1. Toward Braincomputer Interfacing 1st Edition
Guido Dornhege download
https://ebookbell.com/product/toward-braincomputer-
interfacing-1st-edition-guido-dornhege-4149536
Explore and download more ebooks at ebookbell.com
2. Here are some recommended products that we believe you will be
interested in. You can click the link to download.
Toward Braincomputer Interaction In Paralysis A New Approach Based On
Visual Evoked Potentials And Depthoffield 1st Edition Anibal Cotrina
Auth
https://ebookbell.com/product/toward-braincomputer-interaction-in-
paralysis-a-new-approach-based-on-visual-evoked-potentials-and-
depthoffield-1st-edition-anibal-cotrina-auth-5882504
Towards Practical Braincomputer Interfaces Bridging The Gap From
Research To Realworld Applications 1st Edition Brendan Z Allison
https://ebookbell.com/product/towards-practical-braincomputer-
interfaces-bridging-the-gap-from-research-to-realworld-
applications-1st-edition-brendan-z-allison-4404600
Raveling The Brain Toward A Transdisciplinary Neurorhetoric 1st
Edition Jordynn Jack
https://ebookbell.com/product/raveling-the-brain-toward-a-
transdisciplinary-neurorhetoric-1st-edition-jordynn-jack-23894914
Zen And The Brain Toward An Understand Of Meditation And Consciousness
Austin
https://ebookbell.com/product/zen-and-the-brain-toward-an-understand-
of-meditation-and-consciousness-austin-11640830
3. Zenbrain Horizons Toward A Living Zen James H Austin Md
https://ebookbell.com/product/zenbrain-horizons-toward-a-living-zen-
james-h-austin-md-56401730
Mindbraingene Toward Psychotherapy Integration Hardcover John Arden
https://ebookbell.com/product/mindbraingene-toward-psychotherapy-
integration-hardcover-john-arden-7347546
Theodore W Berger Dennis L Glanzman Toward Replacement Parts For The
Brain Implantable Biomimetic Electronics As Neural Prostheses
https://ebookbell.com/product/theodore-w-berger-dennis-l-glanzman-
toward-replacement-parts-for-the-brain-implantable-biomimetic-
electronics-as-neural-prostheses-56389312
Neurobiology Of Social Behavior Toward An Understanding Of The
Prosocial And Antisocial Brain 1st Edition Michael Numan
https://ebookbell.com/product/neurobiology-of-social-behavior-toward-
an-understanding-of-the-prosocial-and-antisocial-brain-1st-edition-
michael-numan-5138316
Chronic Medical Disease And Cognitive Aging Toward A Healthy Body And
Brain Kristine Yaffe
https://ebookbell.com/product/chronic-medical-disease-and-cognitive-
aging-toward-a-healthy-body-and-brain-kristine-yaffe-51268884
6. Neural Information Processing Series
Michael I. Jordan and Thomas Dietterich, editors
Advances in Large Margin Classifiers, Alexander J. Smola, Peter L. Bartlett, Bernhard
Schölkopf, and Dale Schuurmans, eds., 2000
Advanced Mean Field Methods: Theory and Practice, Manfred Opper and David Saad,
eds., 2001
Probabilistic Models of the Brain: Perception and Neural Function, Rajesh P. N. Rao,
Bruno A. Olshausen, and Michael S. Lewicki, eds., 2002
Exploratory Analysis and Data Modeling in Functional Neuroimaging, Friedrich T. Som-
mer and Andrzej Wichert, eds., 2003
Advances in Minimum Description Length: Theory and Applications, Peter D. Grunwald,
In Jae Myung, and Mark A. Pitt, eds., 2005
Nearest-Neighbor Methods in Learning and Vision: Theory and Practice, Gregory
Shakhnarovich, Piotr Indyk, and Trevor Darrell, eds., 2006
New Directions in Statistical Signal Processing: From Systems to Brains, Simon Haykin,
José C. Prı́ncipe, Terrence J. Sejnowski, and John McWhirter, eds., 2007
Predicting Structured Data, Gökhan Bakir, Thomas Hofmann, Bernard Schölkopf, Alexan-
der J. Smola, Ben Taskar, S. V. N. Vishwanathan, eds., 2007
Toward Brain-Computer Interfacing, Guido Dornhege, José del R. Millán, Thilo Hinter-
berger, Dennis J. McFarland, Klaus-Robert Müller, eds., 2007
Large Scale Kernel Machines, Léon Bottou, Olivier Chapelle, Denis DeCoste, Jason West-
man, eds., 2007
7. Toward Brain-Computer Interfacing
edited by
Guido Dornhege
José del R. Millán
Thilo Hinterberger
Dennis J. McFarland
Klaus-Robert Müller
foreword by
Terrence J. Sejnowski
A Bradford Book
The MIT Press
Cambridge, Massachusetts
London, England
8. c
2007 Massachusetts Institute of Technology
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form by any electronic or mechanical means
(including photocopying, recording, or information storage and retrieval) without permission in writing from the
publisher.
