applebee and Stein measures (α = 0.61 and 0.69, respectively) were notably lower. We anticipated that the highest level of inter-rater agreement would occur with the plot and theme measures, as the coding task was limited to identification of specific story components. We were impressed that the coding procedures of the NSS facilitated comparable agreement val- ues when compared to the plot and theme measures and strikingly higher agreement levels than the two text-level scoring scales. Upon comparing the distribution of scores across the four scoring schemes, scores from the plot and theme and text-level narrative organization measures were noticeably more skewed and leptokurtic than scores from the NSS, demonstrating that the majority of the scores were closely bunched together near the ceiling.
  • 32. Furthermore, approximately one third of the children demonstrated near-mastery performance and were approaching ceiling on the measures from the literature, while none of the children approached ceiling on the NSS. Upon scoring the narratives with the NSS, we observed a wider distribution of scores, a relatively normal distribution across participants, and ample room for measurement of children with higher and lower narrative skills. We hypothesize that there were two reasons for the increased sensitivity of the NSS. First, by incorporating children’s use of literate language and cohesion, the NSS measured skills that were later developing and/or were Heilmann et al. 615 present when children receive scaffolding. Second, by utilizing examiner judgment, the NSS was able to tap into the perceptual aspects of the narrative process (i.e., ‘sparkle’) that are missed by discrete scoring schemes. Use of a more sensitive scale, such as the NSS, may address the sensitivity issue described by McCabe and Rollins (1994) and provide a more sensitive measure that can distinguish between typically developing children and children with language impairment (cf., Boudreau and Hedberg, 1999). Because children’s narrative skills continue to develop through the school years, a more sensitive measure that assesses higher level narrative
  • 33. features allows clinicians and researchers to document narrative organization skills in older school-age children. In the present study, the child with the strongest narrative organization skills received a score of 26 on the NSS, while a perfect score would be 35. Based on these data, there are an additional 9 points that can be earned on the NSS for even the best narrators. We predict that NSS scores continue to increase in older children with stronger language skills and continue to develop additional reference databases to evaluate a wider range of ages. It is important to note that the data from the present study were potentially constrained by the limited sampling context. The data were acquired from a relatively short and sim- ple story. The extensive use of Frog, Where are You? in the literature has greatly increased our understanding of how children develop narrative proficiency. However, additional work is needed to document the effect of different stories on measures of narrative orga- nization. The plot and theme and text-level narrative organization measures may be more sensitive when children have the opportunity to produce longer and more complex narra- tives. From a clinical standpoint, however, increasing the story length and complexity increases the time requirements for elicitation and analysis of the language samples. Our goal was to use a story that could be collected and analyzed quickly and efficiently. Use of the NSS in research, educational, and clinical contexts
  • 34. Research examining the mastery of oral narrative skills in children can assist in better understanding of the development of broader cognitive skills. Studies of children’s developing narrative have improved our understanding of the development of cognitive schemas; Berman and Slobin (1994) completed the most comprehensive description of the relationship between linguistic form and function. The study of narrative develop- ment has also shaped theories about the relationship between socio-linguistic factors and developing narrative competence (Eaton, Collis, & Lewis, 1995; McCabe, 1997; Peterson & McCabe, 2004; Quasthoff, 1997) as well as the relationship between oral narratives and general cognitive skills, such as working memory (e.g., van den Broek, 1997). Having a sensitive measure of children’s narrative organization skills, such as the NSS, is essential for documenting children’s narrative organization skills and will facilitate further advancements of our understanding of human cognition. A sensitive and feasible measure of narrative organization, such as the NSS, has important educational policy implications. While oral language skills are a key part of most schools’ curricula, curriculum based assessment in the general classroom has pri- marily focused on documentation of children’s reading skills (see Reschly, Busch, Betts, Deno, & Long, 2009). Continuing to recognize the important relationship between
  • 35. 616 Language Testing 27(4) oral narrative skills and broader reading and academic achievements will encourage greater advocacy for regular assessment of children’s oral narrative skills in the general curriculum. However, for this to occur, teachers and practitioners need to have sensitive and efficient measures, such as the NSS. The NSS can also be an important tool for clinicians working with children with lan- guage impairment and for those working with second language learners. The composite NSS score provides a single estimate of children’s overall narrative competence that is sensitive for young children producing narrative retells. Given the flexibility of the mea- sure, it is likely also appropriate to use with children producing more complex and less complex narratives than those produced by the children in this study. NSS composite scores may also be used to assist with the identification of children experiencing lan- guage learning difficulties. Several databases are available with the SALT software that allows clinicians to compare their clients’ performance to a sample of typically develop- ing speakers. One database summarizes NSS scores collected from monolingual English- speaking children producing narrative retells; the samples used in this study are included in that database. The second major database with NSS scores was collected on a large group of English language learners who produced narrative
  • 36. retells in both English and Spanish. In addition to a composite narrative score, the NSS provides examiners with estimates of children’s performance within seven different aspects of the narrative pro- cess. Examiners can examine the profile of performance across each aspect of the NSS to identify areas of relative strength and relative weakness, which may assist with further determining the nature of a child’s language impairment and assist with the development of treatment goals. Upon initiating treatment, the NSS can be used to monitor children’s progress during the intervention program. Conclusion Decades of research and clinical practice literature have identified the limitations of norm referenced testing and have identified CR assessment as a viable alternative for overcom- ing many of these shortcomings. CR assessments provide a detailed description of chil- dren’s performance when completing naturalistic and meaningful tasks. With the call for increased use of CR assessments, more rigorous testing of the properties of these tasks must be completed. When evaluating assessment measures, factors influencing the devel- opmental sensitivity of the measure should be considered, including the linguistic features under study and the scaling of the assessment measure. In this paper, we reviewed four different methods of measuring children’s narrative organization skills and found that the NSS was more sensitive than the three other measures for young school-age children
  • 37. completing a narrative retell procedure. These analyses identified features that allowed for more sensitive analysis of narrative organization skills, including assessment of higher level narrative concepts and incorporation of examiner judgment across multiple narrative features. These analyses also provided an example of methods that can be used to evaluate the measurement properties of additional CR assessment procedures. Such analyses and discussion are increasingly important as more clinicians are implementing the CR assess- ment into clinical practice. Heilmann et al. 617 Acknowledgements We would like to thank Claudia Dunaway and the SLPs in the San Diego and El Cajon school districts for their assistance in collecting the narrative retell data. We would also like to thank Karen Andriacchi and all of the past and present members of the language analysis lab who have been dedicated to accurately transcribing and efficiently organizing all of the language samples. References Allen, M. S., Kertoy, M. K., Sherblom, J. C., & Pettit, J. M. (1994). Children’s narrative productions: A comparison of personal event and fictional stories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15(2), 149–176.
  • 38. Applebee, A. (1978). The child’s concept of story: Ages two to seven. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. August, D., & Hakuta, K. (1997). Improving schooling for language-minority children. Washing- ton, DC: National Academy Press. August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.) (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Bachman, L. (2000). Modern language testing at the turn of the century: Assuring that what we count counts. Language Testing, 17(1), 1–42. Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competencies. Journal of Child Language, 18(3), 689–710. Berman, R. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative. Discourse Processes, 11(4), 469–497. Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (Eds.) (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic develop- mental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Bishop, D., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4- year-olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 52(2), 156–173. Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders. New York: Wiley.
  • 39. Botting, N. (2002). Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic impairments. Child Language Teaching & Therapy, 18(1), 1–22. Boudreau, D. M., & Hedberg, N. (1999). A comparison of early literacy skills in children with specific language impairment and typically developing peers. American Journal of Speech- Language Pathology, 8(3), 249–260. Boudreau, D., & Hedberg, N. (1999). A comparison of early literacy skills in children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8(3), 249–260. Bourque, L., & Clark, V. (1992). Processing data: The survey example. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Coolican, H. (2004). Research Methods and Statistics in Psychology. London: Hodder & Stoughton. Curenton, S., & Justice, L. (2004). African American and Caucasian preschoolers’ use of decon- textualized language: Literate language features in oral narratives. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 35(3), 240–253. de Temple, J. M., Wu, H.-f., & Snow, C. E. (1991). Papa Pig just left for Pigtown: Children’s oral and written picture descriptions under varying instructions. Discourse Processes, 14(4), 469–495.
  • 40. 618 Language Testing 27(4) Dickinson, D. K., & McCabe, A. (2001). Bringing it all together: The multiple origins, skills, and environmental supports of early literacy. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16(4), 186–202. Eaton, J., Collis, G., & Lewis, V. (1999). Evaluative explanations in children’s narratives of a video sequence without dialogue. Journal of Child Language, 26(3), 699–720. Elman, J. (1995). Language as a dynamical system. In R. Port & T. van Gelder (Eds.), Mind as motion: Exploring the dynamics of cognition (pp. 195–223). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Fazio, B., Naremore, R., & Connell, P. (1996). Tracking children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(3), 52–63. Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2004). Oral and written story composition skills of children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1301–1318. Fiestas, C. E., & Peña, E. D. (2004). Narrative discourse in bilingual children: Language and task effects. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35(2), 155–168.
  • 41. Fivush, R. (1984). Learning about school: The development of kindergartners’ school scripts. Child Development, 55(5), 1697–1709. Gillam, R. B., & Johnston, J. R. (1992). Spoken and written language relationships in language/ learning-impaired and normally achieving school-age children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 35(6), 1303–1315. Goodsitt, J., Raitan, J. G., & Perlmutter, M. (1988). Interaction between mothers and preschool chil- dren when reading a novel and familiar book. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11(4), 489–505. Greenhalgh, K., & Strong, C. (2001). Literate language features in spoken narratives of children with typical language and children with language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32(2), 114–125. Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language, 24(71), 123–147. Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. (2002). Narratives in two languages: Assessing performance of bilingual children. Linguistics and Education, 13(2), 175–197. Halliday, M., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman. Hedberg, N., and Westby, C. (1993). Analyzing storytelling skills: Theory to practice. Tucson, AZ: Communication Skill Builders.
  • 42. Heilmann, J., Miller, J., Iglesias, A., & Francis, D. (2009, June). Differences in oral narratives as a function of story in bilingual children. Paper presented at the Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI. Helms, J., Weichbold, V., Baumann, U., von Specht, H., Schon, F., Muller, J., et al. (2004). Analysis of ceiling effects occurring with speech recognition tests in adult cochlear-implant patients. Journal for Oto-Rhino-Laryngology and Its Related Specialties, 66(3), 130–135. Hemphill, L., Snow, C., Olson, D. R., & Torrance, N. (1996). Language and literacy development: Discontinuities and differences. In D. R. Olson, and N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development: New models of learning, teaching and schooling (pp. 173–201). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Hudson, J. A., Shapiro, L. R., McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1991). From knowing to telling: The development of children’s scripts, stories, and personal narratives. In A. McCabe & C. Peterson (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. 89–136). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Heilmann et al. 619 Hughes, D., McGillivray, L., & Schmidek, M. (1997). Guide to narrative language. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
  • 43. Krippendorff, K. (1980). Content analysis: An introduction to its methodology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Laing, S., & Kamhi, A. (2003). Alternative assessment of language and literacy in culturally and linguis- tically diverse populations. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 34(1), 44–55. Liles, B. (1985). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and language-disordered children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 28(1), 123–133. Linn, R. L., & Gronlund, N. E. (2000). Measurement and assessment in teaching (8th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Mandler, J. M., Scribner, S., Cole, M., & DeForest, M. (1980). Cross-cultural invariance in story recall. Child Development, 51(1), 19–26. Manhardt, J., & Rescorla, L. (2002). Oral narrative skills of late talkers at ages 8 and 9. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(1), 1–21. Marquardt, T., & Gillam, R. (1999). Assessment in communication disorders: Some observations on current issues. Language Testing, 16(3), 249–269. Martinez, M., & Roser, N. (1985). Read it again: The value of repeated readings during storytime. The Reading Teacher, 38(8), 782–786. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York: Dial Press. McCabe, A. (1997). Developmental and cross-cultural aspects
  • 44. of children’s narration. In G. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp. 137–174). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. McCabe, A., & Rollins, P. (1994). Assessment of preschool narrative skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 3(1), 45–56. McCauley, R., & Swisher, L. (1984). Use and misuse of norm- referenced tests in clinical assess- ment: A hypothetical case. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49(4), 34–42. McFadden, T., & Gillam, R. (1996). An examination of the quality of narratives produced by children with language disorders. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27(1), 48–56. Miles, S., & Chapman, R. S. (2002). Narrative content as described by individuals with Down syndrome and typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(1), 175–189. Miller, L., Gillam, R., & Peña, E. (2001). Dynamic assessment and intervention: Improving chil- dren’s narrative skills. Austin, TX: Pro-ed. Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & Francis, D. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 21(1), 30–43. Miller, J. F. & Iglesias, A. (2008). Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (Research Version
  • 45. 9.1) [Computer software]. Madison, WI: Language Analysis Lab. Muñoz, M. L., Gillam, R. B., Peña, E. D., & Gulley-Faehnle, A. (2003). Measures of language development in fictional narratives of Latino children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Ser- vices in Schools, 34(4), 332–342. Newman, R. M., & McGregor, K. K. (2006). Teachers and laypersons discern quality differences between narratives produced by children with or without SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(5), 1022–1036. Nippold, M. (2007). Later language development: School-age children, adolescents, and young adults (3rd ed.). Austin, Texas: Pro-ed. Norbury, C., & Bishop, D. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Communication Disorders, 38(3), 287– 313. 620 Language Testing 27(4) North Carolina Department of Public Instruction (2004). English language arts: Standard course of study and grade level competencies, K – 12. Raleigh, NC: North Carolina Department of Public Instruction (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED496004). Oller, D. & Pearson, B. (2002). Assessing the effects of
  • 46. bilingualism: A background. In D. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy development in bilingual children (pp. 3–21). Tonawanda, NY: Multilingual Matters. Ollendick, T., Grills, A., & King, N. (2001). Applying developmental theory to the assessment and treatment of childhood disorders: Does it make a difference? Clinical Psychology & Psycho- therapy, 8, 304–315. O’Neill, D. K., Pearce, M. J., & Pick, J. L. (2004). Preschool children’s narratives and performance on the Peabody Individualized Achievement Test–Revised: Evidence of a relation between early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language, 24(71), 149–183. Paul, R. & Smith, R. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds with normal, impaired, and late-developing language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(3), 592– 598. Pearce, W., McCormack, P, & James, D. (2003). Exploring the boundaries of SLI: Findings from mor- phosyntactic and story grammar analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17(4–5), 325–334. Pelligrini, A., Galda, L., Bartini, M., & Charak, D. (1998). Oral language and literacy learning in content: The role of social relationships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44(1), 38–54. Peña, E., Gillam, R., Malek, M., Ruiz-Felter, R., Resendiz, M., Fiestas, C., & Sabel, T. (2006). Dynamic assessment of school-age children’s narrative ability: An
  • 47. experimental investigation of classifica- tion accuracy. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(5), 1037–1057. Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (2004). Echoing our parents: Parental influences on children’s nar- ration. In Pratt, M. & Fiese, B. (Eds.), Family stories and the life course: Across time and generations (pp. 27–54) Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Quasthoff, U. (1997). An interactive approach to narrative development. In Bamberg, M. (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp. 51–83). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Rapin, I., & Allen, D. (1983). Developmental language disorders: Nosological considerations. In U. Kirk (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language, reading and spelling (pp. 155–184). New York: Academic Press. Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). Frog, Where are you? Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury and Williams Syndrome. In B. Wulfeck & J. Reilly (Eds.), Plasticity and development: Language in atypical children. Spe- cial issue, Brain & Language, 88(2), 229–247. Reschly, A., Busch, T., Betts, J., Deno, S., & Long, J. (2009). Curriculum-Based Measurement Oral Reading as an indicator of reading achievement: A meta- analysis of the correlational evidence. Journal of School Psychology 47(6), 427–469. Ripich, D. N., & Griffith, P. L. (1988). Narrative abilities of
  • 48. children with learning disabilities and nondisabled children: Story structure, cohesion, and propositions. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21(3), 165–173. Rojas, R., & Iglesias, A. (2009, March). Making a case for language sampling. The ASHA Leader, 14, 10–11. Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Cooper, D. H. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of the connection between oral language and early reading. Journal of Educational Research, 95(5), 259–272. Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., Cooper, D. H., & De La Paz, S. (1996). Unresolved mysteries: How do metalinguistic and narrative skills connect with early reading? The Journal of Special Education, 30(3), 257–277. Heilmann et al. 621 Schneider, P., & Dubé, R. (2005). Story presentation effects on children’s retell content. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(1), 52–60. Snow, C. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Edu- cational Review, 53(2), 165–189. Snow, C. E., Dickinson, D. K., Jennings, E. M., & Purves, A. C. (1991). Skills that aren’t basic in a new conception of literacy. In E. Jennings & A. Purves (Eds.), Literate systems and individual
  • 49. lives: Perspectives on literacy and schooling (pp. 179–191). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of storytelling skill. In M. Franklin & S. Barten (Eds.), Child language: A reader. New York: Oxford University Press. Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis for story comprehension in elementary school. In R. Free- dle (Eds.), New directions in discourse processing, Vol. 2. (pp. 53–119). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Strong, C. (1998). The Strong Narrative Assessment Procedure. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications. Strong, C., & Shaver, J. (1991). Stability of cohesion in the spoken narratives of language-impaired and normally developing school-aged children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 34(1), 95–111. Tabors, P., Snow, C., & Dickinson, D. (2001). Homes and schools together: Supporting language and literacy development. In D. Dickinson & P. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with lan- guage (pp. 313–334). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Weiss, A., Catts, H., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2004). Dimensions of indi- vidual differences in communication skills among primary grade children. In M. Rice & S. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotpyes to etiologies (pp. 53–76). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Tomblin, J., Records, N., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E.,
  • 50. & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260. Uccelli, P., & Páez, M. M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary development of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and associations among English and Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38(3), 225–236. van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cement of the universe: The development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In P. van den Broek, P. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension: Bridging fictional and actual events (pp. 321–342). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Westby, C. (2005). Assessing and facilitating text comprehension problems. In H. Catts & A. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (pp. 157–232). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Appendix A: Narrative Organization Scoring Procedures 1. Plot and Theme (Reilly et al., 2004) Plotline (8 points possible): Setting – 1 point Instantiation (frog escapes) – 1 point 5 main search episodes – 0–5 points Resolution – 1 point
  • 51. 622 Language Testing 27(4) Theme (4 points possible): Mention that the frog was missing and the boy was looking for him – 0–2 points Search theme mentioned through the story: 0 = no additional mentions, 1 = one or two mentions, 2 = three or more mentions 2. Ordinal adaptation of Applebee’s Narrative Maturity Scale (adapted from Manhardt & Rescorla, 2002 and Hughes et al., 1997) 1. Heap: events described with no central theme 2. Sequence: events related to a single theme; no causal links between story concepts 3. Primitive narrative: stories have a concrete core 4. Focused chain: story revolves around a central character going through a series of events, story well connected 5. True narrative: well-developed story that has a central theme or moral 3. Ordinal adaptation of Stein’s story levels (adapted from Pearce et al., 2003 and Hughes et al., 1997) 1. Isolated description: description of random characters and actions
  • 52. 2. Descriptive sequence: describe characters and actions, but no causal relationships 3. Action sequence: actions described in correct chronological order, but no causal relationships 4. Reactive sequence: series of actions with some causal relationships, no goal- directed descriptions 5. Abbreviated episode: story is goal-directed, but characters’ intent not explicitly stated 6. Incomplete episode: characters’ intent is explicitly stated but one of the following episode components is missing: initiating event, attempt, or consequence 7. Complete episode: story contains all three aspects of a complete episode: initiating event, attempt, and consequence 8. Complex episode: full episode is elaborated by including an obstacle to obtaining the goal 9. Multiple episode: story contains more than one episode (either complete or incomplete) 10. Embedded episodes: one episode embedded within another 11. Interactive episodes: use multiple perspectives to describe events; multiple charac-
  • 53. ters and multiple goals mutually influence each other Heilmann et al. 623 A p p e n d ix B : T he N ar ra ti ve S co ri ng
  • 98. tin ue d) 624 Language Testing 27(4) A p p e n d ix B : (C o nt in ue d) C ha ra