For information about special quantity discounts, please email special sales@mitpress.mit.edu.
This book was set in LaTex by the authors.
Printed and bound in the United States of America.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Toward brain-computer interfacing / edited by Guido Dornhege ... [et al.] ; foreword by Terrence J. Sejnowski.
p.; cm. – (Neural information processing series)
”A Bradford book.”
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN 978-0-262-04244-4 (hardcover : alk. paper)
1. Brain-computer interfaces. I. Dornhege, Guido. II. Series.
[DNLM: 1. Brain Mapping. 2. User-Computer Interface. 3. Brain–physiology. 4. Psychomotor Performance.
5. Rehabilitation–instrumentation. WL 335 T737 2007]
QP360.7.T69 2007
612.8’2–dc22
2007000517
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
9. Contents
Foreword . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix
Terrence J. Sejnowski
Preface . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xi
1 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Andrea Kübler and Klaus-Robert Müller
I BCI Systems and Approaches 27
2 Noninvasive Brain-Computer Interface Research at the Wadsworth Center 31
Eric W. Sellers, Dean J. Krusienski, Dennis J. McFarland,
and Jonathan R. Wolpaw
3 Brain-Computer Interfaces for Communication in Paralysis: A Clinical
Experimental Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
Thilo Hinterberger, Femke Nijboer, Andrea Kübler,
Tamara Matuz, Adrian Furdea, Ursula Mochty, Miguel Jordan,
Thomas Navin Lal, N. Jeremy Hill, Jürgen Mellinger,
Michael Bensch, Michael Tangermann, Guido Widman,
Christian E. Elger, Wolfgang Rosenstiel, Bernhard
Schölkopf, and Niels Birbaumer
4 Graz-Brain-Computer Interface: State of Research . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
Gert Pfurtscheller, Gernot R. Müller-Putz, Alois Schlögl,
Bernhard Graimann, Reinhold Scherer, Robert Leeb,
Clemens Brunner, Claudia Keinrath, George Townsend,
Carmen Vidaurre, Muhammad Naeem, Felix Y. Lee,
Selina Wriessnegger, Doris Zimmermann, Eva Höfler,
and Christa Neuper
10. vi Contents
5 The Berlin Brain-Computer Interface: Machine Learning-Based Detection
of User Specific Brain States . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
Benjamin Blankertz, Guido Dornhege, Matthias Krauledat,
Volker Kunzmann, Florian Losch, Gabriel Curio,
and Klaus-Robert Müller
6 The IDIAP Brain-Computer Interface: An Asynchronous Multiclass
Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
José del R. Millán, Pierre W. Ferrez, and Anna Buttfield
7 Brain Interface Design for Asynchronous Control . . . . . . . . . . . . . 111
Jaimie F. Borisoff, Steve G. Mason, and Gary E. Birch
II Invasive BCI Approaches 123
8 Electrocorticogram as a Brain-Computer Interface Signal Source . . . . 129
Jane E. Huggins, Bernhard Graimann, Se Young Chun,
Jeffrey A. Fessler, and Simon P. Levine
9 Probabilistically Modeling and Decoding Neural Population Activity in
Motor Cortex . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147
Michael J. Black and John P. Donoghue
10 The Importance of Online Error Correction and Feed-Forward Adjustments
in Brain-Machine Interfaces for Restoration of Movement . . . . . . . . . 161
Dawn M. Taylor
11 Advances in Cognitive Neural Prosthesis: Recognition of Neural Data with
an Information-Theoretic Objective . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 175
Zoran Nenadic, Daniel S. Rizzuto, Richard A. Andersen,
and Joel W. Burdick
12 A Temporal Kernel-Based Model for Tracking Hand Movements from
Neural Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 191
Lavi Shpigelman, Koby Crammer, Rony Paz, Eilon Vaadia,
and Yoram Singer
III BCI Techniques 203
13 General Signal Processing and Machine Learning Tools for BCI Analysis 207
Guido Dornhege, Matthias Krauledat, Klaus-Robert Müller,
and Benjamin Blankertz
11. Contents vii
14 Classifying Event-Related Desynchronization in EEG, ECoG, and MEG
Signals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 235
N. Jeremy Hill, Thomas Navin Lal, Michael Tangermann,
Thilo Hinterberger, Guido Widman, Christian E. Elger,
Bernhard Schölkopf, and Niels Birbaumer
15 Classification of Time-Embedded EEG Using Short-Time Principal
Component Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 261
Charles W. Anderson, Michael J. Kirby, Douglas R. Hundley,
and James N. Knight
16 Noninvasive Estimates of Local Field Potentials for Brain-Computer
Interfaces . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 279
Rolando Grave de Peralta Menendez, Sara Gonzalez Andino,
Pierre W. Ferrez, and José del R. Millán
17 Error-Related EEG Potentials in Brain-Computer Interfaces . . . . . . . 291
Pierre W. Ferrez and José del R. Millán
18 Adaptation in Brain-Computer Interfaces . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 303
José del R. Millán, Anna Buttfield, Carmen Vidaurre,
Matthias Krauledat, Alois Schlögl, Pradeep Shenoy,
Benjamin Blankertz, Rajesh P. N. Rao, Rafael Cabeza,
Gert Pfurtscheller, and Klaus-Robert Müller
19 Evaluation Criteria for BCI Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 327
Alois Schlögl, Julien Kronegg, Jane E. Huggins, and Steve G. Mason
IV BCI Software 343
20 BioSig: An Open-Source Software Library for BCI Research . . . . . . . 347
Alois Schlögl, Clemens Brunner, Reinhold Scherer,
and Andreas Glatz
21 BCI2000: A General-Purpose Software Platform for BCI . . . . . . . . . 359
Jürgen Mellinger and Gerwin Schalk
V Applications 369
22 Brain-Computer Interfaces for Communication and Motor
Control—Perspectives on Clinical Applications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 373
Andrea Kübler, Femke Nijboer, and Niels Birbaumer
12. viii Contents
23 Combining BCI and Virtual Reality: Scouting Virtual Worlds . . . . . . 393
Robert Leeb, Reinhold Scherer, Doron Friedman,
Felix Y. Lee, Claudia Keinrath, Horst Bischof,
Mel Slater, and Gert Pfurtscheller
24 Improving Human Performance in a Real Operating Environment through
Real-Time Mental Workload Detection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 409
Jens Kohlmorgen, Guido Dornhege, Mikio L. Braun,
Benjamin Blankertz, Klaus-Robert Müller, Gabriel Curio,
Konrad Hagemann, Andreas Bruns, Michael Schrauf,
and Wilhelm E. Kincses
25 Single-Trial Analysis of EEG during Rapid Visual Discrimination: Enabling
Cortically Coupled Computer Vision . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 423
Paul Sajda, Adam D. Gerson, Marios G. Philiastides,
and Lucas C. Parra
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 441
Contributors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 491
Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 503
13. Foreword
The advances in brain-computer interfaces in this book could have far-reaching conse-
quences for how we interact with the world around us. A communications channel that
bypasses the normal motor outflow from the brain will have an immediate benefit for
paraplegic patients. Someday the same technology will allow humans to remotely con-
trol agents in exotic environments, which will open new frontiers that we can only dimly
imagine today.
The earliest systems to be developed were based on noninvasive electroencephalo-
graphic (EEG) recordings. Because these systems do not require invasive surgical implants,
they can be used for a wide range of applications. The disadvantage is the relatively low
rate of signaling that can be achieved. Nonetheless, advances in signal processing tech-
niques and the development of dry electrodes make this an attractive approach.
Three separate research areas have contributed to major advances in invasive brain-
computer interfaces. First, the neural code for motor control was uncovered based on
recordings from single neurons in different cortical areas of alert primates. The second was
the development of mathematical algorithms for converting the train of spikes recorded
from populations of these neurons to an intended action, called the decoding problem.
Third, it was necessary to achieve stable, long-term recordings from small, cortical neurons
in a harsh aqueous environment.