  • 149. r pr o m pt s) . Heilmann et al. 625 Appendix C: Sample Narratives Sample 1: A little boy went out one day and caught a frog. He put the frog in a jar and stared at him when he got in his room again. The dog looked in the jar and saw the frog too. While the boy was sleeping the frog jumped down and ran out the window, while he was sleeping. The next morning the boy woke up and looked at the jar. The frog was not inside the jar. The boy looked everywhere. The dog put his head inside the jar. His head got stuck. Then they looked out the window and called for the frog to come back. The dog fell off the thing and the jar broke. The boy picked him up to see if he was OK. And the dog licked him for that. The dog and the boy went out in the backyard and went in the forest. They searched and searched and searched. But they still couldn’t find the frog. The little boy crawled in a
  • 150. gopher’s hole. The gopher popped out and started running away. The dog was barking at a beehive. And the beehive fell down. And a swarm of bees came out that minute while the boy was looking inside a tree. The bees swarmed and chased the dog. The owl creeped out and scared the boy away. After that he climbed up a tall rock and leaned on some branches. They started moving upwards and turning. This was a deer and not real branches. The dog ran with the deer and started barking at the deer. The deer stopped in a sudden moment. And the boy and the dog fell down. They landed in a warm pond and heard the sound of a croak. The boy told the dog to be quiet because if he wasn’t then the frog would hop away because of the babyies over a dead log. They found his old frog with a mama frog. And they had eight tiny babies. One jumped up to his hand to greet the boy. The boy liked him. And the frog liked him also. So he took that frog for a new pet and left the other frogs together so the family would be safe. And they had seven tiny babies to take care of, not one. Plot & Theme: 11 (search theme reiterated only twice) Applebee: 5 Stein: 10 NSS: 26 • Introduction:4 • CharacterDev:4 • MentalStates:2 • Referencing:4 • ConflictRes:4 • Cohesion:4
  • 151. • Conclusion:4 Sample 2: Once there was a little boy named Tom. He had a little frog and a dog. One night, when Tom and his dog were sleeping, the frog crept away. When Tom and his dog leaned over the next morning, the frog was gone. He looked everywhere for the frog. He called out the window. When he looked into the jar, his head got stuck in the jar. And when he leaned over, he fell out the window. And Tom jumped out and picked him up to see if he was OK. He gave him a lick. And then Tom spent the rest of the day looking for his frog. He called down the hole but there 626 Language Testing 27(4) was a gopher. The dog barked at a tree and some bees scared them. Tom looked into a tree hole. There’s an owl. He climbed up a big rock. And he leaned onto some big branches. But they weren’t branches. They were a deer’s antlers. And the deer ran with Tom on his head. He put the dog and Tom in the water. And they snuck up very quietly. And they found his frog and lots of other frogs. He took the baby frog as his new pet. And he waved bye to his old frogs. Plot & Theme: 11 (search theme reiterated only once) Applebee: 5 Stein: 10 NSS: 21
  • 152. • Introduction:3 • CharacterDev:3 • MentalStates:2 • Referencing:3 • ConflictRes:3 • Cohesion:4 • Conclusion:3 Sample 3: The boy was looking for his frog. All day he looked for the frog and couldn’t find him. Finally a beehive, the dog barked at a beehive in the tree. And the dog got in trouble. And so did the boy because the gopher and the owl. And ran away. And then he chased the dog. And he looked out from it. And then he climbed on the branch. But it wasn’t branches. It was a deer. And the deer shoved him off of the cliff. And then he went to the frog. And he had a family. And then a frog jumped out to get him. And then he took that frog home and left his old frog where his old frog was. Plot & Theme: 5 (Plot: 2 search episodes (beehive and deer scenes), resolution; Theme: initial mention that the boy was looking for the frog, one additional mention of searching for the frog) Applebee: 3 Stein: 5 (Abbreviated Episode): Story is goal-directed (i.e., searching for the frog). But, there are no complete episodes in the story. NSS: 14
  • 153. • Introduction: 1 • CharacterDev: 3 • MentalStates: 1 • Referencing: 3 • ConflictRes: 1 • Cohesion: 2 • Conclusion: 3 Copyright of Language Testing is the property of Sage Publications, Ltd. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. The Development of Narrative Identity in Late Adolescence and Emergent Adulthood: The Continued Importance of Listeners Monisha Pasupathi University of Utah Timothy Hoyt University of New Mexico Research on narrative identity in late adolescence and early adulthood has not extensively examined how conversational storytelling affects the development of narrative
  • 154. identity. This is a major gap, given the importance of this age period for narrative identity development and the clear importance of parent– child conversations in the development of narrative identity. The authors present a series of 3 studies (n � 220) examining how late adolescents and early adults construct narrative identity in ways that are shaped by their listeners. The findings suggest that late adolescents and early adults construct more meaning-laden, interpretive accounts of their everyday experiences when they converse with responsive friends. Further, even within this sample’s abbreviated age range, the authors found evidence for age-related increases in the factual content of personal memories. Such findings illuminate the importance of friends in the construction of narrative identity during this key developmental period. Keywords: narrative identity development, autobiographical memory development, parent– child remi- niscing, social construction Late adolescence and early adulthood are accorded special sig- nificance for the development of self and identity (Arnett, 2000; Dusek & McIntyre, 2003; Erikson & Erikson, 1997; Harter, 1998; Kroger, 2003), narrative identity (Habermas & Bluck, 2000; McLean, 2005), and autobiographical memory (Holmes & Con- way, 1999; Rubin & Schulkind, 1997). In fact, work on the reminiscence bump suggests that adolescence and early adulthood are a privileged developmental period for the encoding of auto- biographical memories, which in narrative terms constitute the stuff of which selves are made. But despite the widespread acknowledgment of the importance
  • 155. of late adolescence and early adulthood for identity development, there are some important gaps in existing research. Perhaps most notable among these is a relative lack of attention to the micro- processes by which identity in general and narrative identity in particular develop during this period. In the present article, our focus is particularly on the microprocess of conversational story- telling as a critical process by which narrative identity more broadly develops. Further, we show, across three studies, that having responsive, attentive friends as listeners for conversational storytelling helps further narrative identity development in late adolescence and early adulthood. Next, we outline the unaddressed issues in identity development work that can be better examined with the narrative and, more specifically, conversational storytell- ing frameworks. Identity Development in Adolescence and Early Adulthood Erikson and Erikson (1997) originally defined ego identity as entailing a sense of uniqueness or individuality, an emerging commitment to a place in society, and a sense of continuity over time. Much of the extant work on identity development, as well as work on self-development, has emphasized the uniqueness or individuality aspect of self and identity by focusing on explicit self-descriptive statements that participants do or do not en- dorse. Research in this arena has suggested that identity devel- opment in terms of exploring alternative identities and commit- ting to some rather than others is a nonlinear process that appears to occur primarily in late adolescence and early adult-
  • 156. hood (Constantinople, 1969; Meilman, 1979; Waterman, 1982). Over adolescence and early adulthood, self-descriptions be- come increasingly multifaceted and complex, and changes in the content of self-descriptions also occur into early adulthood (Dusek & McIntyre, 2003; Harter, 1998; Sutin & Robins, 2005). The ability to recognize contradictions in the self emerges by midadolescence, but the capacity for resolving them is a phe- nomenon of later adolescence (Harter & Monsour, 1992), con- sistent with increasing cognitive and epistemic abilities across this period (e.g., Kitchener, Lynch, Fischer, & Wood, 1993). Contextual factors such as parent– child relationships, peer re- lationships, and exposure to college and larger sociopolitical contexts are linked to identity development over this age range (Dusek & McIntyre, 2003; Hair, Moore, Garrett, Ling, & Cleve- land, 2008; McNulty & Swann, 1994; Waterman, 1982). This literature underscores the importance of late adolescence and early adulthood for identity development but is relatively Monisha Pasupathi, Department of Psychology, University of Utah; Timothy Hoyt, Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico. These studies were supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant 1R03MH64462 awarded to Monisha Pasupathi. Our gratitude is due to Benjamin Rich, for his extensive efforts in data collection, as well as to Martin Cryer and Leslie Rheinhold, who assisted with data coding. In addition, Carol Sansone, Kate McLean, and Frank Drews provided valu- able comments on earlier drafts of this article.
  • 157. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Monisha Pasupathi, Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 390 South 1530 East, Beh-502, Salt Lake City, UT 84112. E-mail: [email protected] Developmental Psychology © 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 45, No. 2, 558 –574 0012-1649/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014431 558 uninformative about the microprocesses by which identity is con- structed in that age period and does not capture the way in which people construct a sense of themselves as continuous across time. As an alternative, narrative approaches to identity development capture both individuality and the continuity aspects of identity, and their focus on the construction of identity is one step closer to an examination of the processes of identity formation. Narrative Identity in Late Adolescence and Early Adulthood: Closer to Process Narrative approaches to identity development examine how people construct meaning in relation to their experiences and thus further their sense of self and identity (McAdams, 1996). The construction of narratives about past experiences also involves the
  • 158. creation of a sense of continuity over time—as the past self is represented and interpreted by the present self. Thus, narrative conceptualizations of identity hold the promise of integrating Erikson and Erikson’s (1997) aspects of identity as well as illu- minating the processes by which individuals develop identity. For example, narratives construct personal continuity over time by linking past to the present and future. They also highlight individ- ual uniqueness and, at the same time, make use of cultural scripts and schemas for organizing one’s experience in narrative form. In fact, proponents of narrative identity research (McLean, Pasupathi, & Pals, 2007; Pasupathi, 2001; Thorne, 2000) have argued that the process of constructing narratives about the personal past is the paramount process by which narrative identity in particular, and self and identity more broadly, develop. In making this case, they specifically suggest that narrative identity develops via the micro- process of constructing specific narratives in specific situations. That is, narrative identity emerges out of countless actions of narrative construction. The study of narrative identity development consequently requires the exploration of processes that influence narrative identity creation in the moment. It is important to note that narrative and narrative identity are not synonymous. Although all stories must provide an account of what happened—the setting and actions that occurred— each sto-
  • 159. ryteller may have a unique sense of how the actions were con- nected, which were important, and what the broader implications and associations of the experience may entail. These interpretive, meaning-laden features of a narrative render a set of facts uniquely reflective of the individual whose story they compose, and it is these features that both reflect and construct narrative identity. Moreover, it is these features that across various operationaliza- tions are related to macrolevel development of self and identity in early childhood (e.g., Bird & Reese, 2006; Bohanek, Marin, & Fivush, 2006; Fivush, 1991; Fivush, Bohanek, Robertson, & Duke, 2004; Harley & Reese, 1999; Howe, Courage, & Peterson, 1994) and in adolescence and early adulthood (McLean & Pratt, 2006; see also Sutin & Robins, 2005), and to well-being and maturity throughout adulthood (King & Patterson, 2000; McAdams, Reyn- olds, Lewis, Patten, & Bowman, 2001; Pals, 2006a, 2006b). The overall elaboration of narratives, including both factual and interpretive information, increases across childhood (e.g., P. J. Bauer, 2006; Fivush & Nelson, 2004). Little is known about changes in general elaboration thereafter. Researchers focusing on adolescence and adulthood have emphasized broader, global types of meanings; these are conceptually connected to interpretive elaboration. The prevalence of those larger identity-related ele- ments in narratives increases with age into middle adulthood (J. J. Bauer & McAdams, 2004a, 2004b, 2005; Bluck & Glueck, 2004; McAdams et al., 2006; McLean, 2005; McLean & Thorne, 2003;
  • 160. Pasupathi & Mansour, 2006), and they become more positive and more nuanced (McAdams et al., 2006). This work suggests, but does not explicitly demonstrate, increases in the prevalence and sophistication of interpretive elaboration in narratives from ado- lescence into middle adulthood. Moreover, this work is largely mute about just how such macrolevel increases might come about; that is, it does not necessarily investigate the processes by which people come to vary in their construction of narrative identity. Conversational Storytelling: The Process of Constructing Narrative Identity Conversations about the personal past with caregivers are a major force in shaping young children’s ways of constructing narratives and developing narrative identity (see, e.g., P. J. Bauer, 2006; Fivush & Nelson, 2004). Some parents engage children in highly responsive, elaborative conversation about the past, whereas others are more focused and repetitive (Cleveland & Reese, 2005; Fivush & Nelson, 2004; Harley & Reese, 1999; Reese & Fivush, 1993; Wang, 2004). Children with more elabo- rative mothers subsequently engage in more elaborative remem- bering with other listeners and about other events. Maternal lis- tening, then, influences the story created within that conversation but over time exerts cumulative, long-term effects on children’s style of remembering. Converging findings from experimental (Peterson, Jesso, & McCabe, 1999) and prospective longitudinal designs (e.g., Haden, Haine, & Fivush, 1997) provide strong evidence for a causal role for parent– child remembering. For very young children like those studied in this work, parental respon- siveness is important for both factual and interpretive
  • 161. elaboration in narratives. Further, the development of basic capacities for narration and the development of narrative identity are tightly intertwined during early childhood. Within a burgeoning literature on narrative identity in late adolescence and early adulthood (e.g., McLean et al., 2007), however, we know surprisingly little about the role played by conversational storytelling and the listeners for such stories. This is in spite of the acknowledged importance of identity develop- ment in this age range and the fact that this period coincides with a shift from parents as the primary audience for personal story- telling to friends as an additional, increasingly important audience for such stories (McLean, 2005; see also Buhrmeister, 1996; Cooper, 1999; Daddis, 2008; Hartup, 1996; Hazan & Zeifman, 1994; Updegraff, McHale, & Whiteman, 2006). Some tiny pieces of evidence from studies of adolescents and emergent adults suggest that listeners are important in the con- struction of narrative identity in this age range. For example, conversational interactions with parents do matter. More open and free-ranging conversations with parents are associated with greater autonomy and identity development in adolescents (Cooper & Grotevant, 1985). By early adulthood, friends and parents appear to serve similar purposes as listeners for personal events, according to retrospective reports (McLean, 2005). Further, conversations with friends during adolescence are quite strikingly concerned with identity (Korobov & Bamberg, 2004; Parker & Gottman,
  • 162. 559NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT 1989), and work with late adolescents suggests that listeners prefer narratives that entail more sophisticated identity-related insights— indicative of a listener preference for more interpretive elaboration (Thorne, McLean, & Lawrence, 2004). Some work with young adults has linked the action of telling a personal story to changes in more traditionally assessed self-conceptions or event percep- tions and has shown that variations in the way listeners respond to that story, or variations in the goals with which the story is told, correspond to immediate self-perception changes (e.g., McGregor & Holmes, 1999; McLean, 2005; Pasupathi, Alderman, & Shaw, 2007; Pasupathi & Rich, 2005; Thoman, Sansone, & Pasupathi, 2007). A handful of studies have explicitly looked at variations in listener behavior and storytelling among young adults (Bavelas, Coates, & Johnson, 2000; Dickinson & Givon, 1995; Pasupathi, Stallworth, & Murdoch, 1998). These studies examined story- telling by young adults to a stranger and experimentally varied the stranger’s behavior during storytelling. That variation cre- ated either distracted or disagreeable listeners; the comparison conditions always involved a responsive, agreeable listener. Generally, responsive and agreeable listeners concurrently elicit more elaborative stories than do distracted or disagreeable listeners, whether about brief film stimuli (Dickinson & Givon, 1995; Pasupathi et al., 1998) or about personal experiences (Bavelas et al., 2000). In fact, listeners in the responsive and
  • 163. agreeable conditions behave more elaboratively than do listen- ers in the distracted conditions, in these studies. That is, they ask questions, make sympathetic exclamations, demonstrate that they have understood the speaker’s point, and follow the speaker’s interests rather than impose their own agenda. These behaviors are quite similar to those observed among more elaborative mothers in work on parent– child reminiscing. One goal of the present studies was to demonstrate that the development of narrative identity is affected by the process of storytelling, by showing that the extent to which late adoles- cents and early adults construct elaborative, richly detailed personal stories is connected to the behavior of their listening friends. But as we noted earlier, not all elaborations are equally relevant for narrative identity. Interpretive information in par- ticular makes memory into narrative identity by tying recalled experiences to the individual’s goals, feelings, thoughts, and beliefs. In childhood, when memory conversations involve learning how to remember, responsive listening is clearly linked to elaboration of all types of information, although for self- development the elaboration of interpretive or evaluative con- tent is more important. By contrast, in adulthood, responsive listening may be of particular importance for interpretive infor- mation. In fact, Bavelas et al. (2000) showed that distracted (and therefore unresponsive) listeners were selectively impaired in their capacity to respond to the meaning of what storytellers were saying, although they did not examine whether this cor- responded to a selective suppression of storytellers’ inclusion of such meanings. Thus, our second aim was to examine whether responsive friends were especially important in facilitating the construction of richly interpretive accounts of personal experi- ences, that is, in helping late adolescents infuse memory with narrative identity. Overview of the Present Studies
  • 164. The major goal of the present studies was to provide laboratory- based evidence for the microprocess. Critically, these studies were not designed to address the macrodevelopmental process of nar- rative identity process but, rather, to address the critical micropro- cess building block for narrative identity—the process of conver- sational storytelling. Other work suggests that if responsive listening has immediate importance for narrative identity in the short term, it will also have long-term implications for macrolevel self-development, an issue to which we return in the discussion of our findings. The present studies examine in vivo storytelling about personal experiences to close, same-gender friends by late adolescents and early adults (defined as the age range from 18 to 35 years). We chose to examine storytelling to same-gender close friends because narrative identity suggests that close friends and family are the primary audience for personal storytelling in adulthood and be- cause close friends and parents appear to serve similar roles as listeners among late adolescents and early adults (McLean, 2005). Restricting the focus to same-gender friendships allowed us to reduce the possible impact of different relationships between lis- teners and speakers (i.e., romantic vs. primarily platonic relations). We compared responsive and unresponsive (distracted) friends during storytelling across three studies, examining how variations
  • 165. in responsiveness influenced the elaboration of factual and inter- pretive aspects of storytelling. We chose experimental methods because exploratory work in our laboratory suggested that under normal conditions, most friends are fairly responsive listeners in a laboratory setting in which there are few distractions and their defined task is to listen. Experimentally creating an unresponsive listener via distraction ensured that some of our participants would not function like highly elaborative mothers by asking questions or expressing strong interest. Experimental methods also permit stronger con- clusions about causality and link the present studies to work on young adults. In addition to examining our major goals, we also examined two other issues in the present studies. First, although age differences, particularly in this restricted age range, were not a primary focus of our study, we examined whether age was associated with increases in the elaboration of factual or interpretive information, given other findings on autobiographical memory development during this age period. Second, by late adolescence and beyond, people also have the capacity to engage in deliberate, strategic self-presentation (e.g., Baumeister, Stilman, & Wotman, 1990; Tice, 1992). Such deliberate self-presentation would suggest that people strategically change their stories, and strategic changes in narration imply a different type of identity-related implication than do changes in narrative identity construction that occur without a
  • 166. great deal of self-awareness on the part of the individual. Study 1 Study 1 provides a first examination of how late adolescents and young adults construct narrative identity around positive experi- ences, in collaboration with listeners who vary in their responsive- ness. We expected that late adolescents and emergent adults talk- ing with distracted friends would construct less elaborated stories 560 PASUPATHI AND HOYT in general but would be especially likely to construct less inter- pretive accounts. Method Participants Participants were 40 same-gender pairs of friends (50% men, 50% women) recruited from the introductory psychology subject pool at the University of Utah. To recruit pairs of friends, we first recruited target participants (assigned to the speaker role), and these participants were asked to bring a close, same-gender friend to the experiment. The average age of speakers was 21.6 years (SD � 3.2). Participating friends had known each other an average of 3.2 years (SD � 3.8).1 The majority of participants were
  • 167. European American (90%). Pairs were randomly assigned to an attentive or distracted listener condition; conditions did not differ in the length of time friends had known one another or in ratings of friendship quality. Equipment failure caused loss of transcripts in two cases. Sample sizes for analyses reported below vary accordingly. Procedure After arriving, participants completed several measures, includ- ing personality, relationship quality, and background measures. Speakers identified recent undisclosed events they considered to be positive, selected one event to describe to the friend, and provided some preliminary ratings of the events. Meanwhile, friends were separated from speakers for instruction. Attentive friends were told, “Listen to your friend the way you typically do when you’re being a good listener.” Distracted friends were told, “We are interested in how conversations go when one person is distracted.” They were asked to count all words beginning with the letters th while listening to the speaker and practiced this task on 10 prepared stories read aloud by the experimenter, who provided feedback about task accuracy after each story. Listeners were offered an incentive of $5 if they were accurate in counting words beginning with th, with accuracy defined as coming within a 4-point margin of error during the subsequent conversation with the speaker. This target was chosen on the basis of listener per- formance in the training session, because listeners were rarely
  • 168. especially accurate but achieved this standard of performance at a rate of about 50% of the time. Pilot work revealed that participants proved to have difficulty ignoring their friend’s storytelling in favor of the th task, and the incentive improved their level of distraction. The dyad was then reunited and videotaped while the speaker described the chosen experience to the listener. Following the conversation, the speaker completed additional ratings of lis- tener responsiveness and agreeableness, and rated the extent to which the story was detailed and coherently told. Participants were fully debriefed and given information about both listener condi- tions, and were told about our interest in how listeners influence storytelling. Measures Manipulation check. As a manipulation check, we obtained speaker ratings of the extent to which the listener agreed with his or her version of events (a single item) and of the listener’s responsiveness (an average across five items, including, for exam- ple, “The listener was very responsive”; Cronbach’s � � .84). Agreement and responsiveness often covary, but they are concep- tually distinct (e.g., Pasupathi, Carstensen, Levenson, & Gottman, 1999). Both were assessed because manipulations of listener re- sponsiveness sometimes influence both.