For both invasive and noninvasive BCIs interdisciplinary teams of scientists and engi-
neers needed to work closely together to create successful systems.
Success in brain-computer interfaces has also depended on the remarkable ability of the
brain to adapt to unusual tasks, none more challenging than “mind control” of extracorpo-
real space. We are still at an early stage of development, but the field is moving forward
rapidly and we can confidently expect further advances in the near future.
Terrence J. Sejnowski
La Jolla, CA
14. Preface
The past decade has seen a fast growing interest to develop an effective communication
interface connecting the human brain to a computer, the “brain-computer interface” (BCI).
BCI research follows three major goals: (1) it aims to provide a new communication
channel for patients with severe neuromuscular disabilities bypassing the normal output
pathways, (2) it provides a powerful working tool in computational neuroscience to con-
tribute to a better understanding of the brain, and finally (3)—often overseen—it provides
a generic novel independent communication channel for man-machine interaction, a di-
rection that is at only the very begining of scientific and practical exploration. During a
workshop at the annual Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS) conference, held
in Whistler, Canada, in December 2004, a snapshot of the state of the art in BCI research
was recorded. A variety of people helped in this, especially all the workshop speakers and
attendees who contributed to lively discussions. After the workshop, we decided that it
would be worthwhile to invest some time to have an overview about current BCI research
printed.
We invited all the speakers as well as other researchers to submit papers, which were
integrated into the present collection. Since BCI research has previously not been covered
in an entire book, this call has been widely followed. Thus, the present collection gathers
contributions and expertise from many important research groups in this field, whom we
wholeheartedly thank for all the work they have put into our joint effort. Note, of course,
that since this book is the outcome of a workshop, it cannot cover all groups and it may—
clearly unintentionally—contain some bias.
However, we are confident that this book covers a broad range of present BCI research:
In the first part we are able to present overviews about many important noninvasive (that
is, without implanting electrodes) BCI groups in the world. We have been also able to win
contributions from a few of the most important invasive BCI groups giving an overview
of the current state of the invasive BCI research. These contributions are presented in
the second part. The book is completed by three further parts, namely an overview of
state-of-the-art techniques from machine learning and signal processing to process brain
signals, an overview about existing software packages in BCI research, and some ideas
about applications of BCI research for the real world.
15. xii Preface
It is our hope that this outweighs the shortcomings of the book, most notably the fact
that a collection of chapters can never be as homogeneous as a book conceived by a
single author. We have tried to compensate for this by writing an introductory chapter
(see chapter 1) and prefaces for all five parts of the book. In addition, the contributions
were carefully refereed.
Guido Dornhege, José del R. Millán, Thilo Hinterberger, Dennis J. McFarland, and
Klaus-Robert Müller
Berlin, Martigny, Tübingen, Albany, August 2006
Acknowledgments
Guido Dornhege and Klaus-Robert Müller were funded by BMBF (FKZ 01IBE01A,
16SV2231, and 01GQ0415), EU PASCAL. Guido Dornhege furthermore acknowledges
the support by the Fraunhofer Society for the BCI project. José del R. Millán acknowl-
edges support from the European IST Programme FET Project FP6-003758, the European
Network of Excellence “PASCAL,” and the Swiss National Science Foundation NCCR
“IM2.” Thilo Hinterberger was supported by the German Research Society (DFG, SFB
550, and HI 1254/2-1) and the Samueli Institute, California. Dennis McFarland was sup-
ported by NIH (NICHD (HD30146) and NIBIB/NINDS (EB00856)) and by the James S.
McDonnell Foundation. Klaus-Robert Müller also wishes to thank, for warm hospitality
during his stay in Tübingen at the Max-Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, the
Friedrich Miescher Laboratory and the Department of Psychology at University of Tbin-
gen, where part of this book was written. Klaus-Robert Müller furthermore acknowledges
generous support by the Fraunhofer Society for the BCI endeavor and in particular his
sabbatical project.
Finally, we would like to thank everybody who contributed toward the success of this
book project, in particular to Mel Goldsipe, Suzanne Stradley, Robert Prior, to all chapter
authors, and to the chapter reviewers.
16. 1 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
Andrea Kübler
Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioural Neurobiology
Eberhard-Karls-University Tübingen, Gartenstr. 29
72074 Tübingen, Germany
Klaus-Robert Müller
Fraunhofer–Institute FIRST
Intelligent Data Analysis Group (IDA)
Kekuléstr. 7, 12489 Berlin, Germany
Technical University Berlin
Str. des 17. Juni 135
10 623 Berlin, Germany
1.1 Abstract
We provide a compact overview of invasive and noninvasive brain-computer interfaces
(BCI). This serves as a high-level introduction to an exciting and active field and sets the
scene for the following sections of this book. In particular, the chapter briefly assembles
information on recording methods and introduces the physiological signals that are being
used in BCI paradigms. Furthermore, we review the spectrum from subject training to
machine learning approaches. We expand on clinical and human-machine interface (HMI)
applications for BCI and discuss future directions and open challenges in the BCI field.
1.2 Overview
Translating thoughts into actions without acting physically has always been material of
which dreams and fairytales were made. Recent developments in brain-computer interface
(BCI) technology, however, open the door to making these dreams come true. Brain-
machine interfaces (BMI1) are devices that allow interaction between humans and artificial
devices (for reviews see e.g. Kübler et al. (2001a); Kübler and Neumann (2005); Lebedev
and Nicolelis (2006); Wolpaw et al. (2002)). They rely on continuous, real-time interaction
between living neuronal tissue and artificial effectors.
Computer-brain interfaces2 are designed to restore sensory function, transmit sensory
information to the brain, or stimulate the brain through artificially generated electrical sig-
17. 2 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
nals. Examples of sensory neuroprostheses are the retina implant (e.g. Eckmiller (1997);
Zrenner (2002)) and the cochlear implant, which circumvents the nonfunctioning audi-
tory hair cells of the inner ear by transmitting electrically processed acoustic signals via
implanted stimulation electrodes directly to the acoustic nerve (e.g., Zenner et al. (2000);
Merzenich et al. (1974); Pfingst (2000)). Further, with an implanted stimulating neuropros-
thesis, hyperactivity of the subthalamic nuclei can be inhibited to improve Parkinsonian
symptoms (e.g., Mazzone et al. (2005); Benabid et al. (1991)).
Brain-computer interfaces provide an additional output channel and thus can use the
neuronal activity of the brain to control artificial devices, for example, for restoring
motor function. Neuronal activity of few neurons or large cell assemblies is sampled and
processed in real-time and converted into commands to control an application, such as a
robot arm or a communication program (e.g., Birbaumer et al. (1999); Müller-Putz et al.
(2005b); Taylor et al. (2002); Hochberg et al. (2006); Santhanam et al. (2006); Lebedev and
Nicolelis (2006); Haynes and Rees (2006); Blankertz et al. (2006a); Müller and Blankertz
(2006)).
Brain activity is either recorded intracortically with multielectrode arrays or single
electrodes, epi- or subdurally from the cortex or from the scalp. From the broad band
of neuronal electrical activity, signal detection algorithms filter and denoise the signal of
interest and decoded information is commuted into device commands.
Over the past twenty years, increased BCI research for communication and control has
been driven by a better understanding of brain function, powerful computer equipment, and
by a growing awareness of the needs, problems, and potentials of people with disabilities
(Wolpaw et al. (2002); Kübler et al. (2001a)). In addition to addressing clinical and quality
of life issues, such interfaces constitute powerful tools for basic research on how the
brain coordinates and instantiates human behavior and how new behavior is acquired and
maintained. This is because a BCI offers the unique opportunity to investigate brain activity
as an independent variable. In traditional psychophysiological experiments subjects are
presented with a task or stimuli (independent variables), and the related brain activity
is measured (dependent variable). Conversely, with neurofeedback by means of a BCI,
subjects can learn to deliberately increase or decrease brain activity (independent variable)
and changes in behavior can be measured accordingly (dependent variable). Studies on
regulation of slow cortical potentials, sensorimotor rhythms, and the BOLD response
(see below) yield various specific effects on behavior, such as decreased reaction time
in a motor task after activation of contralateral motor cortex (Rockstroh et al. (1982)),
faster lexical decisions (Pulvermüller et al. (2000)), or improved memory performance as
a function of deactivation of the parahippocampal place area (Weiskopf et al. (2004b)).