  • 169. Speaker’s perceptions of story quality. To assess deliberate changes in storytelling, we obtained speakers’ ratings of the qual- ity of the story they told across nine items, using 7-point Likert- type scales in which 1 indicated lower quality and 7 higher quality. Example items include “My story used few details/many details” and “My story was very incoherent/very coherent.” These nine items were averaged to create a single index of story quality (Cronbach’s � � .90). Coding of conversational data. Each recorded conversation was transcribed by undergraduate research assistants. Transcripts were then divided into “idea units” that roughly corresponded to verb phrases. Idea units were then coded according to their con- tent. We examined two main types of narrative content. The first main category was event factual information, that is, information available to the perceptual capabilities of any bystander. Examples of this category include such as statements “Mom and I had a talk while you were gone,” “We talked for almost two hours,” and “She started crying and everything.” The second category was interpre- tive information, or information that emphasizes the subjective, meaning-laden, and internal aspects of experience—in short, nar- rative identity. Examples of this category include “It was just a good talk,” “She’s always sappy like that,” “We have become closer and everything,” and “It makes it feel like things have
  • 170. really changed since I left for school.” Additional categories included statements aimed at regulating the conversation (establishing un- derstanding, providing minimal back channels, etc.), off-topic statements (references to the experimental setting), and uncodable statements. The Appendix presents an entire conversation, in units and coded. To establish reliability between coders, the two authors first independently coded several conversations. These independently coded sets were then compared by means of Cohen’s kappa. Once the authors established substantial independent agreement (� � .91) and resolved remaining discrepancies through discussion, the second author then trained a team of two additional undergraduate coders. After learning the coding scheme, the coding team applied the coding scheme to the 38 conversations in the study. The coding team held weekly meetings in which problematic coding units were discussed and agreement on content was sought. Fifteen percent of the conversation pairs were coded independently by two coders to quantify reliability across the full set of content catego- ries in the coding scheme. Across 373 idea units, coders agreed 77% of the time on the content category (� � .68). For each pair, we computed the number of speaker-generated facts and evalua- tions. These content code variables served as the primary depen-
  • 171. dent measures for our analyses. 1 The length of time partners had known one another was unrelated to any of the results reported here; nor did relationship quality have any impact on any of the findings. 561NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT Results Manipulation Check A general linear model examining the effects of experimental condition and gender on perceived listener attentiveness and agree- ableness revealed only an overall effect of condition, F(2, 37) � 26.8, p � .01, �p 2 � .59. Univariate tests showed that condition effects were evident for perceptions of listener attentiveness, F(1, 38) � 6.3, p � .02, �p 2 � .14, and for perceptions of listener agreeableness, F(1, 38) � 54.9, p � .01, �p 2 � .59. Speakers perceived distracted listeners to be less responsive (attentive: M � 5.4, SD � 1.0; distracted: M � 3.1, SD � 1.0) and less agreeable (attentive: M � 6.1, SD � 1.4; distracted: M � 4.9, SD � 1.7).
  • 172. Impact of Listener Responsiveness on Amount and Type of Information Recalled We expected that when faced with distracted listeners, our partic- ipants would construct shorter stories in general, and especially for interpretive, as opposed to factual, content. To test this hypothesis, we computed a general linear model (multivariate analysis of variance) with the number of speaker-generated facts and evaluations as depen- dent variables, listener condition and gender as between- subjects variables, and type of information (facts or evaluations) as a within- subjects variable. The results revealed significant main effects of condition, F(1, 34) � 5.2, p � .03, �p 2 � .13, and type of information, F(1, 34) � 6.0, p � .01, �p 2 � .15. We also computed simple effects tests examining the effect of listener condition within each type of information separately. These contrasts revealed that distracted listen- ers elicited significantly fewer facts, F(1, 34) � 5.2, p � .03, �p 2 � .14, and tended to elicit fewer interpretations, F(1, 34) � 2.9, p � .10,
  • 173. �p 2 � .08 (see Table 1). These effects are shown in Figure 1. As seen here, factual information predominated. Distracted listeners elicit less information overall, and given the absence of a significant interaction between information type and listener condition, the information decrement appears consistent across information type, contrary to our hypotheses. Are Speakers Aware of the Impact of Listeners on Their Stories? To evaluate whether participants were aware of the impact of listeners on their stories, we computed an analysis of variance examining perceived story quality as a function of listener condi- tion and gender. There were no differences in self-reported story quality as a function of listener condition, F(1, 36) � 1, ps � .40 (attentive: M � 3.8, SD � 1.4; distracted: M � 3.8, SD � 1.0). There were also no significant effects involving gender. Cross-Sectional Age Differences in Story Content To examine age differences in the content of participants’ sto- ries, we computed a partial correlation between age and factual and interpretive units, controlling for the perceived attentiveness of listeners. Older participants tended to report more facts than younger participants, r(35) � .31, p � .07, but they did not
  • 174. differ significantly in their inclusion of interpretive information, r(35) � .18, p � .20. These correlations are not significantly different from each other, however, and with greater power both types of infor- mation may in fact increase with age. Study 2 The results of Study 1 suggested that (a) late adolescents and emergent adults constructed less rich and elaborated stories when they spoke to distracted friends; (b) these same participants’ rat- ings of their own stories did not differ as a function of their friends’ behavior; and (c) across even this restricted age range and with limited power, there were possible age differences in the elaboration of factual information in conversational narration. Our initial expectation, that distracted listeners would be especially problematic in relation to interpretive content, was not confirmed. However, we had relatively little statistical power given our small sample size and, in particular, given that the stories told in this study had more factual than interpretive content. Further, we had examined only positive events in this study, whereas earlier work on narrative identity emphasized the potential for negative events to involve self-
  • 175. construction in childhood and, to some extent, in adulthood (Bird & Reese, 2006; Thorne et al., 2004). Thus, in Study 2, we expanded our focus to examine both positive and negative events. Participant pairs were randomly assigned to a positive or negative event condition. Moreover, the addition of a negative event condition also increased our statistical power for examining age differences. Aside from this change, Study 2 was an exact replication of Study 1. Method Participants Participants in this study were 81 same-gender friend dyads (50% men, 50% women) recruited from the introductory psychol- ogy subject pool and through flyers posted at the University of Utah. The average age of speakers was 19.6 years (SD � 1.9), again, firmly within the age range of late adolescence and early adulthood. Within dyads, participants had known each other an average of 4.7 years (SD � 6.0).2 One pair was excluded because the speaker was not a native speaker of English, leaving a total sample of 80 pairs. The sample predominantly comprised Euro- pean American participants (76.5%), with Hispanic participants composing an additional 12.3% of the sample. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two event-valence conditions (posi- tive or negative) and to one of two listener conditions (attentive or distracted). As in Study 1, there were no differences by condition
  • 176. in the length of time participants had known one another or in their ratings of relationship quality. 2 The length of time that partners had known one another and ratings of the quality of their relationship did not influence the results presented below. Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations of Dependent Measures in Study 1 Measure Attentive Distracted M SD M SD Facts 40.1 31.6 21.0 15.0 Evaluations 29.1 23.8 17.7 15.1 562 PASUPATHI AND HOYT Procedure The procedure was identical to that in Study 1, except that half the participants assigned to the speaker role were asked to identify recent, undisclosed negative events, and the other half were asked to identify recent, undisclosed positive events. Measures
  • 177. Manipulation check. Speakers rated their perceptions of the listeners’ responsiveness across the same five items used in Study 1 (Cronbach’s � � .83) and rated their perception of the listeners’ agreement with their views on a 7-point Likert-type scale, again, as in Study 1. Speaker’s perceptions of story quality. Speakers rated their perceptions of story quality along the same nine items employed in Study 1, and we averaged the items to create an index of perceived story quality. The internal consistency of this measure was lower in this study than in Study 1 (Cronbach’s � � .68). Coding of conversations. Coding employed the same system as in Study 1. Twenty-five percent (n � 20) of the conversation pairs were coded independently to quantify reliability; coders agreed on about 82% of 1,169 idea units (� � .75). As in Study 1, we computed the number of speaker-generated event facts and evaluations as primary dependent measures. Results Manipulation Check A general linear model testing for effects of listener condition (attentive vs. distracted), event valence (positive or negative), and gender on the speaker’s perceptions of the agreeableness and
  • 178. responsiveness of the listener revealed a main effect of listener condition, F(2, 72) � 21.4, p � .01, �p 2 � .37, and a main effect of gender, F(2, 72) � 4.3, p � .02, �p 2 � .11. Examination of the univariate effects for these two main effects suggested that listener condition influenced perceptions of listener responsiveness, F(1, 73) � 42.9, p � .01, �p 2 � .37, but not perceptions of listener agreement, F(1, 73) � 1.2, p � .20. As expected, listeners in the distracted condition were perceived as less responsive (M � 3.6, SD � 1.2) than listeners in the responsive condition (M � 5.3, SD � 1.2). Gender was also related to perceptions of listener responsiveness, F(1, 73) � 4.8, p � .05, �p 2 � .06, but not to perceptions of listener agreement, F(1, 73) � 1.8, p � .15. Across conditions, women rated their listeners (who were also women) as more attentive (M � 4.7, SD � 1.6) than did men (M � 4.2, SD � 1.3). Our manipulation of listener behavior was successful, as perceived by the speakers, but had a more narrowly focused impact on responsiveness than in Study 1. Note that even testing for effects of listener condition on agreeableness within the positive event condition did not result in perceptions that listeners differed
  • 179. in agreeableness. Differences in attentiveness by gender did not interact with the manipulation. Do Speakers Change Their Story Depending on Listener Condition and Event Valence? We conducted a general linear model with information type (factual, interpretive) as a within-subjects factor and gender, lis- tener condition, and event valence as between-subjects factors. There were main effects of type of information, F(1, 72) � 4.4, p � .05, �p 2 � .06, and gender, F(1, 72) � 4.0, p � .05, �p 2 � .05, 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45
  • 180. 50 snoitaterpretnIstcaF Type of Information N um be r of Id ea U ni ts Attentive Distracted Figure 1. Effect of distracted versus attentive friends on information during conversational remembering in Study 1. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. 563NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT and a trend toward a main effect of listener condition, F(1, 72) � 3.8, p � .06, �p
  • 181. 2 � .05. In addition, there was an interaction of listener condition and type of information, F(1, 72) � 5.6, p � .03, �p 2 � .07. In contrast to the results of Study 1, conversations focused more on interpretive information (M � 33.8, SD � 48.8) than on facts (M � 23.9, SD � 23.4), and women produced a greater number of units on average, across all types of information (M � 71.6, SD � 85.1) than did men (M � 43.1, SD � 23.2), which is consistent with the fact that women in this study rated their listeners as more attentive than did men across all conditions. Attentive friends tended to elicit longer accounts (M � 72.0, SD � 85.2) than did distracted listeners (M � 43.4, SD � 26.4), but this effect was qualified by an interaction with information type. To follow up the interaction effect, we tested the simple effect of listener condition within each type of information. Only one of these simple effects tests was significant: that for interpretive information, F(1, 72) � 5.3, p � .03, �p 2 � .07. Participants told attentive friends nearly twice as many interpretive units (M � 45.7, SD � 64.9) than they told to distracted listeners (M � 21.1, SD � 16.9). Figure 2 displays the amount of information contained in participants’ stories as a function of information type and listener condition. Thus, there were no significant effects of listeners on factual
  • 182. content and no effects of valence on memory content. See Table 2. Are Speakers Aware of the Impact of Listeners on Their Stories? As in Study 1, we examined whether speakers’ perception of the quality of their story differed significantly as a function of listener condition, event valence, or gender. There were no significant main effects or interactions, and speakers in the two listener conditions rated their stories quite similarly (attentive: M � 4.3, SD � 1.3; distracted: M � 4.0, SD � 1.0). Cross-Sectional Age Differences in Story Content As in Study 1, we computed a partial correlation between age and factual and interpretive information scores, controlling for perceptions of listener attentiveness and this time, based on the results above, for gender. Age was again significantly and posi- tively correlated with the inclusion of factual information, r(76) � .23, p � .05, and was not significantly correlated with the inclu- sion of interpretive information, r(76) � �.04, p � .50. To summarize, the results of Study 2 showed that (a) late adolescents and emergent adults told significantly less interpretive stories to distracted friends than to attentive friends, across both positive and negative events; (b) these same participants were not aware of the changes in the stories they told; and (c) once again, older participants, compared with younger participants, told stories
  • 183. that involved more factual content. We also observed main effects of gender on both perceptions of listener responsiveness and the overall length of stories. These main effects were quite congruent: Women in this study perceived their listeners as more responsive and produced longer stories. In contrast to the results of Study 1, participants did not reduce the factual content of their stories when talking to distracted friends. Notably, distracted friends in this study were perceived as less responsive but as equally agreeable. It is possible that when people perceive their listener to disagree, they repeat and otherwise emphasize interpretive content to persuade or convince listeners to accept their perspective. Thus, in Study 1, listeners’ unresponsive- ness and disagreeableness may have had opposing effects on interpretive information, diminishing the impact of responsiveness 0 10 20 30 40 50
  • 184. 60 snoitaterpretnIstcaF Type of Information N um be r of Id ea U ni ts Attentive Distracted Figure 2. Effect of distracted versus attentive friends on information during conversational remembering in Study 2. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. 564 PASUPATHI AND HOYT on this type of information. Notably, earlier work on film clips, in which participants perceived a listening stranger as both distracted
  • 185. and disagreeable, obtained findings similar to those of Study 1 (Pasupathi et al., 1998)—significant decrements in both factual and interpretive information. Work on children’s personal storytelling typically examines naturally occurring variability in maternal responsiveness and elaborativeness; elaborative mothers usually combine warm, sup- portive, and generally agreeable attitudes with high levels of attentiveness to the child’s story. Listening in adulthood likewise combines responsiveness with warmth and agreeableness under most circumstances but not all (see, e.g., Pasupathi et al., 1999). Ideally, the ability to disentangle responsiveness and agreement experimentally would permit us to explore more directly the im- plications of responsiveness, as distinct from agreement, for the types of information participants include when constructing a personal story in conversations. Study 3 was designed to attempt to disentangle the implications of agreement from listeners and re- sponsiveness from listeners for the construction of narrative iden- tity and to do so in an experimentally controlled manner. This disentangling was motivated by the desire to reconcile the con- flicting findings from Study 1 and Study 2. In addition, however, responsiveness combined with disagreement may have some of its own particular developmental implications, perhaps especially in the context of the dynamics of individuation that are so important for adolescents and young adults. Thus, although the primary purpose of our project was to examine variations in listener re- sponsiveness, variations in listener agreement with the speaker
  • 186. may be relevant for the development of narrative identity in a distinctive way, and Study 3 also provides preliminary evidence of that. Study 3 The major task in Study 3 was to find a way to examine how unresponsive and disagreeable listening differentially influence the construction of identity in conversational narratives. One way to approach this was to borrow from experimental social psychology and to create an experience in our laboratory that permitted some conversational narration and that was at least potentially relevant to narrative identity construction but also somewhat standardized across participants. We did this by asking participants to try a computer game called Sims (Electronic Arts, Redwood City, CA) for the first time. First-time activities, though not of great impor- tance in an individual’s life, offer opportunities for thinking about new aspects of one’s identity and for construing novel situations in terms of enduring identities. The Sims game was also ideal for our purposes because it entails being a simulated individual. Players must “live” in the game by taking care of basic needs such as eating, bathing, and toileting, as well as more elaborate needs for income via work, social
  • 187. interac- tion, and self-enhancement. Needs, indicated via bars on the screen, are addressed by choosing to take particular actions (e.g., clean the apartment, call a friend). Unpredictable, random events such as fires, burglaries, and loss of a job occur and must be dealt with by the player. The game has relatively high appeal for women, in contrast to many computer games, and permits some degree of identity investment on the part of players. Participants all played the game for the first time, and were then asked to talk about this experience with a same-gender friend, following procedures similar to those employed in Studies 1 and 2. However, because pilot work showed that such conversations nearly always revolved around evaluating whether the game was fun, we were able to create a condition in which listeners could disagree with speakers’ views of the game. This was done by asking players to evaluate the game just after playing. The players’ evaluation was then employed to instruct their friends to convince them of the opposite view: If players thought the game was fun, as most participants did, then friends were asked to express the view that the game was boring. This procedure raised three methodological issues. First, are descriptions of playing a computer game for the first time analo- gous to stories about real personal experiences? Second, in those descriptions, how does one address information about how the
  • 188. game is played, which is neither factual nor, in the usual sense of our coding scheme, interpretive? And third, does disagreement about the evaluation of the game change the storytelling task to one involving persuasion in ways that would undermine compar- isons with the previous two studies? With respect to the first issue, first-time experiences with an activity, of which the present experiment can be seen as an example, are in fact among the personal experiences that we frequently disclose to others. Recently collected data in our labo- ratory showed that of 59 people asked to report on a recent (within the last 2 weeks) novel experience, 53 had already told others about that event, and across different types of events (self- discrepant, self-typical, repeated, and novel), novel events were the most likely to have been disclosed to others. So storytelling about new experiences is an ecologically valid subcategory of the more general category of personal storytelling. Moreover, it is likely that storytelling about first-time experiences is a very im- portant aspect of the construction of narrative identity, in the sense that such storytelling may revolve around the potential role that the Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations of Dependent Measures in Study 2 Measure Attentive Distracted Positive events Negative events Positive events Negative events