In these examples, the link between activation and deactivation of a specific cortical
area and changes in behavior is quite evident. More general effects on learning such as
better musical performance in music students (techniques and subjective interpretation)
and better dancing performance in dance students (technicality, flair, overall execution)
were observed after regularization of alpha and theta activity (Gruzelier and Egner (2005);
Raymond et al. (2005))
An often overlooked direction of BCI applications beyond clinical and basic research
aspects is the yet unexplored use of BCI as an additional independent channel of man-
18. 1.3 Approaches to BCI Control 3
machine interaction (see chapters 23, 24 and 25 for first examples in this direction of
research). In particular, brain signals can provide direct access to aspects of human brain
state such as cognitive workload, alertness, task involvement, emotion, or concentration.
The monitoring of these will allow for a novel technology that directly adapts a man-
machine interface design to the inferred brain state in real-time.
Furthermore, BCI technology can in the near future serve as an add-on when devel-
oping new computer games, for example, fantasy games that require the brain-controlled
mastering of a task for advancing to the next game level.
A variety of technologies for monitoring brain activity may serve as a BCI. In addi-
tion to electroencephalography (EEG) and invasive electrophysiological methods, these in-
clude magnetoencephalography (MEG), positron emission tomography (PET), functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and optical imaging (functional near infrared spec-
troscopy, fNIRS). As MEG, PET, and fMRI are demanding, tied to the laboratory, and
expensive, these technologies are more suitable to address basic research questions and
short-term intervention for location of sources of brain activity and alteration of brain ac-
tivity in diseases with known neurobiological dysfunction. In contrast, EEG, NIRS, and
invasive devices are portable, and thus may offer practical BCIs for communication and
control in daily life.
Current BCIs for human users have been mainly used for cursor control and communi-
cation by means of selection of letters or items on a computer screen (e.g., Birbaumer et al.
(1999); Blankertz et al. (2006a); Hochberg et al. (2006); Obermaier et al. (2003); Wolpaw
and McFarland (2004)). An overview of BCI applications in clinical populations is given
in chapter 22.
Interfaces between machine and the animal brain have been used to control robotic
arms (e.g., Taylor et al. (2002); Wessberg et al. (2000), and for a review, see Lebedev
and Nicolelis (2006)). However, before BCIs can be utilized across a wide range of
clinical or daily life settings, many open technological issues must be resolved. Sensors
are the bottleneck of todays invasive and noninvasive BCIs: invasive sensors can last only
a limited time before they lose signal (Hochberg et al. (2006); Nicolelis et al. (2003)),
and noninvasive sensors need long preparation time due to the use of conductive gel.
More neurobiological and psychological research is necessary to understand the interaction
between neurons and behavior related to the use of BCIs. Already machine learning and
advanced signal processing methods play a key role in BCI research as they allow the
decoding of different brain states within the noise of the spontaneous neural activity in
real-time (see chapters 9, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, and 18). There is, however, a need for
continuous improvement; in particular, higher robustness, online adaptation to compensate
for nonstationarities, sensor fusion strategies, and techniques for transferring classifier or
filter parameters from session to session are among the most burning topics.
1.3 Approaches to BCI Control
Two separate approaches to BCI control exist, while almost all BCIs realize a mix-
ture of both approaches: (1) Learning to voluntarily regulate brain activity by means of
19. 4 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
neurofeedback and operant learning principles. Following subject training, in which the
subject learns to regulate a specific brain activity by means of feedback, different brain
states can be produced on command and, thus, become suitable as control commands. (2)
Machine learning procedures that enable the interference of the statistical signature of spe-
cific brain states or intentions within a calibration session (see chapter 5).
1.3.1 The Biofeedback Approach—Voluntary Control of the Brain Response
Biofeedback is a procedure that, by means of feedback of a (seemingly) autonomous
parameter, aims at acquiring voluntary control over this parameter. Participants receive
visual, auditory, or tactile information about their cardiovascular activity (heartrate, blood
pressure), temperature, skin conductance, muscular activity, electrical brain activity (EEG,
MEG), or the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response (with fMRI). In discrete or
continuous trials, the participants are presented with the task to either increase or decrease
the activity of interest. By means of the feedback signal, participants receive continuous
information about the alteration of the activity. At the end of the trial, participants are
informed about their performance (e.g., by highlighting a correctly hit target) and correct
trials may be positively reinforced by a smiling face (Kübler et al. (1999); see also
chapter 3) or by earning tokens that can be exchanged later for toys (e.g., training of
children with ADHD in first author’s affiliation). If participants are repeatedly trained,
they learn to manipulate the activity of interest, which is then—at least to a certain extent—
under voluntary or conscious (cortical) control.
1.3.2 The Machine Learning Approach—Detection of the Relevant Brain Signal
A somewhat opposite approach3 is the machine learning approach to BCI, where the train-
ing is relocated from the subject to the learning algorithm. Thus, decoding algorithms are
individually adapted to the users that perform the task. For obtaining a qualitative impres-
sion about the variability that is to be compensated, see chapter 13, figures 13.1 and 13.2,
where different individuals perform finger tapping or motor imagery. Note that even the
intraindividual variance between sessions is high. Learning algorithms require examples
from which they can infer the underlying statistical structure of the respective brain state.
Therefore, subjects are first required to repeatedly produce a certain brain state during a
calibration session (e.g., for the BBCI, this calibration session takes approximately twenty
minutes, see chapter 5). Even from such a small amount of data, current learning machines
can extract spatiotemporal blueprints of these brain states, which are readily usable in the
subsequent feedback session. The tackling of the enormous trial-to-trial variability is a ma-
jor challenge in BCI research. We believe that advanced techniques for machine learning
are an essential tool in this endeavor. The use of state-of-the-art learning machines enables
not only the achievement of high decision accuracies for BCI (e.g., chapters 5, 6, 9, 12,
13, and 14), but also, as a by-product of the classification, the few most salient features for
classification are found, which can then be matched with neurophysiological knowledge.
In this sense, machine learning approaches are useful beyond the pure classification or
adaptive spatiotemporal filtering step, as they can contribute to a better interpretation and
20. 1.4 Clinical Target Groups—Individuals in Need of a BCI for Motor Control and Communication 5
understanding of a novel paradigm per se (see Blankertz et al. (2006b)). Thus, machine
learning can be usefully employed in an exploratory scenario, where (1) a new paradigm
is tested that also could generate unexpected neurophysiological signals, (2) a hypothesis
about underlying task relevant brain processes is generated automatically by the learning
machine through feature extraction, and (3) the paradigm can be refined, and thus a better
understanding of the brain processes could be achieved (see figure 13.8). In this sense, a
machine learning method offering explanation can be of great use in the semiautomatic ex-
ploration loop for testing new paradigms. Note that this holds also for data analysis beyond
decoding of brain signals.
1.3.3 Integration of the Two Approaches
The two last paragraphs reflect opposite positions. In practice, BCIs will neither rely solely
on feedback learning of the users nor only on machine learning. For example, in the BBCI
(see chapter 5) that has no explicit user biofeedback training, a user’s brain activity will
adapt to the settings of the decoding algorithm when using the BCI in feedback mode,
such that the most successful EEG activity pattern will be repeatedly produced. Thus, a
coadaptation of the learning user and algorithm occurs inevitably. However, it remains
unclear how to optimally bring these two interacting learning systems into synchrony; a
thorough study is still missing. Experimentally, the two learning systems can be coupled
using online learning (see chapter 18 for discussion).
It is furthermore important to note that in a proportion of the subject population, typically
in 20 percent of the users, one is unable to successfully classify the brain activation
patterns. We refer to this group as the BCI illiterates. This finding holds no matter whether
machine learning or biofeedback is used to train the subjects. Further research is needed to
fully understand and overcome the BCI illiteracy phenomenon.
1.4 Clinical Target Groups—Individuals in Need of a BCI for Motor Control and Communication
A variety of neurological diseases such as motor neuron diseases, spinal cord injury, stroke,
encephalitis, or traumatic brain injury may lead to severe motor paralysis, which may also
include speech. Patients may have only a few muscles to control artificial devices for
communicating their needs and wishes and interacting with their environment. We refer
to the locked-in state if some residual voluntary muscular movement, such as eye or lip
movement, is still possible. People who lost all voluntary muscular movement are referred
to as being in the complete locked-in state (see also chapter 22 and Birbaumer (2006a)). In
the realm of BCI use, it is of particular importance how and how much the brain is affected
by disease. To provide a detailed discussion of all diseases that may lead to the locked-in
syndrome would go beyond the scope of this introduction. Thus, we will refer to only those
diseases that have been repeatedly reported in the BCI literature, that is amyotrophic lateral
sclerosis, high spinal cord injury, and stroke; all three diseases have quite different effects
on the brain.