  • 189. M SD M SD M SD M SD Facts 26.3 23.0 24.8 26.0 20.5 19.2 24.2 14.1 Evaluations 44.0 52.6 49.0 76.5 22.6 17.3 19.5 16.8 565NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT new experience or activity may or may not play in a person’s life (see Thoman et al., 2007). To address the second issue, we expanded our coding scheme to include a category called schematic inferences. This category included idea units that referred to information about how the game could be played, such as the rules, goals, and opportunities within the game. Examples of this category included such state- ments as “You gotta spend all this time beefing up your dude,” “You’re just watching her go through her daily routine,” and “You have to make friends.” Such information was not, strictly speaking, factual in the sense of reporting on what happened during game play. Like interpretive information, it required inferences based on the participants’ experience, but unlike interpretive information, those inferences were about how to succeed at the game, rather than about the broader category of interpretations related to the participants’ experiences and evaluations. As a consequence, we were able to examine how variations in listener behavior influ- enced three types of information: factual details about events that occurred during the participants’ game experience, schematic
  • 190. in- ferences about how the game works, and interpretive information about the overall game experience, including the extent to which the game was fun or interesting to play. Note that schematic information is also an aspect of personal remembering that has been extensively examined in early childhood, and when young children narrate novel experiences, they often focus extensively on this type of information (e.g., P. J. Bauer, 2006). Thus, this addition also permitted Study 3 to explore the construction of schematic knowledge in the context of responsive and unrespon- sive listening in a sample of early adults. The third issue, which is whether disagreement about the eval- uation of the game changes a storytelling task to a persuasion task, is crucial. However, most remembering to friends or family re- volves around making some type of interpretive claim and backing it up with selected facts (Hyman, 1994). In other words, the storytelling in Studies 1 and 2 also reflected attempts to tell a story that made one or another point, but the specific points at stake were heterogeneous given the varied events participants chose. Method Participants Participants in this study were 104 same-gender friend dyads (49% men, 51% women) recruited from the introductory psychol- ogy subject pool at the University of Utah. The average age of
  • 191. speakers was 20.8 years (SD � 3.4), again, consistent with the targeted age range. Within dyads, participants had known one another an average of 4.6 years (SD � 5.5).3 This sample predom- inantly comprised European American participants (88.5%). Par- ticipants were randomly assigned to one of five conditions: atten- tive agreeable (n � 20), attentive disagreeable (n � 22), distracted (n � 21), distracted agreeable (n � 22), and distracted disagree- able (n � 20). On the basis of initial analyses of our manipulation check measures, we collapsed these categories into attentive– agreeable (n � 20), attentive– disagreeable (n � 22), and dis- tracted (n � 63) conditions. Procedure Upon arrival, participants were assigned to speaker and listener roles. As in previous studies, the pairs were separated to complete background measures not included in the present study. In contrast to what participants did in previous studies, rather than generate a list of recent experiences, speakers played the Sims game for 15 min. The experimenter first explained the video game and pro- vided brief instructions. All participants began the game at an identical starting condition, in terms of simulated money, skills, and status; women played a female character and men a male character. After playing the game, participants completed ques- tionnaires assessing interest in the game. During this game play time, listeners were given separate in-
  • 192. structions. Agreeable and attentive friends and distracted listeners were given instructions as in Studies 1 and 2. Disagreeable and attentive friends were told that the speaker liked or disliked the game, according to the speaker’s ratings of the game postplay, and were asked to convince the speaker of the opposite evaluation. Distracted–agreeable and distracted– disagreeable listeners were asked to practice either mild confirmatory responses (e.g., “yeah”) or mild disconfirming responses (e.g., “hmm”) while engaging in the same practice of counting th words as the distracted listeners. Measures Manipulation check. As in Studies 1 and 2, speakers rated listeners’ responsiveness along the same 7-point Likert-type items indexing indicators of listener responsiveness (Cronbach’s � � .82). Because agreement from listeners was being manipulated in this study, we added two items assessing listener concordance (“Your friend supported your opinion of Sims” and “Your friend agreed with your view of Sims”), again rated on 7-point Likert- type scales in which low ratings indicated no concordance and high ratings indicated higher concordance. These items were av- eraged to provide a measure of perceived listener concordance (Cronbach’s � � .84). Speaker’s perceptions of story quality. The same nine items used in Studies 1 and 2 were averaged to create an assessment of overall story quality (Cronbach’s � � .87).
  • 193. Coding of transcripts. Each transcription of a conversation was transcribed and divided into idea units by undergraduate research assistants. These idea units were coded for content through the same coding scheme used in Studies 1 and 2, with the addition of the schematic inference category. Weekly meetings were held in which problematic coding units were discussed and agreement on content was sought. Twenty percent of the conver- sation pairs (n � 20) were coded independently to quantify reli- ability. Intercoder reliability for Study 3 across the detailed coding scheme was good (83% agreement; � � .79). Results Manipulation Check and Preliminary Analyses As noted, participants were assigned into three conditions: an attentive and agreeable condition (n � 20 pairs), an attentive but disagreeable condition (n � 22 pairs), and a distracted condition (n � 63 pairs). Reanalyzing the manipulation checks revealed that the two attentive conditions differed from the distracted condition 3 Again, the amount of time known and the quality of the relationships did not affect the results presented for this study. 566 PASUPATHI AND HOYT on attentiveness alone, that the distracted and disagreeable condi-
  • 194. tions together differed from the attentive–agreeable condition on agreement alone. In other words, the distracted condition in this study, as in Study 1, is better described as a distracted– disagreeable condition. Although the full disentangling of agree- ableness and attentiveness that we attempted was not achieved, the present study did succeed in providing a partial disentangling of agreement and attentiveness. Specifically, the disagreeable condi- tion combined disagreement with high responsiveness, whereas the distracted condition reflected perceptions of disagreement with low responsiveness. The attentive and agreeable condition resulted in listeners perceived as highly responsive and highly agreeable. Do Speakers Change Their Story Depending on Listener Behavior? We conducted a general linear model with information type (factual, game schematic, and interpretive) as a within-subjects factor and listener condition (attentive–agreeable, attentive– disagreeable, and distracted) and gender as between-subjects fac- tors. The results suggested a significant main effect of condition, F(2, 90) � 8.4, p � .01, �p 2 � .16, as well as interactions involving listener condition and type of information, F(4, 180) � 5.7, p � .01, �p 2 � .11, and listener condition, type of information, and gender, F(4, 180) � 2.8, p � .03, �p
  • 195. 2 � .06. The main effect of listener condition was reflected by the fact that across all types of information, participants constructed stories with more information of all types when they had an attentive– agreeable (M � 31.3, SEM � 3.6) or attentive– disagreeable (M � 31.0, SEM � 3.6) listener, in contrast to stories constructed for a distracted– disagreeable friend (M � 17.7, SEM � 2.1). The two attentive conditions did not differ from each other. The Condi- tion � Type of Information interaction, however, suggested that this difference was not consistent across the different types of information. Post hoc comparisons examining the effect of listener condition separately for each type of information suggested that listener condition significantly affected all three types of information, 3.4 � F(2, 90) � 13.5, ps � .05, �p 2 � .07. For facts about the game, participants constructed more elaborated stories for attentive–agreeable friends (M � 45.1, SEM � 6.9), compared with attentive– disagreeable friends (M � 22.3, SEM � 6.9) and distracted– disagreeable friends (M � 26.5, SEM � 4.0), who had similar effects. Differences between the attentive–agreeable con- dition and the other two conditions were marginally significant ( ps � .07) based on pairwise comparisons. For schematic inferences about the game, participants con- structed stories with more schematic inferences for attentive–
  • 196. disagreeable listeners (M � 35.7, SEM � 3.8), followed by attentive–agreeable listeners (M � 23.9, SEM � 3.8) and distracted– disagreeable listeners (M � 14.8, SEM � 2.2). Pairwise comparisons showed that the difference between the attentive– disagreeable and distracted– disagreeable condition was significant ( p � .01), whereas the difference between the attentive– agreeable and attentive– disagreeable condition was marginal ( p � .09), and the difference between the attentive–agreeable and distracted– disagreeable condition was not significant ( p � .12). Interpretive information followed a highly similar pattern, with attentive– disagreeable listeners eliciting the highest number of such statements (M � 34.5, SEM � 4.0), followed by attentive– agreeable listeners (M � 25.1, SEM � 4.0) and distracted listeners (M � 11.8, SEM � 2.3). In this case, pairwise comparisons showed no significant differences between the attentive– agreeable and attentive– disagreeable conditions ( p � .27), but the distracted condition differed significantly from both of the other conditions ( ps � .02). Finally, to explore the three-way interaction with gender, we again examined the effect of listener condition separately for each type of information, but this time we also did this separately for men and women. The resulting contrasts showed effects of con- dition on schematic information for men, F(2, 90) � 3.8, p � .03,
  • 197. �p 2 � .08, and women, F(2, 90) � 8.6, p � .01, �p 2 � .16, and on interpretive information for both men, F(2, 90) � 5.2, p � .01, �p 2 � .10, and women, F(2, 90) � 10.3, p � .01, �p 2 � .19. However, for factual information, whereas listener condition had a signifi- cant impact on men, F(2, 90) � 5.1, p � .01, �p 2 � .10, there was no comparable effect for women, F(2, 90) � 1. Pairwise compar- isons of men and women across all other cells of the design revealed one marginal difference and one significant difference between men and women. Both differences were evident in the attentive condition. Men in the attentive condition tended to pro- duce more factual accounts than women ( p � .07) and produced significantly less interpretive accounts ( p � .02). This difference in the baseline, in which women produced relatively nonfactual accounts, is the likely reason that listener condition did not sig- nificantly influence factual content for women. These results are shown in Figure 3. Table 3 presents means and standard deviations for all types of information by gender and listener condition. Are Speakers Aware of the Impact of Listeners on Their Stories?
  • 198. As in Study 1, speakers rated their stories along the same indicators. An analysis of variance of overall story quality as a function of gender and listener condition revealed no significant differences in perceived story quality as a function of listener condition, F(2, 96) � 2.6, p � .08, �p 2 � .05. There were also no effects involving gender. Notably, this effect is a trend. Examina- tion of the estimated marginal means suggests that the overall trend is due to the fact that stories told to attentive and agreeable listeners were rated more highly (M � 4.4, SEM � 0.2) than stories told to distracted listeners (M � 3.8, SEM � 0.1). Exam- ining the items that make up the composite story quality rating suggested that only a single item was rated differently by partic- ipants with distracted listeners. Specifically, stories told to distracted– disagreeable listeners were rated as significantly less emotional than stories told to attentive–agreeable listeners, al- though not differently from stories told to attentive– disagreeable friends. In sum, as in Studies 1 and 2, participants did not report awareness of telling different stories to friends whose listening behavior differed. Cross-Sectional Age Differences in Story Content As in Studies 1 and 2, we computed a partial correlation be- tween age and factual, interpretive, and schematic units, control- ling for perceptions of listener attentiveness and gender based on
  • 199. the analyses above. Again, age was positively and significantly 567NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT correlated with factual content, r(92) � .30, p � .05, but not with interpretive content, r(92) � .03, p � .70, or schematic content, r(92) � �.07, p � .40. General Discussion The present studies demonstrate that the development of narra- tive identity during late adolescence and early adulthood, in the microcontext of conversational storytelling, is shaped by the re- sponsiveness, and perhaps the agreeableness, of listening friends. Specifically, by early adulthood, responsive listening is critical for the inclusion of interpretive information in storytelling. Given that interpretive information is important for long-term effects on self and identity assessed in a variety of ways (Bird & Reese, 2006; McLean & Pratt, 2006; Pasupathi, 2007), the findings both confirm the role of relationships in identity development and point to a process by which relationships influence identity development. In Study 3, the effects of distracted listening were similar for interpretive information and schematic inferences. The literature on the early childhood emergence of autobiographical remember-
  • 200. ing has addressed not only the emergence of self (e.g., Bird & Reese, 2006) but also the emergence of scripts and schemas (e.g., P. J. Bauer & Fivush, 1992). Conversations about a novel activity involve both scripts and interpretive content, and faced with un- responsive friends, our late adolescent participants were impaired in both. Tentatively, this suggests that distracted listeners more broadly suppress meanings, both those that revolve around “what I’m like” and those that refer to “how things get done.” Because both of these kinds of meanings are important for developmental trajectories, this finding warrants further investigation across a broader range of age groups, listeners, and activities. The role of responsiveness on the construction of narrative identity in the moment was distinguishable from agreement, which was related to whether participants constructed elaborated factual narratives. This has some important developmental implications to which we return below. Finally, these effects of listeners were not reducible to deliberate self-presentation, as our participants were 0 10 20 30
  • 201. 40 50 60 70 80 Facts Schematic Interpretations Facts Schematic Interpretations Males Females N um be r of Id ea U ni ts Attentive/Agreeable Attentive/Disagreeable Distracted Figure 3. Effect of distracted versus attentive and attentive–
  • 202. disagreeable listeners on information during conversational remembering in Study 3. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. Table 3 Means and Standard Deviations of Dependent Measures in Study 3 Measure Attentive Disagreeable Distracted Men Women Men Women Men Women M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD Facts 57.7 50.0 32.4 22.8 19.4 15.8 25.2 26.5 25.8 29.8 27.2 28.4 Schematic inferences 21.7 15.8 26.0 12.6 33.0 17.2 38.4 27.1 16.3 17.6 13.3 11.4 Evaluations 15.3 15.6 34.9 26.5 32.3 23.0 36.8 32.3 11.6 9.7 12.0 11.8 Note. Gender is included because this study revealed differences between male and female conversations. 568 PASUPATHI AND HOYT not aware of constructing different kinds of stories for different listeners. Finally, consistent with the idea that this age period is a privileged one for autobiographical encoding and recall (Holmes & Conway, 1999; Rubin & Schulkind, 1997), we found consis-
  • 203. tently small but reliable age-related increases in the factual content of memories. Below we consider these findings in the context of their impli- cations for narrative approaches to the development of memory and identity (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Fivush & Nelson, 2004; Hooker & McAdams, 2003; McLean et al., 2007; Pasupathi, Mansour, & Brubaker, 2007; Pasupathi & Rich, 2005). Through- out, we discuss specific limitations in the areas where they are relevant to our interpretations. Developing Narrative Identity in Late Adolescence and Early Adulthood Despite the ubiquity of conversational remembering (e.g., Bohanek et al., 2006; Rimé, Finkenauer, Luminet, Zech, & Philli- pot, 1998), previous empirical work on narrative identity in emer- gent adulthood has focused primarily on written or interview- based narratives, in which interviewers strive to avoid influencing speakers. Although narrative identity work clearly acknowledges the constructive nature of memory that has been part of cognitive work for decades (Bartlett, 1932/1995; Conway & Pleydell- Pearce, 2000; Schacter, 1996), extant research emphasizes how storytellers’ goals shape their remembering, ranging from endur- ing, characteristic motivations of rememberers (McAdams, Hoff- man, Mansfield, & Day, 1996; Moffitt & Singer, 1994; Woike,
  • 204. 1995; Woike, Gershkovich, Piorkowski, & Polo, 1999) to exper- imentally induced motivations (Dudukovic, Marsh, & Tversky, 2004; McGregor & Holmes, 1999; Sanitioso, Kunda, & Fong, 1990; Tversky & Marsh, 2000). Although the existing sense of self or personality that a storyteller brings to narration is important and clearly influences subsequent remembering for adults (viz. McLean, 2005; Pasupathi, Alderman, et al., 2007; Sutin & Robins, 2005) and young children (Bird & Reese, 2006; Reese, Haden, & Fivush, 1993), the role of listeners has largely been ignored for emergent adults. Previous work examining conversational storytelling in this age group has focused primarily on relationships between more tradi- tional self-conceptions and conversational storytelling. Those find- ings indicate that such stories are an important medium for the development of self in social contexts among late adolescents and early adults (Pasupathi, 2006; Pasupathi, Alderman, et al., 2007; Pasupathi & Rich, 2005; Thoman et al., 2007). The present results expand these findings to the interpretive aspects of conversational narrative and connect to work on early childhood storytelling, memory, and self-development. Moreover, participants’ consistent lack of awareness that their stories were changing for listeners suggests that the effects of listeners on young adults’ stories were not a result of deliberate self-presentations; this is important be-
  • 205. cause social psychological work suggests that deliberate self- presentation does not always exert influences on subsequent self- perceptions (e.g., see Rhodewalt, 1998, for a review). From the childhood work, evaluative content, related to what we have termed interpretive content, is the most important content of memory narratives for self-conception development (e.g., Bird & Reese, 2006). Interpretive aspects of remembering in early child- hood serve, in part, to reflect and create a sense of agency. As children come to retain memories over longer periods, and to think of themselves as agents that endure across time, they begin to construct an agent in their stories that has more long-lasting qualities—an agent with beliefs, values, and long-term goals as well as immediate emotions and desires (see Habermas & Bluck, 2000). From this perspective, listeners can help or hinder the development of narrative identity in storytelling, in part by either supporting or constraining the agency of storytellers, regardless of whether the storyteller is a child or a young adult (see also Cleveland & Reese, 2005). The experience of agency and its extension in time, of course, change with development. But this change is likely to be reciprocally interrelated with the process of narrating one’s experiences in ways that come to construct and reflect a more enduring agency. Agency in storytelling, however, is multilayered. It includes both the agent within a story, who has feelings, beliefs, desires, and identities expressed in interpretive content, and the
  • 206. storyteller him- or herself, who is giving voice to the story. The interpretive content of autobiographical memories is also the aspect of those memories that is most unique and individuated. Thus, another way to think about distracted listening is that it silences the unique voice of a storyteller. From this perspective, it is not only that unresponsive listeners do not allow us to talk about what experi- ences mean for our self-views— how we as agents think, feel, and believe over time. It is that unresponsive listeners do not allow us to be who we are and who we are becoming, by telling stories in ways that express and further that uniqueness. Generally, these findings suggest continuity of developmental processes, in that listeners were of great importance for the re- membering of personal experiences in emergent adulthood. The process of parent– child remembering is already known to be critical in the development of narrative identity, memory, and self in early childhood (Fivush & Nelson, 2004). As children move into late adolescence and begin the transition to adulthood, the role played by listeners in the development of narrative identity may, in fact, become more specialized. That is, in early childhood, parents help children learn what and how to recall about the personal past, and this means that parents, as listeners, are shaping both narrative capacity and narrative identity. Responsive, elaborative mothers (and fathers) help children learn to tell elaborate, vivid,
  • 207. detailed, and interpretive stories about their past, and such children develop more advanced self-conceptions and identities in other ways as well (Reese et al., 1993). By late adolescence, narrative capacity is present—all our participants were capable of telling an adequate story about their personal experiences. However, the responsive- ness of listening friends continued to matter for the construction of stories that were richly reflective of narrative identity— of the specific, individual experience of our participants. Methodologically, however, and conceptually, our findings also differ from prior work in childhood, and those differences bear some consideration here. One issue concerns the comparability of distracted friends and low-elaborative mothers. In some ways, these two groups can be conceived of as having similar effects, in that both groups’ behaviors may convey a lack of interest in the construction of an elaborative, highly interpretive story. Other work from our laboratory demonstrates relationships between overall memory elaboration and naturally occurring variability in friends’ responsiveness but has not examined interpretive and 569NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT factual content separately (Liu, 2005). However, in other respects, these two groups are not the same. First, low-elaborative mothers
  • 208. are probably more responsive, within a storytelling interaction, than distracted friends. Distracted friends are unresponsive on average, to the entire story, whereas low-elaborative mothers are selectively responsive to specific aspects of events (often factual in published examples) that they construe as important but are unre- sponsive to other aspects in which their child may have more interest. Thus, low-elaborative mothers may convey interest in a preferred version of a story, whereas distracted listeners convey no interest. Both types of listeners may reduce the likelihood that a person constructs an elaborate sense of narrative identity in the moment but in somewhat distinct ways. Second, distracted friends in these studies were unresponsive on a single occasion but, given the generally established nature and high quality of these friend- ships, are probably more responsive on other occasions. Low- elaborative mothers, by contrast, are seen as engaging in a stylistic approach to the construction of narrative identity with their chil- dren—an approach that has some consistency across various rem- iniscing occasions and over developmental time frames. This brings up the larger issue, which is that the idea that conversational storytelling is a microcontext for the development of narrative identity requires consideration of the ways in which that micro- context is linked to more macrolevel developmental outcomes. One possibility is that at least some people have friendships that routinely and consistently discourage elaboration of interpretive
  • 209. aspects of remembering. Although this may seem unlikely, con- sider our gender effects in this context. In terms of storytelling content, we observed only one gender difference: Women’s stories about a computer game differed from men’s, primarily in the context of attentive friends. In that context, men’s stories were more factual and women’s were more interpretive, possibly be- cause of expertise in computer gaming generally, as expertise is linked to the nature of storytelling about an event (e.g., Pasupathi, Alderman, et al., 2007). Other differences by gender, however, were more connected to the idea of stable and consistent differences in the responsiveness of friends. Women also perceived their listeners as more respon- sive in general (Study 2) and specifically in the distracted condi- tion (Study 3). Further, women were more elaborative in general (across factual and interpretive information) in Study 2, suggesting congruence between their perceptions and their construction of narrative identity. Such findings are congruent with findings about parent– child reminiscing practices that differ between male and female children (e.g., Fivush, 1998) and are potentially due to real differences in listener behavior by gender. Past work suggests both men and women prefer female listeners (Clark, 1994), consistent with the idea that women may be more responsive listeners in general and perhaps are better able to remain somewhat responsive even in the face of dual task demands. This capacity, in turn,
  • 210. suggests that the gender of friends may be connected to the extent to which those friends facilitate the construction of narrative identity, and generates some predictions about the prevalence of friendships with women and the construction of narrative identity in this age range. Further, thinking about gender effects in this way suggests the potential importance of looking at continuity and discontinuity in the kinds of listening done by good friends and the kinds of parent– child reminiscing practices that characterized someone’s early childhood, as well as the parent– child reminiscing practices that characterize their current experience. Recent qualitative work (Weeks & Pasupathi, in press) suggests that at least in some cases, there may be interesting continuities between problematic parent– child reminiscing and problematic friend–friend reminiscing in late adolescence, continuities broadly consistent with attachment perspectives on development (Hesse, 1999). In addition to the possibility of some friendships entailing consistently low elaboration, there is also the almost certain like- lihood that friends do engage in unresponsive listening with one another periodically and that there are systematic features of that unresponsive listening. Although in the context of high-quality relationships like those we examined here, friends are seldom
  • 211. unresponsive across the board, they are certainly unresponsive some of the time and to particular kinds of content over time. In the long run, repeated unresponsiveness likely serves to silence a particular aspect of identity within that specific relationship con- text, and perhaps to silence that aspect of identity more broadly, depending on the nature of that relationship for the person in- volved. This set of implications is more consistent with the be- havior of low-elaborative mothers, who are in some sense shaping a conversational story toward a particular, mother-driven form. This kind of selective unresponsiveness can vary in its impact on young adults, from simply making it more difficult to express and explore, in reflection, one’s interest in particular experiences or activities, as in the case of unpopular majors or hobbies, to a real experience of silencing, as in the case of minority sexual identities. The extent of the impact will also depend on the individual’s motivation and capacity to seek out alternative social relationships within which particular identities can be elaborated, reflected upon, and thus further developed—and indeed, on the very avail- ability of alternative relationships. Factual Information: Development and Disagreement The disagreeableness of listeners tended to suppress factual content in remembering, and across all three of our studies, older participants consistently included more facts in their stories. The
  • 212. latter finding is consistent with the idea that memory development continues even into early adulthood (P. J. Bauer, 2006; Holmes & Conway, 1999; Rubin & Schulkind, 1997). Additionally, it is broadly consistent with developments in knowledge about life (e.g., Glueck & Baltes, 2006; Pasupathi, Staudinger, & Baltes, 2001) and in memory for news events, music, and other happen- ings (e.g., Holmes & Conway, 1999) across this same age range. This finding clearly suggests the potential for future work, espe- cially in looking at a broader age range and at more particular types of information. Factual information was suppressed in the context of disagree- able listening for both men and women in Study 1, for men in Study 3, and in past work (Pasupathi et al., 1998). Though tenta- tive, the suppression of factual information in disagreeable dis- course is worth noting, because factual information can ground interpretive information, and interpretations or evaluations without sufficient factual basis may be tenuously held, vulnerable to vari- ability over time or even simply not truly reflective of a person’s mental representation. This is the assumption behind the Adult Attachment Interview, that broad statements about a “good mother” are not plausible without being grounded in factual details 570 PASUPATHI AND HOYT (Hesse, 1999). Although interpretive information was our major
  • 213. focus, the facts of our lives also have developmental import. Further, it is worth noting that disagreement combined with re- sponsiveness can facilitate narrative identity development very well—a fact consistent with the earlier work of researchers inter- ested in autonomy and identity development (Cooper & Grotevant, 1985) and an important reminder that responsiveness is not syn- onymous with positivity or agreeableness. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that the development of narrative identity during late adolescence and early adulthood, at a microlevel, is fundamentally intertwined with the extent to which listeners are responsive and encourage elaboration. Further work that attempts to examine the processes by which identity develops needs to take into consideration the context of conversational storytelling and, within that context, the collaboration of listeners. From childhood through adulthood, responsive listeners are critical for allowing each of us to express and further our own unique voice. References Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469 – 480.
  • 214. Bartlett, F. C. (1995). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. (Origi- nal work published in 1932) Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2004a). Growth goals, maturity, and well-being. Developmental Psychology, 40, 114 –127. Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2004b). Personal growth in adults’ stories of life transitions. Journal of Personality, 72, 573– 602. Bauer, J. J., & McAdams, D. P. (2005). Interpreting the good life: Growth memories in the lives of mature, happy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 203–216. Bauer, P. J. (2006). Remembering the times of our lives: Memory in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Bauer, P. J., & Fivush, R. (1992). Constructing event representations: Building on a foundation of variation and enabling relations. Cognitive Development, 7, 381– 401. Baumeister, R. F., Stilman, A., & Wotman, S. R. (1990). Victim and perpetrator accounts of interpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narra- tives about anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,
  • 215. 994 –1005. Bavelas, J. B., Coates, L., & Johnson, T. (2000). Listeners as co-narrators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 941–952. Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42, 613– 626. Bluck, S., & Glueck, J. (2004). Making things better and learning a lesson: Experiencing wisdom across the lifespan. Journal of Personality, 72, 543–572. Bohanek, J. G., Marin, K. A., & Fivush, R. (2006, March). Family narratives and adolescent identity. Paper presented at the biennial meet- ing of the Society for Research on Adolescence, San Francisco, CA. Buhrmeister, D. (1996). Need fulfillment, interpersonal competence, and the developmental contexts of early adolescent friendship. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 158 –185). Cam- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Clark, R. A. (1994). Children’s and adolescents’ gender preferences for
  • 216. conversational partners for specific communicative objectives. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 313–319. Cleveland, E., & Reese, E. (2005). Maternal structure and autonomy support in conversations about the past: Contributions to children’s autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychology, 41, 376 – 388. Constantinople, A. (1969). An Eriksonian measure of personality devel- opment in college students. Developmental Psychology, 1, 357– 372. Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288. Cooper, C. R. (1999). Multiple selves, multiple worlds: Cultural perspec- tives on individuality and connectedness in adolescent development. In A. Masten (Ed.), Cultural processes in child development (pp. 25–57). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Cooper, C. R., & Grotevant, H. D. (1985). Patterns of interaction in family relationships and the development of identity exploration in adolescence. Child Development, 56, 415– 428.
  • 217. Daddis, C. (2008). Influence of close friends on the boundaries of adoles- cent personal authority. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 75–98. Dickinson, C., & Givon, T. (1995). Memory and conversation: Toward an experimental paradigm. In T. Givon (Ed.), Conversation: Cognitive, communicative, and social perspectives (pp. 91–132). Amsterdam: Ben- jamins. Dudukovic, N. M., Marsh, E. J., & Tversky, B. (2004). Telling a story or telling it straight: The effects of entertaining versus accurate retellings on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 125–143. Dusek, J. B., & McIntyre, J. G. (2003). Self-concept and self- esteem development. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 290 –309). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Erikson, E. H., & Erikson, J. M. (1997). The life cycle completed: Extended version. New York: Norton. Fivush, R. (1991). The social construction of personal narratives. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 37, 59 – 82. Fivush, R. (1998). Gendered narratives: Elaboration, structure, and emo-
  • 218. tion in parent-child reminiscing across the preschool years. In C. P. Thompson, D. J. Herrmann, D. Bruce, J. D. Read, D. G. Payne, & M. P. Toglia (Eds.), Autobiographical memory: Theoretical and applied per- spectives (pp. 79 –104). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Fivush, R., Bohanek, J., Robertson, R., & Duke, M. (2004). Family narratives and the development of children’s emotional well- being. In M. W. Pratt & B. H. Fiese (Eds.), Family stories and the life course: Across time and generations (pp. 55–76). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and language in the emergence of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15, 586 –590. Glueck, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Using the concept of wisdom to enhance the expression of wisdom knowledge: Not the philosopher’s dream but differential effects of developmental preparedness. Psychology and Ag- ing, 21, 679 – 690. Habermas, T., & Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a life: The development of the life story in adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 748 – 769. Haden, C. A., Haine, R. A., & Fivush, R. (1997). Developing narrative
  • 219. structure in parent– child reminiscing across the preschool years. Devel- opmental Psychology, 33, 295–307. Hair, E. C., Moore, K. A., Garrett, S. B., Ling, T., & Cleveland, K. (2008). The continued importance of quality parent-adolescent relationships during late adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18, 187– 199. Harley, K., & Reese, E. (1999). Origins of autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1338 –1348. Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In N. Eisenberg & W. Damon (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 553– 617). New York: John Wiley and Sons. Harter, S., & Monsour, A. (1992). Developmental analysis of conflict caused by opposing attributes in the adolescent self-portrait. Develop- mental Psychology, 28, 251–260. Hartup, W. W. (1996). The company they keep: Friendships and their developmental significance. Child Development, 67, 1–13. Hazan, C., & Zeifman, D. (1994). Sex and the psychological tether. Advances in Personal Relationships, 5, 151–177.
  • 220. 571NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT Hesse, E. (1999). The adult attachment interview: Historical and current perspectives. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 395– 433). New York: Guilford Press. Holmes, A., & Conway, M. A. (1999). Generation identity and the remi- niscence bump: Memory for public and private events. Journal of Adult Development, 6, 21–34. Hooker, K., & McAdams, D. P. (2003). Personality reconsidered: A new agenda for aging research. Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psycho- logical Sciences and Social Sciences, 58B, P296 –P304. Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., & Peterson, C. (1994). How can I remember when “I” wasn’t there: Long-term retention of traumatic experiences and emergence of the cognitive self. Consciousness and Cognition, 3, 327– 355. Hyman, I. E. (1994). Conversational remembering: Story recall with a peer versus for an experimenter. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 49 – 66.
  • 221. King, L. A., & Patterson, C. (2000). Reconstructing life goals after the birth of a child with Down Syndrome: Finding happiness and growing. International Journal of Rehabilitation and Health, 5(1), 17–30. Kitchener, K. S., Lynch, C. L., Fischer, K. W., & Wood, P. K. (1993). Developmental range of reflective judgment: The effect of contextual support and practice on developmental stage. Developmental Psychol- ogy, 29, 893–906. Korobov, N., & Bamberg, M. (2004). Positioning a “mature” self in interactive practices: How adolescent males negotiate “physical attrac- tion” in group talk. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 471– 492. Kroger, J. (2003). Identity development during adolescence. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 205–226). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Liu, T. (2005). The planning function of imagined conversation. Unpub- lished master’s thesis, University of Utah. McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological
  • 222. Inquiry, 7, 295–321. McAdams, D. P., Bauer, J. J., Sakaeda, A. R., Anyidoho, N. A., Machado, M. A., Magrino-Failla, K., et al. (2006). Continuity and change in the life story: A longitudinal study of autobiographical memories in emerg- ing adulthood. Journal of Personality, 74, 1371–1400. McAdams, D. P., Hoffman, B. J., Mansfield, E. D., & Day, R. (1996). Themes of agency and communion in significant autobiographical scenes. Journal of Personality, 64, 339 –377. McAdams, D. P., Reynolds, J., Lewis, M., Patten, A. H., & Bowman, P. J. (2001). When bad things turn good and good things turn bad: Sequences of redemption and contamination in life narrative and their relation to psychosocial adaptation in midlife adults and in students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 474 – 485. McGregor, I., & Holmes, J. G. (1999). How storytelling shapes memory and impressions of relationship events over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 403– 419. McLean, K. C. (2005). Late adolescent identity development: Narrative meaning making and memory telling. Developmental
  • 223. Psychology, 41, 683– 691. McLean, K. C., Pasupathi, M., & Pals, J. L. (2007). Selves creating stories creating selves: A process model of narrative self-development. Person- ality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 262–278. McLean, K. C., & Pratt, M. W. (2006). Life’s little (and big) lessons: Identity statuses and meaning making in the turning point narratives of emerging adults. Developmental Psychology, 42, 714 –722. McLean, K. C., & Thorne, A. (2003). Late adolescents’ self- defining memories about relationships. Developmental Psychology, 39, 635– 645. McNulty, S. E., & Swann, W. B. J. (1994). Identity negotiation in room- mate relationships: The self as architect and consequence of social reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1012– 1023. Meilman, P. W. (1979). Cross-sectional age changes in ego identity status during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 15, 230 –231. Moffitt, K. H., & Singer, J. A. (1994). Continuity in the life story: Self-defining memories, affect, and approach/avoidance personal striv- ings. Journal of Personality, 62, 21– 43.
  • 224. Pals, J. L. (2006a). Constructing the “springboard effect”: Causal connec- tions, self-making, and growth within the life story. In D. McAdams, R. Josselson, & A. Lieblich (Eds.), Identity and story: Creating self in narrative (pp. 175–199). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Pals, J. L. (2006b). Narrative identity processing of difficult life experi- ences: Pathways of personality development and positive self- transformation in adulthood. Journal of Personality, 74, 1079 – 1110. Parker, J. G., & Gottman, J. M. (1989). Social and emotional development in a relational context: Friendship interaction from early childhood to adolescence. In T. J. Berndt & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child development (pp. 95–131). Oxford, England: John Wiley and Sons. Pasupathi, M. (2001). The social construction of the personal past and its implications for adult development. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 651– 672. Pasupathi, M. (2006). Silk from sows’ ears: Collaborative construction of everyday selves in everyday stories. In D. McAdams, R. Josselson, & A.
  • 225. Lieblich (Eds.), Identity and story: Creating self in narrative (pp. 129 –150). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Pasupathi, M. (2007). Telling and the remembered self: Linguistic differ- ences in memories for previously disclosed and undisclosed events. Memory, 15, 258 –270. Pasupathi, M., Alderman, K., & Shaw, D. (2007). Talking the talk: Col- laborative remembering and self-perceived expertise. Discourse Pro- cesses, 43, 55–77. Pasupathi, M., Carstensen, L. L., Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1999). Responsive listening in long-married couples: A psycholinguis- tic perspective. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 173–193. Pasupathi, M., & Mansour, E. (2006). Adult age differences in autobio- graphical narratives: Integrating experiences with the self. Developmen- tal Psychology, 42, 798 – 808. Pasupathi, M., Mansour, E., & Brubaker, J. (2007). Developing a life story: Constructing relations between self and experience in autobiographical narratives. Human Development, 50, 85–110. Pasupathi, M., & Rich, B. (2005). Inattentive listening
  • 226. undermines self- verification in personal storytelling. Journal of Personality, 73, 1051– 1086. Pasupathi, M., Stallworth, L. M., & Murdoch, K. (1998). How what we tell becomes what we know: Listener effects on speakers’ long-term mem- ory for events. Discourse Processes, 26, 1–25. Pasupathi, M., Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Seeds of wisdom: Adolescents’ knowledge and judgment about difficult life problems. Developmental Psychology, 37, 351–361. Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language, 26, 49 – 67. Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (1993). Parental styles of talking about the past. Developmental Psychology, 29, 596 – 606. Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversations about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403– 430. Rhodewalt, F. (1998). Self-presentation and the phenomenal self: The “carryover effect” revisited. In J. M. Darley & J. Cooper (Eds.),
  • 227. Attri- bution and social interaction (pp. 373–398). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Rimé, B., Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Zech, E., & Phillipot, P. (1998). Social sharing of emotion: New evidence and new questions. European Review of Social Psychology, 9, 145–189. Rubin, D. C., & Schulkind, M. D. (1997). Distribution of important and 572 PASUPATHI AND HOYT word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year- old adults. Psychology and Aging, 12, 524 –535. Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z., & Fong, G. T. (1990). Motivated recruitment of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 59, 229 –241. Schacter, D. L. (1996). Searching for memory: The brain, the mind, and the past. New York: Basic Books. Sutin, A. R., & Robins, R. W. (2005). Continuity and correlates of emotions and motives in self-defining memories. Journal of Personality,
  • 228. 73, 793– 824. Thoman, D. B., Sansone, C., & Pasupathi, M. (2007). Talking about interest: Exploring the role of social interaction for regulating motivation and the interest experience. Journal of Happiness Studies, 8, 335–370. Thorne, A. (2000). Personal memory telling and personality development. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(1), 45–56. Thorne, A., McLean, K. C., & Lawrence, A. M. (2004). When remember- ing is not enough: Reflecting on self-defining memories in late adoles- cence. Journal of Personality, 72, 513–541. Tice, D. M. (1992). Self-concept change and self-presentation: The looking glass self is also a magnifying glass. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 435– 451. Tversky, B., & Marsh, E. J. (2000). Biased retellings of events yield biased memories. Cognitive Psychology, 40(1), 1–38. Updegraff, K. A., McHale, S. M., & Whiteman, S. D. (2006). The nature and correlates of Mexican-American adolescents’ time with parents and peers. Child Development, 77, 1470 –1486. Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs:
  • 229. Autobio- graphical memory and self-description in European American and Chi- nese Children. Developmental Psychology, 40, 3–15. Waterman, A. S. (1982). Identity development from adolescence to adult- hood: An extension of theory and a review of research. Developmental Psychology, 18, 341–358. Weeks, T. L., & Pasupathi, M. (in press). Autonomy, identity, and joint autobiographical remembering in adolescence. In K. C. McLean & M. Pasupathi (Eds.), Narrative development in adolescence: Creating the storied self. New York: Springer. Woike, B. A. (1995). Most memorable experiences: Evidence for a link between implicit and explicit motives and social cognitive processes in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 1081– 1091. Woike, B., Gershkovich, I., Piorkowski, R., & Polo, M. (1999). The role of motives in the content and structure of autobiographical memory. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 600 – 612. (Appendix follows) 573NARRATIVE IDENTITY DEVELOPMENT
  • 230. Appendix Sample Transcript With Coding Example: Pair 29 Content S: All right, well my experience was a couple days ago 1 we ran out of toilet paper. And 2 Mary was like, okay, 2 I’ll go get toilet paper, because 2 it’s her turn, right? 3 Yeah. 1 Ten minutes later, she’s like, hey Eileen, 2 can you get some toilet paper? 2 I was so mad. 3 Yeah, like, yesterday all day long she’s like, 2 I don’t have anything to do all day. 2 I’m like, why don’t you go get some toilet paper because 2 we’re out of toilet paper. 2 She’s like, no, 2 I just want to sit here and 2 play on the Internet. 2 L: Oh. 1 S: So mad. 3 Finally today I went and got it. 2 (inaudible) 9 I was like, napkins are good. 2 L: That’s smart. 3 S: Well, then there’s the part where 3 I’m the younger 3 (inaudible), 9 supposedly, because she’s twenty-six. 3 L: Oh. 1
  • 231. S: And she’s like, no, 2 I don’t want to. 2 Being lazy. 3 She’s like, I’ll pay you back. 2 I’m like, Smith’s is two minutes down the road. 2 Go get some. 2 L: That’s funny. Funny. 3 Roommate troubles. 2 S: Drama, drama, drama 3 We’re done. 1 Note. S � speaker; L � listener; 1 � regulatory; 2 � event; 3 � interpretive; 9 � uncodable. Received August 9, 2007 Revision received September 29, 2008 Accepted October 14, 2008 � 574 PASUPATHI AND HOYT Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme Using Narrative Retells in Young School-Age Children John Heilmann East Carolina University, Greenville, NC Jon F. Miller Ann Nockerts University of Wisconsin—Madison Claudia Dunaway
  • 232. San Diego Unified School District, CA Purpose: To evaluate the clinical utility of the narrative scoring scheme (NSS) as an index of narrative macrostructure for young school-age children. Method: Oral retells of a wordless picture book were elicited from 129 typically developing chil- dren, ages 5–7. A series of correlations and hierarchical regression equations were completed using microstructural measures of vocabulary and grammar to predict NSS scores. Results: While the NSS was significantly corre- lated with age and each of the microstructural measures, the hierarchical regression analyses revealed a unique relationship between vocabu- lary and narrative macrostructure. Conclusion: The NSS is an efficient and informa- tive tool for documenting children’s development of narrative macrostructure. The relationship between the NSS and microstructural measures demonstrates that it is a robust measure of children’s overall oral narrative competence and a powerful tool for clinicians and researchers. The unique relationship between lexical diversity and the NSS confirmed that a special relationship exists between vocabulary and narrative organi- zation skills in young school-age children. Key Words: narrative, language sample analysis, story grammar, vocabulary A nalysis of oral narratives provides a rich source of data that documents children’s language use in a
  • 233. naturalistic context. Narrative analysis is a highly effective clinical and research tool, as it allows examiners to analyze multiple linguistic features simultaneously using a single short sample. Examiners have the opportunity to doc- ument children’s productive vocabulary and grammar using microstructural analyses as well as children’s broader text- level narrative organization skills by utilizing macrostructural analyses (see Westby, 2005). Microstructural analyses primarily focus on children’s lin- guistic form and content, which are measured within individ- ual utterances. Linguistic form is commonly assessed by analyzing children’s grammatical and syntactic abilities using mean length of utterance (e.g., Brown, 1973; Miller, 1981) and various measures of sentence complexity (e.g., Nippold, Hesketh, Duthie, & Mansfield, 2005; Schuele & Tolbert, 2001; Scott & Stokes, 1995). Measures of linguistic content are used to document children’s productive vocabulary skills, which typically calculate children’s lexical diversity using measures such as type–token ratio (Templin, 1957) and number of dif- ferent words (Miller, 1987; Miller & Klee, 1995). While these measures are the most commonly cited microstructural anal- yses in the literature, this list is in no way exhaustive. Micro- structural analyses continue to be reviewed, critiqued, and reanalyzed (see Justice et al., 2006, for a review and discussion). Macrostructural analyses, on the other hand, examine children’s language skills beyond the utterance level and document children’s ability to relate concepts that transcend the individual utterance. Most macrostructural analyses of children’s narratives are rooted in the story grammar tradi- tion, which proposes that all stories have a setting and episode system (Mandler & Johnson, 1977; Rumelhart, 1975; Stein &
  • 234. Glenn, 1979). The setting provides background information Research American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19 • 154–166 • May 2010 • A American Speech-Language-Hearing Association154 on the characters and their environment, while the episode system includes three main components that occur in all stories: (a) a problem (initiating event and/or internal response), (b) attempts at solving the problem, and (c) consequences/ outcomes. To be a complete episode, a narrator must include all three of these key components (see Strong, 1998, for a review). These settings and episodes can be combined in an infinite number of ways to create individual stories. Relationship Between Microstructural and Macrostructural Measures Previous research has demonstrated that lexical and gram- matical gains play an important role in children’s acqui- sition of narrative proficiency (Berman & Slobin, 1994; Bishop & Donlan, 2005). In their seminal work, Berman and Slobin (1994) documented the role of linguistic forms taking on new functions that aid in the organization of narratives. This work described the trade-offs that occurred when differ- ent microstructural features were used for different functions, including the organization of narratives. Children’s narrative organization skills were positively related to advances in use of microstructural features, including grammatical forms (e.g., verb tense, aspect, and voice), lexical forms (e.g., lexical aspect and manner/cause of verbs of motion), and lexico-grammatical features (e.g., locative particles, prepositional phrases, and
  • 235. connectives). This pattern of results is consistent with Slobin’s long-standing claim that “new forms first express old func- tions, and new functions are first expressed by old forms” (Slobin, 1973, p. 184). Bishop and Donlan (2005) observed that children’s ability to encode and retell a story was more strongly related to linguistic ability than nonverbal IQ. Chil- dren’s microstructures, including complex syntax and relating causal concepts, were more influential in event memory and story organization than their cognitive skills. These data docu- menting the unique relationship between microstructures and macrostructures are contrary to evidence demonstrating that children’s narrative organization skills emerge from more general cognitive capabilities, including executive function (van den Broek, 1997), and sociolinguistic processes (Eaton, Collis, & Lewis, 1999; McCabe, 1997; Peterson & McCabe, 2004; Quasthoff, 1997a, 1997b). Narrative Skills of Children With Language Impairment Given the strong relationship between microstructural and macrostructural measures, it is not surprising that children with language impairment have substantial difficulty using appropriate vocabulary and grammar when telling stories (Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Gillam & Johnston, 1992; Pearce, McCormack, & James, 2003; Reilly, Losh, Bellugi, & Wulfeck, 2004) in addition to substantial difficulty with text-level orga- nization of narratives (Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Manhardt & Rescorla, 2002; Merritt & Liles, 1987; Pearce et al., 2003; Reilly et al., 2004). The presence of both microstructural and macrostructural deficits in children with language impair- ment counters theories that identify the primary cause of language impairment as deficiencies with grammatical com- petence (e.g., Clahsen, 1989; Gopnik & Crago, 1991; Rice, Wexler, & Cleave, 1995). Alternatively, the global narrative difficulties of children with language impairment suggest that
  • 236. their language deficits may be due to broader information- processing deficits, such as a reduced processing capacity (Boudreau, 2007; Colozzo, Garcia, Megan, Gillam, & Johnston, 2006). Further study of the relationship between children’s microstructural and macrostructural language skills will pro- vide a better understanding of the nature of the impairment. Better understanding of children’s narrative skills in general, and macrostructural skills in particular, also has substantial clinical implications. Difficulty with narrative organization can have a dramatic impact on children with language impair- ment, as such discourse-level skills are required to effectively communicate. While vocabulary and grammar deficits limit children’s ability to produce fully competent utterances, im- paired macrostructural skills affect children’s ability to gen- erate coherent and age-appropriate extended discourse. Furthermore, narratives are a major component of the school curriculum, and children are expected to understand and use appropriate narrative form effectively. Therefore, narrative macrostructure skills must be efficiently and accurately docu- mented, and should be considered within an extensive assess- ment protocol for children with language difficulties. Methods of Measuring Narrative Macrostructure While most narrative macrostructure measures are rooted in the story grammar tradition and share the same underlying principles, the coding of narrative macrostructure varies widely throughout the literature. Some narrative macro- structure coding schemes documented children’s inclusion of specific story grammar components (e.g., Strong, 1998) or identified the presence or absence of specific story grammar components for a given story (e.g., Berman, 1988; Boudreau & Hedberg, 1999; Miles & Chapman, 2002; Reilly et al., 2004). In these studies, children who produced more story grammar components and/or more advanced story grammar features were thought to have stronger narrative organization
  • 237. skills. The second major class of macrostructural measures used text-level judgments of children’s narrative proficiency (Applebee, 1978; Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Stein, 1988). Rather than identifying the presence or absence of specific story grammar components, these measures required holistic judgments by the examiner to rate the quality and develop- mental level of the narrative. The major advantage of the simple story grammar coding procedures is that it facilitates a relatively high level of ac- curacy across coders. There is less room for differences across coders, as the coder is only responsible for identifying the presence or absence of specific story-related themes. The major disadvantage of simple story grammar analyses is that they are potentially limited in their ability to account for the abstract interutterance concepts and qualitative aspects of the narrative, or the story’s “sparkle” (Peterson & McCabe, 1983). McFadden and Gillam (1996) demonstrated that holistic rat- ings of children’s narratives capture the more refined aspects of narratives, such as charm and depth, and that these ratings were better than simple story grammar analyses for documenting differences between children with language impairment and their typically developing peers between the ages of 9;0 (years; months) and 11;7. Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 155 Additional Skills Required to Tell an Effective Story: Beyond Story Grammar The development of narratives in children and adults has been studied extensively and has revealed additional areas of advancement beyond inclusion of story grammar features, including children’s use of literate language and cohesive de-
  • 238. vices (Bamberg & Damrad-Frye, 1991; Halliday & Hasan, 1976; Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Wigglesworth, 1997). Use of literate language occurs when children use abstract language features commonly used by teachers and found in the curri- culum (Westby, 2005). Some key literate language features related to narrative competence include use of metacognitive verbs (e.g., think or know), metalinguistic verbs (e.g., say or talk), and elaborated noun phrases (e.g., the boy in the restau- rant with the frog; see Nippold, 2007, and Westby, 2005, for a review). Bamberg and Damrad-Frye (1991) identified that more sophisticated and later developing narratives included abstract language such as metacognitive and metalinguistic verbs. In their analysis of productions of Frog, Where Are You? (Mayer, 1969), Bamberg and Damrad-Frye identified that these abstract language skills emerged at age 5 years, demonstrated robust development through adulthood, and were essential for relating the hierarchical relationships be- tween events in complex narrative productions. Additional studies have identified that literate language skills were pres- ent in children’s oral narratives during the preschool years (Curenton & Justice, 2004), developed through the school years and into adolescence (Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001; Nippold, 2007; Pelligrini, Galda, Bartini, & Charak, 1998), and were used less frequently by children with language im- pairment (Greenhalgh & Strong, 2001). Another high-level narrative feature that continues to develop through the school years is the cohesiveness of children’s nar- rative productions (Halliday & Hasan, 1976; Wigglesworth, 1997). To tell a story, narrators must effectively use cohesive devices to carry concepts across individual utterances. Three major categories of cohesive devices are (a) referential cohe- sion, which allows a narrator to maintain appropriate refer- ence to the characters, objects, and locations across utterances using both noun phrases and pronouns; (b) conjunctive cohe- sion, which allows a narrator to sustain concepts across phrases
  • 239. and utterances using conjunctive words and phrases (e.g., and, but, besides, on the other hand, finally, in addition); and (c) lexical cohesion, which allows a narrator to effectively use vocabulary to link concepts across utterances. Measures of cohesiveness can be a sensitive index of language use, as children with language impairment have more difficulty with correct use of cohesive ties (Hedberg & Westby, 1993; Liles, 1985; Strong & Shaver, 1991). While cohesion is often con- sidered a microstructural measure, we treated it as a macro- structural measure because aspects of cohesion transcend individual utterances and are necessary for producing coher- ent narratives. The Narrative Scoring Scheme: A Comprehensive Measure of Narrative Proficiency Our goal in developing the narrative scoring scheme (NSS) was to create a metric that documents the range of skills required for school-age children to effectively tell a coherent and in- teresting story (see Appendix). To extend beyond simple story grammar analyses, the NSS incorporates multiple aspects of the narrative process into a single scoring rubric and provides an overall impression of the child’s narrative ability. This metric combines both the basic features of the story grammar approaches as well as the higher level narrative skills that continue to develop through the school-age years. In addition to adding higher level narrative skills in the scoring scheme, the NSS uses a combination of discrete coding criteria and examiner judgment. The NSS was created to improve on the simple story grammar measures by requiring the examiners to make interutterance text-level judgments, which have been shown to be more effective than discrete coding schemes in identifying children with language impairment (McFadden & Gillam, 1996). By breaking the judgments into seven skill
  • 240. areas, examiners have the opportunity to reflect on each com- ponent of the narrative process and judge the child’s profi- ciency in that area. This combination of explicit scoring guidelines and flexibility to allow for examiner judgment reflects the hybrid nature of the NSS. The scores from the seven NSS categories are combined to provide a single com- posite score, which allows the examiner to generate an index of children’s overall narrative ability. The first step in developing the NSS was establishing the key components from the story grammar literature, which included the story’s introduction, the major conflicts and res- olutions (conflict resolution), and a conclusion. To document children’s use of literate language, the NSS includes catego- ries that assess metacognitive and metalinguistic verbs. The mental states component documents children’s abilities to use metacognitive verbs (e.g., think and know) to describe the characters’ thoughts and feelings. The characterdevelopment component of the NSS also documents children’s literate language skills by measuring the ability to use metalinguistic verbs (e.g., talk and say), differentiate between main and sup- porting characters, and talk in the first person to depict the characters in the story. The NSS evaluates two separate as- pects of cohesive ties that were adapted from Halliday and Hasan (1976). The referencing component measures aspects of referential cohesion, including appropriate use of pronouns and antecedents. The cohesion component documents the conjunctive and lexical aspects of cohesion, including appro- priate ordering, emphasis of critical events, and transitions between events. Goals of the Study Children’s performance on the NSS has been reported in studies that examined the narrative organization skills of na- tive Spanish-speaking children who were learning English
  • 241. as a second language (Miller et al., 2006). The goal of the present study was to describe the NSS from a clinical perspec- tive and to further analyze the linguistic properties of the measure in a group of children who were fluent in English. Furthermore, the literature has revealed that there is a special relationship between children’s microstructural and macro- structural language skills (Berman & Slobin, 1994; Bishop & Donlan, 2005). To better understand the linguistic properties of the NSS and to extend our understanding of the relationship 156 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19 • 154–166 • May 2010 between microstructural and macrostructural measures, a second goal was to document the relationship between chil- dren’s vocabulary, grammar, and narrative organization skills. To achieve this, we examined the relationship between mea- sures of vocabulary, grammar, and the NSS. These analyses further clarified the key constructs that the NSS is measuring and also provided additional evidence for the role of vocabu- lary and grammar in the development of young school-age children’s narrative organization skills. This study addressed the following questions: 1. Are age and measures of vocabulary and grammar signifi- cantly correlated with NSS scores in narrative retells of young school-age children? 2. Are measures of vocabulary uniquely related to NSS scores in narrative retells of young school-age children? 3. Are measures of grammar uniquely related to NSS scores in narrative retells of young school-age children?
  • 242. Method Participants A total of 129 typically developing children age 5–7 years were recruited for this study. The children were recruited from public schools in the San Diego (CA) City School and Cajon Valley School Districts. Administrators from the two districts assisted with obtaining informed consent from each of the children’s primary caregivers. The pool of potential partici- pants was reviewed by the school’s speech-language pathol- ogists (SLPs) and classroom teachers to identify children who qualified for the study. To participate in the study, children were required to have average scores on all summative class- room, district, and state assessments. The classroom teachers reviewed the records for each student to identify him or her as average performing. The child’s academic record was also reviewed to identify that he or she had no history of language impairment and/or learning disability. While academic data were used as inclusionary criteria for the participants, test scores and descriptions of performance were not recorded and were not available for further analysis. The majority of the participants were native English speakers. A small percentage of the children were Spanish/English bilingual and designated as “fluent English.” This designation was made by the child’s respective school district and was based on a passing grade on an English proficiency test and grade-level academics. The SLPs and teachers confirmed that each child met the inclu- sionary criteria and enrolled the eligible children in the study. The individual children participating in the study provided verbal assent prior to completing the protocol. Table 1 summarizes the demographic data for the partici- pants. Sixty-one percent of the participants were in kinder- garten, 36% of the children were in first grade, and 2% were in preschool. The numbers of male and female participants were roughly equal, with slightly fewer boys than girls. The
  • 243. school SLPs attempted to recruit students who reflected the racial and ethnic diversity of their schools. The SLPs first identified the children’s ethnicity (Hispanic or not Hispanic) and then documented the non-Hispanic children’s race. A review of the race and ethnicity data revealed that the sample is a relatively heterogeneous group. Socioeconomic status was measured by calculating the highest number of years of education that the child’s mother completed. On average, mothers completed 14.4 years of education (SD = 2.5), with a range of 9–20 years. The majority of the mothers completed at least some college, while only eight of the children’s mothers did not complete high school. Procedure Each participant completed a narrative retell of the word- less picture book Frog, Where Are You? (Mayer, 1969). The purpose of collecting the narratives was to establish a nor- mative database reflecting typically developing children’s oral narrative skills and to further our understanding of chil- dren’s developing narrative competence. The head SLP from each district was responsible for training 18 school-based SLPs to elicit the oral narratives. The head SLP and clinicians met on three separate occasions and had the opportunity to practice the protocol with each other several times. The clini- cians read the scripted instructions to the students and cued them to listen to a taped version of the story while following along with the pictures in the book. The students then retold the story using the book as an aid. Examiner prompts were limited to encouragement to begin the story and open-ended cues to continue the retell. The scripted instructions and audiotaped story script were adapted from the Strong Narrative Assess- ment Procedure (Strong, 1998) and were used to facilitate high fidelity among the numerous examiners completing the language sample elicitation.
  • 244. Transcription and Coding The children’s narrative productions were digitally recorded and sent to the Language Analysis Lab at the University of Wisconsin—Madison, where they were transcribed by trained research assistants who had at least 10 hr of transcription TABLE 1. Demographic data for all participants. Variable n Grade Preschool 3 Kindergarten 79 First grade 47 Sex Female 69 Male 60 Race/ethnicity White 87 Hispanic 16 Othera 15 African American 7 No data 4 Note. Ethnicity data were collected for children who were Hispanic or Latino. Race data are provided for all children who were non- Hispanic/ non-Latino. aOther races/ethnicities included Arabic (2), Chinese (3), Japanese (2), Korean (1), Filipino (5), Portuguese (1), and Samoan (1).
  • 245. Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 157 experience using standard coding conventions for Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) software (Miller & Iglesias, 2008). Utterances were segmented into communi- cation units (C-units; Labov & Waletzky, 1967), which in- cluded a main clause and all dependent clauses. The transcripts began and ended with the child’s first and last utterance, re- spectively. See Miller and Iglesias (2008) for a full review of the transcription conventions. After completing the orthographic transcription, the research assistant reviewed the transcript and completed the NSS. To score the NSS, the transcriber carefully reviewed the narrative transcript and assigned a score of 0–5 for each of the seven categories summarized in the Appendix. Categories that could not be scored received a score of zero or NA. Scores of zero were given if the child did something that precluded the ex- aminer from scoring a section of the NSS, such as skipping a part of the story or refusing to complete the task. If there was an error on the part of the examiner (e.g., not following the protocol or problems with the recording), sections of the NSS that were affected were not scored, and the examiner coded the section as not applicable for analysis (i.e., NA). For all other sections, scores of 1 reflected minimal presence/immature performance, scores of 3 reflected emerging skills, and scores of 5 reflected proficient performance. Transcribers also had the opportunity to assign scores of 2 and 4 if performance was judged to be between the major anchors. To improve the accuracy of the scoring procedure, specific guidelines were provided for scores of 1, 3, and 5 (see the Appendix). These guidelines assisted the transcriber in assigning an accurate score that reflected the child’s performance in each compo-
  • 246. nent of the narrative process and reduced the abstractness of the narrative macrostructure concepts. The scoring across the seven categories received equal weighting because the liter- ature revealed that each of these seven narrative aspects is necessary for telling a well-developed story. Furthermore, keeping the scoring rules straightforward and consistent facilitates simple and accurate scoring. A comprehensive training procedure is available on the SALT Web site (www. saltsoftware.com/training/handcoded/) that includes an overview of the NSS; scoring tips; a description of how to enter NSS scores into a SALT file; excerpts from samples demonstrating minimal/immature, emerging, and proficient performance across each section of the NSS; and a set of practice transcripts. In addition to the NSS, the transcribers completed coding for the subordination index (SI; Scott & Stokes, 1995; Strong, 1998). The transcribers added a code to each C-unit that sum- marized the number of independent and dependent clauses. C-units that were incomplete, unintelligible, or nonverbal, or that had an error at the utterance level, were excluded from the analysis. Utterance-level errors included incorrect word order, omission of more than two words in an utterance, and utterances that simply did not make sense. Elliptical responses to examiner questions were also excluded from the SI analysis. Utterances in which the child inappropriately omitted the sub- ject or copula were coded and received a score of zero. After each C-unit was coded, the SI was generated by dividing the total number of clauses (both main and subordinate) by the total number of C-units. After all the narratives were transcribed and coded, the transcripts were analyzed using SALT (Miller & Iglesias, 2008), which produced a rectan- gular data file summarizing each dependent measure for each of the transcripts. This file was formatted for statistical anal-
  • 247. ysis using SPSS Version 16.0. Agreement Accuracy of the transcription and coding process was ex- amined at three levels. Protocol accuracy was calculated by the principal investigator, who reviewed 10% of the written transcripts to identify whether the transcribers were adhering to the transcription conventions. Percentage agreement be- tween the transcribers and principal investigator was 98% for segmentation rules, 99% for word-level codes, and 98% for coding of reduplications and reformulations (i.e., mazes). To determine transcription accuracy and coding agreement, 10% of the narratives were independently transcribed and coded for the NSS and SI by a second research assistant. Transcription accuracy was calculated by comparing the in- dependent transcripts at the word and morpheme level (94% agreement), utterance segmentation decisions (98% agree- ment), placement of mazes (93% agreement), and utterance types (100% agreement). Calculating agreement for the two coding schemes pre- sented a greater challenge. Simple interrater agreement scores can be misleading, as small differences between coders (e.g., NSS scores of 24 and 25) are treated the same as large dif- ferences between coders (e.g., NSS scores of 16 and 28). Therefore, agreement for the NSS and SI coding was calcu- lated using Krippendorff’s alpha, which accounted for both chance agreement and the degree of difference between tran- scribers (Krippendorff, 1980). Alpha values accounting for differences in ordinal data were calculated using the summed NSS and SI scores that were calculated by the two independent transcribers (a = .92 for SI; a = .79 for NSS). Krippendorff established benchmarks for alpha values, with ≥.80 reflecting adequate agreement and values between .67 and .80 reflect- ing acceptable agreement for exploratory research and draw-
  • 248. ing tentative conclusions. For a review of the accuracy and agreement process and a further discussion of Krippendorff’s alpha, see Heilmann et al. (2008). Language Sample Measures To test the relationship between microstructural measures and the NSS, we used the following language sample mea- sures that repeatedly have been found to be robust and devel- opmentally sensitive to the population and context used in the present study (i.e., typically developing 5–7-year-olds who produced oral narratives): Length/productivity. Number of total words (NTW) is a measure of productivity that documents the amount of infor- mation provided in the story (Allen, Kertoy, Sherblom, & Pettit, 1994; Paul & Smith, 1993). In addition, NTW was used to assist with statistically controlling sample length, which has the potential to affect additional language sample measures. Vocabulary. Number of different words (NDW) is a mea- sure of lexical diversity that provides a robust estimate of children’s productive vocabulary (Klee, 1992; Miller, 1987; Miller & Klee, 1995) and has been widely used as an index of 158 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19 • 154–166 • May 2010 vocabulary skills in studies examining children’s oral nar- rative skills (Gazella & Stockman, 2003; Humphries, Cardy, Worling, & Peets, 2004; Swanson, Fey, Mills, & Hood, 2005; Uccelli & Páez, 2007). Grammar. Mean length of C-unit (MLCU) measures the
  • 249. average number of morphemes that children use per C-unit and is an index of general grammatical skills that increases with age through the school-age years, particularly when analyzed using narratives and expositories (Leadholm & Miller, 1992; Nippold et al., 2005). Also, the SI, a measure of clausal density, indicates the average number of subordinate clauses produced per C-unit. Use of subordinate clauses emerges during the preschool years (Diessel & Tomasello, 2000) and continues to develop through the school-age years (Nippold, 2007). Because the productions of oral narratives were relatively short, issues of sample length required special consideration. For conversational language samples, it is common practice to control sample length across children by using a consistent number of utterances, words, or elapsed time (e.g., 50 utter- ances; Miller, 1981). In the present study, children produced between 12 and 77 utterances. Using a standard transcrip- tion cut of 50 utterances would limit the analyses to 11 tran- scripts. If we chose to maintain 75% of the data, 97 transcripts would remain in the analysis, and each transcript would be cut at 29 utterances and would ultimately withhold 955 utter- ances from the data set. Our goal was to maintain the maxi- mum amount of data available by maintaining the entire sample and to use statistical procedures to account for differences in transcript length. Furthermore, using the entire sample for linguistic analyses, including NDW, is common practice in contemporary studies of children’s oral narratives (Gazella & Stockman, 2003; Humphries et al., 2004; Swanson et al., 2005; Uccelli & Páez, 2007). Results The NSS scores were first reviewed to identify how many child and examiner errors precluded scores to be adminis- tered. In the present study, a total of 903 scores were completed using the children’s narrative transcripts (seven categories were
  • 250. scored across 129 transcripts). Across the 903 sections, only six sections received scores of zero (four stories lacked an in- troduction, and two children omitted a conclusion), and only one of the 903 sections received a score of NA; 99% of the NSS sections were able to be scored correctly, confirming that the training and elicitation procedures facilitated a high level of child compliance and examiner fidelity during the elici- tation process. The skewness statistic was calculated for the NSS to de- termine if there was an unequal distribution of NSS scores across the sample (see Coolican, 2004, for a review). Skew- ness measures of zero indicate a perfectly normal distribution, while skewness values below –0.8 or above 0.8 have been described as “noticeably skewed” (Bourque & Clark, 1992, p. 69). The skewness statistic for the NSS was –0.5, indicat- ing that scores were more concentrated toward the ceiling but were not noticeably skewed according to the criteria established by Bourque and Clark. Descriptive statistics for age, the NSS, and each of the microstructural measures are presented in Table 2. Table 2 also summarizes the bivariate correlations between the NSS, children’s age, and each of the microstructures. All correla- tions were significant and were moderate in strength (Cohen, 1988). To further explore the covariance structure between the variables and to identify unique relationships between the microstructures and the NSS, two separate hierarchical re- gression equations were completed. Hierarchical regressions allow examination of variance that is uniquely explained by a given variable. The first hierarchical regression equation is summarized in Table 3. Sample length was first controlled by entering NTW into Model 1. Length was controlled because
  • 251. measures of lexical diversity are inevitably influenced by the NTW in the sample (Malvern & Richards, 2002). That is, the more total words that a child produces, the more opportunity he or she has to produce different words. Model 2 identified the unique relationship between vocab- ulary and NSS after controlling for length. Taken together, NTWand NDW were significantly correlated with NSS scores (r = .58). Adding NDW in Model 2 increased the explained variance from 24% to 33%, a net increase of 9%. A one-way analysis of variance was completed to determine whether the increase in explained variance was significant, and an f 2 sta- tistic was calculated to estimate its effect size. According to Cohen (1988), effect sizes of 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35 are con- sidered small, medium, and large, respectively. The 9% increase in explained variance was significant, F(1, 124) = 16.5, p < .001, f 2 = 0.12, demonstrating that NDW was uniquely TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics for language sample measures and correlations with the narrative scoring scheme (NSS). Measure M SD Range Correlation with NSS Age 6.0 0.7 5.0–7.0 .30* Narrative macrostructure (NSS) 19.0 3.0 11.0–26.0 — Productivity (NTW) 264.1 77.2 133.0–608.0 .50** Vocabulary (NDW) 92.5 19.0 47.0–150.0 .58** Grammar (MLCU) 7.0 0.9 4.8–9.6 .44** Grammar (SI) 1.1 0.1 1.0–1.3 .35**
  • 252. Note. NTW = number of total words; NDW = number of different words; MLCU = mean length of C-unit; SI = subordination index. *p = .001. **p < .001. TABLE 3. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis with grammatical measures uniquely predicting NSS scores in Model 3. Model Predictors r Adjusted r2 r2 change 1 Productivity (NTW) .50 .24* 2 Productivity (NTW) .58 .33* .09* Vocabulary (NDW) 3 Productivity (NTW) .61 .35* .02 Vocabulary (NDW) Grammar (MLCU, SI) *Significant at p < .01. Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 159 related to children’s NSS scores above and beyond the vari- ability explained by NTW. Model 3 was completed to identify if there was a unique relationship between the two grammatical measures (MLCU and SI) and NSS scores, after controlling for NTW and NDW. The measures in this final model were significantly correlated with the NSS (r = .61). Adding MLCU and SI to the third
  • 253. regression equation added an additional 2% prediction to NSS scores, which was not significant, F(2, 122) = 2.9, p = .060, f 2 = 0.03. In this first hierarchical regression analysis, NDW uniquely predicted NSS scores after controlling for length using NTW. The two grammatical measures, however, did not add unique prediction of children’s NSS scores. To con- firm that NDW was the major unique predictor of NSS scores, a second hierarchical regression equation was completed and is summarized in Table 4. Again, sample length was controlled by entering NTW in the first model. The two grammatical measures were next entered in the second equation, which resulted in a combined correlation of r = .57. Adding SI and MLCU to NTW increased the explained variance from .24 to .30, documenting that the grammatical measures explain 6% of the variance in NSS after controlling for sample length. This increase was significant, F(2, 123) = 6.0, p = .002, f 2 = 0.08. NDW was added to the third model to test the unique prediction of vocabulary on NSS scores. Adding NDW in the third model resulted in a 5% increase in explained variance between the microstructures and NSS, which was significant, F(1, 122) = 8.7, p = .003, f 2 = 0.06. In sum, this series of hierarchical regression equations documented that children’s use of vocabulary is the major significant and unique microstructural variable in predicting their story organization skills as measured by the NSS. Chil- dren’s productive grammar, while significantly correlated with NSS scores, did not provide unique prediction of the children’s narrative macrostructure ability. Discussion Upon establishing a set of reference databases for chil- dren’s narrative retells, the Language Analysis Lab at the University of Wisconsin—Madison set out to identify a clini- cally useful measure of children’s narrative organization skills.
  • 254. The NSS was created to bring together the benefits of con- crete scoring criteria combined with judgment of text-level constructs. The NSS also incorporated higher level narrative components, including cohesive markers and measures of literate language, to measure a wider range of skills than tradi- tional story grammar analyses. To determine whether the NSS was developmentally ap- propriate for the children in this study, the skewness statistic was calculated. This analysis revealed that the data were not noticeably skewed according to Borque and Clark’s (1992) criteria and that the NSS appeared to be a sensitive measure for school-age children who produced an oral retell. (We are cur- rently completing a more thorough investigation of the de- velopmental sensitivity of the NSS as compared to alternative methods of documenting narrative macrostructure skills.) To document the relationship between the microstructural features of language samples and children’s performance on the NSS, a series of correlation and hierarchical regression analyses were completed. These analyses confirmed that a close relationship existed between children’s productivity, vocab- ulary, grammar, and narrative macrostructure skills. The correlation analyses documented that age and each of the microstructural measures (NTW, NDW, MLCU, and SI) were significantly correlated with children’s narrative organi- zation skills. It is noteworthy that the correlation between age and NSS was the weakest observed correlation. The socio- cultural theory of narrative macrostructure development pro- poses that children who have more experience with stories will have greater narrative competence (e.g., Eaton et al., 1999; Stein & Glenn, 1979). The children in the present sample spanned 2 years in age. The older children in this sample likely had more experience listening to and telling stories. How- ever, it was the children’s vocabulary and grammar skills that
  • 255. were most strongly related to their narrative macrostructure scores. While this study did not directly control for the amount of experience the children had with narratives, the data pro- vide some additional evidence for the importance of chil- dren’s linguistic proficiency in predicting narrative organization skills (Berman & Slobin, 1994; Bishop & Donlan, 2005). Two separate hierarchical regression analyses were com- pleted to identify the unique relationships between each of the microstructures and NSS scores. After controlling for length, the unique relationship between the NSS and mea- sures of vocabulary and grammar was calculated. The anal- yses showed that children’s productive vocabulary skills were the only unique predictor of narrative organization skills. Grammatical measures, on the other hand, provided no unique prediction of NSS scores. The unique importance of vocab- ulary in predicting narrative organization skills was a novel finding. Bishop and Donlan (2005) documented that children’s use of complex syntax and expression of causal concepts uniquely predicted children’s ability to organize their oral narratives. Bishop and Donlan examined children between 7 and 9 years of age, while the present study investigated children age 5–7 years. The children in the present study were using minimal complex syntax. As observed in Table 2, SI values averaged 1.1, showing that children produce approx- imately one subordinate clause every 10 utterances. The children’s use of subordination may have been influenced by the story used in this study; the children’s SI values, on aver- age, were just slightly lower than the SI value from the story script (SI = 1.15). These low levels of subordination could explain, in part, the modest correlations and lack of a unique relationship between the grammatical measures and the NSS. TABLE 4. Summary of hierarchical regression analysis with vocabulary measures uniquely predicting NSS scores in Model 3.
  • 256. Model Predictors r Adjusted r2 r2 change 1 Productivity (NTW) .50 .24* 2 Productivity (NTW) .57 .30* .06* Grammar (MLCU, SI) 3 Productivity (NTW) .61 .35* .05* Grammar (MLCU, SI) Vocabulary (NDW) *Significant at p < .01. 160 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19 • 154–166 • May 2010 Relationship Between Vocabulary and the NSS The present study revealed that there is a special and im- portant relationship between narrative organization and vo- cabulary skills that emerges prior to children becoming fully literate. This study demonstrated that the development of story schema and vocabulary acquisition is developing along a similar path. The importance of vocabulary in narrative organization skills is not surprising given the literature de- scribing the development of narrative form. Preschool and young school-age children typically produce narratives that simply chain sequences of events in temporal order (Berman, 1988) and provide simple descriptive sequences (Stein & Glenn, 1979). It is not until the later school-age years that children hierarchically organize the events in their narrative productions (Berman, 1988) and take multiple perspectives to relate the events (Stein & Glenn, 1979). To produce these
  • 257. more advanced narratives, children must use complex syntax (Bamberg & Damrad-Frye, 1991; Bishop & Donlan, 2005). Before children are proficient in using complex syntax, they likely have to rely on their vocabulary skills to organize their narrative productions. Furthermore, the emerging literacy literature has documented the importance of vocabulary in the development of children’s narrative and comprehension skills. There is a well-documented relationship between children’s vocabulary skills and reading comprehension (see Scarborough, 2001). Furthermore, preschool and young school-age chil- dren acquire new vocabulary through repeated exposures to narrative form (Robbins & Ehri, 1994; Senechal & Cornell, 1993) and by receiving adult scaffolding that highlights the story’s structure (Hargrave & Senechal, 2000; Penno, Wilkinson, & Moore, 2002). Limitations The results from the hierarchical regression equations could have been affected by the sampling context. Because all of the measures were collected from a single language sam- ple, high internal validity was achieved. While each of the language sample measures theoretically and empirically re- flects its respective linguistic domain, strong intercorrelations between language sample measures have been observed (Miller & Klee, 1995). The hierarchical regression equations did af- ford better understanding of the covariance structure. How- ever, vocabulary and grammatical measures acquired from alternate tasks may provide a more informative test of the relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and narrative macrostructure. Clinical Implications The NSS was created as a clinically useful comprehensive narrative macrostructure measure. To be clinically feasible,
  • 258. an assessment tool must be able to be completed in a short amount of time to accommodate the busy schedule of the SLP. In our lab, trained transcribers could complete the NSS using a narrative transcript in approximately 3 min. In addition to efficient scoring, the NSS was developed to facilitate accurate scoring both within and between examiners. The Krippen- dorff alpha analyses revealed that the NSS had lower inter- rater agreement than we would ideally observe and that the NSS alpha was not as high as the alpha for the SI. The dif- ference in alphas between the NSS and SI was not surprising, however, as the NSS requires much greater individual judg- ment when compared to the relatively straightforward scoring rules for the SI. However, recall that these subjective ratings are often the most sensitive when identifying children with language impairment (e.g., McFadden & Gillam, 1996). Our goal is to continue developing new training methods and complete additional research identifying ways to increase coding accuracy for the NSS and other macrostructural anal- yses of children’s natural language use. Upon scoring the NSS, clinicians are provided with a clini- cally useful benchmark for children’s overall narrative profi- ciency. The data described in this study used the composite NSS score, which was the summed score for all seven sec- tions of the NSS. We proposed that the NSS composite score provided an index of children’s overall narrative organization skills. The NSS data described in this study are available as part of the SALT Narrative Story Retell database and can be downloaded free of charge from the SALT Web site. SALT software, used to access the data, can compare a target child’s NSS scores with those of age-matched peers. In addition to comparing NSS scores to the SALT database, composite NSS scores can be useful for monitoring progress and documenting treatment outcomes by collecting multiple narrative samples from a child and documenting changes in NSS scores over
  • 259. time. The NSS also provides examiners with the opportunity to identify specific aspects of the narrative process that are dif- ficult for a child. Because the NSS separately judges seven aspects of the narrative process, examiners can evaluate per- formance on each section of the NSS to identify areas of strength and areas that require intervention. Compared with narrative macrostructure measures that make holistic text- level judgments of narrative proficiency, there is greater spe- cificity in the NSS scoring procedure. Having a detailed narrative performance profile facilitates a more accurate de- scription of the child’s performance and can assist in the development of treatment goals. For example, a child who performed poorly on the referencing and cohesion sections of the NSS but did well on the other sections likely has difficulty using cohesive devices. Treatment goals could include im- proving the child’s use of referential, lexical, and conjunctive cohesion. Increasing our understanding of the relationships between vocabulary, grammar, and narrative macrostructure has im- portant clinical implications for documenting functional out- comes and identifying treatment goals. One goal of language intervention programs is for the therapy tasks to generalize to functional tasks. Telling a well-formed narrative is a func- tional task that is important to children’s academic success (Bishop & Edmundson, 1987; Griffin, Hemphill, Camp, & Wolf, 2004; O’Neill, Pearce, & Pick, 2004). The data from the present study demonstrated that vocabulary skills were uniquely related to children’s story organization skills. Therefore, a treatment procedure that increases a child’s vocabulary skills would have broader, more functional outcomes if concurrent increases in his or her narrative macrostructure skills were documented. Similarly, we may expect that treatments ad- dressing narrative macrostructure skills could also result in
  • 260. Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 161 concurrent increases in vocabulary skills. Clearer understand- ing of the relationship between microstructures and macro- structures will facilitate selection of appropriate treatment goals. Understanding these relationships can aid in identifying the appropriate microstructures to address when implement- ing interventions that focus on linguistic macrostructures, such as narrative organization. It is important to note that the data in this study are purely correlational and that further research is needed before causal relationships between vocabulary, grammar, and narrative macrostructure can be identified. In sum, narrative language assessment is an effective method for documenting children’s language skills. The NSS was developed by the Language Analysis Lab as a clinically useful index of children’s narrative organization skills. Given its clinical feasibility and its robust relationship with other linguistic measures, the NSS provides clinicians and re- searchers with an additional tool to document children’s global language skills using a functional and curriculum-oriented task. Acknowledgments This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 5 T32 DC005459 (“Interdisciplinary Research Training in Speech-Language Disorders”) and by the Language Analysis Lab at the University of Wisconsin—Madison. This work was also made possible through close collaboration with two public school districts
  • 261. in San Diego County: San Diego City Schools and Cajon Valley Union Schools. The authors express appreciation to Karen Andriacchi and the research assistants from the Language Analysis Lab for their assistance with transcription and organization of the data. Portions of this article were presented at the 2006 American Speech- Language- Hearing Association Annual Convention in Miami, FL. References Allen, M., Kertoy, M., Sherblom, J., & Pettit, J. (1994). Chil- dren’s narrative productions: A comparison of personal event and fictional stories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 149–176. Applebee, A. (1978). The child’s concept of story: Ages two to seven. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to pro- vide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competencies. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689– 710. Berman, R. (1988). On the ability to relate events in narrative. Discourse Processes, 11, 469–497. Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (Eds.). (1994). Relating events in nar- rative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bishop, D., & Donlan, C. (2005). The role of syntax in encoding and recall of pictorial narratives: Evidence from specific lan- guage impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychol- ogy, 23, 25–46.
  • 262. Bishop, D., & Edmundson, A. (1987). Language-impaired 4- year- olds: Distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 52, 156–173. Boudreau, D. (2007). Narrative abilities in children with language impairment. In R. Paul (Ed.), Language disorders from a devel- opmental perspective: Essays in honor of Robin S. Chapman (pp. 331–356). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Boudreau, D., & Hedberg, N. (1999). A comparison of early literacy skills in children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. American Journal of Speech- Language Pathology, 8, 249–260. Bourque, L., & Clark, V. (1992). Processing data: The survey example. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Clahsen, H. (1989). The grammatical characterization of develop- mental dysphasia. Linguistics, 27, 897–920. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Colozzo, P., Garcia, R., Megan, C., Gillam, R., & Johnston, J. (2006, June). Narrative assessment in SLI: Exploring interac- tions between content and form. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Symposium on Research in Child
  • 263. Language Disorders, Madison, WI. Coolican, H. (2004). Research methods and statistics in psychol- ogy. London, England: Hodder & Stoughton. Curenton, S., & Justice, L. (2004). African American and Cauca- sian preschoolers’ use of decontextualized language: Literate language features in oral narratives. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 240–253. Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 131–151. Eaton, J., Collis, G., & Lewis, V. (1999). Evaluative explanations in children’s narratives of a video sequence without dialogue. Journal of Child Language, 26, 699–720. Gazella, J., & Stockman, I. (2003). Children’s story retelling under different modality and task conditions: Implications for standardizing language sampling procedures. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 61–72. Gillam, R., & Johnston, J. (1992). Spoken and written language relationships in language/learning-impaired and normally achiev- ing school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1303–1315. Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. (1991). Familial aggregation of a devel-
  • 264. opmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50. Greenhalgh, K., & Strong, C. (2001). Literate language features in spoken narratives of children with typical language and children with language impairments. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 114–125. Griffin, T., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. (2004). Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language, 24, 123–147. Halliday, M., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London, England: Longman. Hargrave, A., & Senechal, M. (2000). A book reading interven- tion with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: The benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading. Early Child- hood Research Quarterly, 15, 75–90. Hedberg, N., & Westby, C. (1993). Analyzing storytelling skills: Theory to practice. Tucson, AZ: Communication Skill Builders. Heilmann, J., Miller, J., Iglesias, A., Fabiano-Smith, L., Nockerts, A., & Andriacchi, K. (2008). Narrative transcription accuracy and reliability in two languages. Topics in Language Disorders, 28, 178–188. Humphries, T., Cardy, J., Worling, D., & Peets, K. (2004). Narrative comprehension and retelling abilities of children with nonverbal learning disabilities. Brain and Cognition, 56, 77–88. Justice, L., Bowles, R., Kaderavek, J., Ukrainetz, T., Eisenberg, S., & Gillam, R. (2006). The Index of Narrative Microstructure:
  • 265. A clinical tool for analyzing school-age children’s narrative performances. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathol- ogy, 15, 177–191. 162 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19 • 154–166 • May 2010 Klee, T. (1992). Developmental and diagnostic characteristics of quantitative measures of children’s language production. Topics in Language Disorders, 12, 28–41. Krippendorff, K. (1980). Content analysis: An introduction to its methodology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral ver- sions of personal experience. In J. Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 12–44). Seattle: University of Washington Press. Leadholm, B., & Miller, J. F. (1992). Language sample analysis: The Wisconsin guide. Madison, WI: Department of Public Instruction. Liles, B. (1985). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and language- disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 123–133. Malvern, D., & Richards, B. (2002). Investigating accommoda- tion in language proficiency interviews using a new measure of lexical diversity. Language Testing, 19, 85–104.
  • 266. Mandler, J., & Johnson, N. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 111–151. Manhardt, J., & Rescorla, L. (2002). Oral narrative skills of late talkers at ages 8 and 9. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 1–21. Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? New York, NY: Dial Press. McCabe, A. (1997). Developmental and cross-cultural aspects of children’s narration. In G. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative develop- ment: Six approaches (pp. 137–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. McFadden, T., & Gillam, R. (1996). An examination of the quality of narratives produced by children with language disorders. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 48–56. Merritt, D., & Liles, B. (1987). Story grammar ability in children with and without language disorder: Story generation, story retelling, and story comprehension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 539–552. Miles,S., & Chapman,R. S.(2002). Narrative content as described by individuals with Down syndrome and typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 175–189. Miller, J. F. (1981). Assessing language production in children. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press. Miller, J. F. (1987). A grammatical characterization of language
  • 267. disorder. In J. Martin, P. Fletcher, R. Grunwell, & D. Hall (Eds.), Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Specific Speech and Language Disorders in Children (pp. 100–113). London, England: Association for All Speech Impaired Children. Miller, J., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & Francis, D. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 30–43. Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2008). Systematic Analysis of Lan- guage Transcripts (Research Version 9.1) [Computer software]. Middleton, WI: SALT Software. Miller, J., & Klee, T. (1995). Computational approaches to the anal- ysis of language impairment. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 545–572). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Nippold, M. (2007). Later language development: School-age children, adolescents, and young adults (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Nippold, M., Hesketh, L., Duthie, J., & Mansfield, T. (2005). Conversational versus expository discourse: A study of syntactic development in children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1048–1064. O’Neill, D., Pearce, M., & Pick, J. (2004). Preschool children’s narratives and performance on the Peabody Individualized Achievement Test—Revised: Evidence of a relation between
  • 268. early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language, 24, 149–183. Paul, R., & Smith, R. (1993). Narrative skills in 4-year-olds with normal, impaired, and late-developing language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 592–598. Pearce, W., McCormack, P., & James, D. (2003). Exploring the boundaries of SLI: Findings from morphosyntactic and story grammar analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17, 325–334. Pelligrini, A., Galda, L., Bartini, M., & Charak, D. (1998). Oral language and literacy learning in context: The role of social relationships. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 38–54. Penno, J., Wilkinson, I., & Moore, D. (2002). Vocabulary acquisi- tion from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 23–33. Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholin- guistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York, NY: Plenum Press. Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (2004). Echoing our parents: Parental influences on children’s narration. In M. Pratt & B. Fiese (Eds.), Family stories and the life course: Across time and generations (pp. 27–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Quasthoff, U. (1997a). An interactive approach to narrative
  • 269. devel- opment. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp. 51–83). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Quasthoff, U. (1997b). Were you ever in a situation where you were in serious danger of being killed? Narrator-listener inter- action in Labov and Waletzky’s narratives. Journal of Narrative & Life History, 7, 121–128. Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). Frog, Where are you? Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury and Williams syndrome. Brain & Language, 88, 229–247. Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863. Robbins, C., & Ehri, L. (1994). Reading storybooks to kinder- garteners helps them learn new vocabulary words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 56–64. Rumelhart, D. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. G. Bobrow & A. M. Collins (Eds.), Representation and under- standing: Studies in cognitive science (pp. 211–236). New York, NY: Academic Press. Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. Neuman & D. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook for research in early literacy (pp. 97–110). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Schuele, C., & Tolbert, L. (2001). Omissions of obligatory relative
  • 270. markers in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 15, 257–274. Scott, C., & Stokes, S. (1995). Measures of syntax in school-age children and adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 26, 309–319. Senechal,M.,&Cornell,E.(1993). Vocabulary acquisition through shared reading experiences. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 360–374. Slobin, D. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In C. Ferguson & D. Slobin (Eds.), Studies of child language development (pp. 175–208). New York, NY: Aca- demic Press. Stein, N. L. (1988). The development of storytelling skill. In M. Franklin & S. Barten (Eds.), Child language: A reader (pp. 282–297). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis for story comprehen- sion in elementary school. In R. Freedle (Ed.), New directions Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 163 in discourse processing (Vol. 2, pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Strong, C. (1998). The Strong Narrative Assessment Procedure. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications. Strong, C., & Shaver, J. (1991). Stability of cohesion in the
  • 271. spoken narratives of language-impaired and normally developing school- aged children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 95–111. Swanson, L., Fey, M., Mills, C., & Hood, L. (2005). Use of narrative-based language intervention with children who have specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech- Language Pathology, 14, 131–143. Templin, M. (1957). Certain language skills in children: Their development and interrelationships. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Uccelli, P., & Páez, M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary devel- opment of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade: Developmental changes and associations among English and Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 225–236. van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cement of the universe: The development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In P. van den Broek, P. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension: Bridging fic- tional and actual events (pp. 321–342). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Westby, C. (2005). Assessing and facilitating text comprehension problems. In H. Catts & A. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (pp. 157–232). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Wigglesworth, G. (1997). Children’s individual approaches to the organization of narrative. Journal of Child Language, 24, 279–309.
  • 272. Received March 24, 2008 Revision received December 1, 2008 Accepted November 30, 2009 DOI: 10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0024) Contact author: John Heilmann, East Carolina University, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Health Sciences Building, Room 3310T, Greenville, NC 27858-4353. E-mail: [email protected] 164 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19 • 154–166 • May 2010 Appendix (p. 1 of 2) The Narrative Scoring Scheme Characteristic Proficient Emerging Minimal/immature Introduction Setting Setting -Child launches into story with no attempt to provide the setting. -Child states general place and provides some detail about the setting (e.g., reference to the time of the setting— daytime, bedtime, or season). -Child states general setting but provides no detail. -Setting elements are stated at appropriate place in story.
  • 273. -Description or elements of story are given intermittently through story. Characters -Child may provide description of specific element of setting (e.g., the frog is in the jar). -Main characters are introduced with some description or detail provided. OR Characters -Characters of story are mentioned with no detail or description. Character development -Main character(s) and all supporting character(s) are mentioned. -Both main and active supporting characters are mentioned. -Inconsistent mention is made of involved or active characters. -Throughout story it is clear that child can discriminate between main and supporting characters (e.g., more description of and emphasis on
  • 274. main character[s]). -Main characters are not clearly distinguished from supporting characters. -Characters necessary for advancing the plot are not present. -Child narrates in first person using character voice (e.g., “You get out of my tree,” said the owl). Mental states -Mental states of main and supporting characters are expressed when necessary for plot development and advancement. -Some mental state words are used to develop character(s). No use is made of mental state words to develop characters. -A variety of mental state words are used. -A limited number of mental state words are used inconsistently throughout the story. Referencing -Child provides necessary antecedents to pronouns. -Referents/antecedents are used inconsistently.
  • 275. -Pronouns are used excessively. -References are clear throughout story. -No verbal clarifiers are used. -Child is unaware listener is confused. Conflict resolution -Child clearly states all conflicts and resolutions critical to advancing the plot of the story. -Description of conflicts and resolutions critical to advancing the plot of the story is underdeveloped. -Random resolution is stated with no mention of cause or conflict. OR OR -Not all conflicts and resolutions critical to advancing the plot are present. -Conflict is mentioned without resolution. OR -Many conflicts and resolutions
  • 276. critical to advancing the plot are not present. Cohesion -Events follow a logical order. -Events follow a logical order. -No use is made of smooth transitions.-Critical events are included, while less emphasis is placed on minor events. -Excessive detail or emphasis provided on minor events leads the listener astray.-Smooth transitions are provided between events. OR -Transitions to next event are unclear. OR -Minimal detail is given for critical events. OR -Equal emphasis is placed on all events. Heilmann et al.: Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme 165 Appendix (p. 1 of 2)? (p. NaN ) The Narrative Scoring Scheme Characteristic Proficient Emerging Minimal/immature Conclusion -Story is clearly wrapped up using general concluding statements such as “and they were together again happy as could be.” -Specific event is concluded, but no
  • 277. general statement is made as to the conclusion of the whole story. -Child stops narrating, and listener may need to ask if that is the end. Scoring: Each characteristic receives a scaled score of 0–5. Proficient characteristics = 5; Emerging = 3; Minimal/immature = 1. Scores between (i.e., 2 and 4) are undefined; use judgment. Scores of zero and NA are defined below. A composite is scored by adding the total of the characteristic scores. Highest score = 35. A score of zero is given for child errors (such as telling the wrong story, conversing with examiner, not completing/refusing task, using wrong language and creating inability of scorer to comprehend story in target language, abandoned utterances, unintelligibility, poor performance, or components of rubric are in imitation-only). A score of NA (nonapplicable) is given for mechanical/examiner/operator errors (such as interference from background noise, issues with recording such as cut-offs or interruptions, examiner quitting before child does, examiner not following protocol, or examiner asking overly specific or leading questions rather than open-ended questions or prompts). Appendix (p. 2 of 2) The Narrative Scoring Scheme 166 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 19
  • 278. • 154–166 • May 2010 Copyright of American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology is the property of American Speech-Language- Hearing Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.