21. 6 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
1.4.1 Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis
Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) is a progressive neurodegenerative disease involving
the first and second motoneurons and the central nervous system (see also chapter 22).
Patients with ALS show global brain atrophy with regional decreases of grey matter
density being highest in right-hemispheric primary motor cortex and left-hemispheric
medial frontal gyrus (Kassubek et al. (2005)). White matter reduction is found along the
corticospinal tracts, in the corpus callosum, and in frontal and parietal cortices. Clinical
symptoms are atrophic paresis with fasciculations mostly starting in hands and lower arms.
With progressive neuronal degeneration, patients become severely physically impaired. In
later stages of the disease, speech, swallowing, and breathing are also affected. Patients
succumb to respiratory failure unless they choose artificial ventilation via tracheotomy.
Patients with tracheotomy may render the locked-in state with only residual muscular
movement or even the completely locked-in state. Cognitive impairment has been reported
repeatedly (Hanagasi et al. (2002); Ringholz et al. (2005)), but improved learning has
also been shown (Lakerfeld et al. (submitted); Rottig et al. (2006)). Emotional processing
seems to be altered such that positive and negative extremes are attenuated (Lulé et al.
(2005)). Quality of life in ALS patients is surprisingly high and within the range of
patients with nonfatal diseases such as diabetes or irritable bowl syndrome (Kübler et al. (in
preparation)). One important component of individual quality of life repeatedly mentioned
by patients, specifically as the disease progresses, is the ability to communicate.
1.4.2 Cervical Spinal Cord Injury
Most often spinal cord injury follows trauma. It may also occur due to acute ischaemia in
the arteria spinalis-anterior or acute compression. Acute symptoms are spinal shock with
atonic paresis below the lesion, atonic bladder, paralysis of the rectum, disturbed sensitivity
in all qualities (pain, pressure, temperature) and vegetative dysfunction. These symptoms
continue into the post-traumatic phase and are endorsed by painful, involuntary stretching
and bending of extremities (so-called spinal automatisms). Cervical spinal cord injury has
been shown to be accompanied by local cortical grey matter reduction in somatosensory
areas (S1) bilaterally located posterior to the hand region in M1. Atrophy also occurred
in the right leg area and extended to parietal BA5 in the left hemisphere (Jurkiewicz et al.
(2006)). Several years post trauma, patients may be well adapted to a life with impairment,
experience a balanced emotional life, and lead an intact social life. Pain contributes to
poorer quality of life, and gainful employment is related to high quality of life (Lundqvist
et al. (1991)). Clinically relevant symptoms of depression occur specifically in the first year
post injury (Hancock et al. (1993)).
1.4.3 Brain Stem Stroke
The classic locked-in syndrome as defined by Bauer and colleagues is characterized by
total immobility except for vertical eye movement and blinking (Bauer et al. (1979); Smith
and Delargy (2005)). Most often the locked-in syndrome is of cerebrovascular origin such
22. 1.5 Brain-Computer Interfaces for Healthy Subjects 7
that thrombotic occlusion of the arteria basilaris leads to infarction in the ventral pons
(Katz et al. (1992); Patterson and Grabois (1986)). As a result, corticobulbar and cortical
spinal tracts are interrupted as are both the supranuclear and postnuclear oculomotor fibers.
If movements other than vertical eye movement is preserved, the locked-in syndrome is
referred to as incomplete, and if no movement, and thus no communication, is possible as
total (Bauer et al. (1979)). Higher cortical areas or subcortical areas besides the brain stem
are not affected. Consequently, consciousness and cognition are usually unimpaired in such
patients. A survey on quality of life in chronic locked-in patients (more than one year after
diagnosis) with no major motor recovery, revealed no differences to healthy controls in the
perception of mental and general health (Laureys et al. (2005)). In a survey (N = 44) by
Leon-Carrion et al., less than 20 percent of the patients described their mood as bad (5
percent) or reported to be depressed (12.5 percent) and 81 percent met with friends more
than twice a month (Leon-Carrion et al. (2002)). Many locked-in patients return home from
hospital and start a different but meaningful life (Laureys et al. (2005)).
1.5 Brain-Computer Interfaces for Healthy Subjects
Applications of BCI technology go beyond rehabilitation. Although BCI for healthy sub-
jects is pursued much less, it is of high industrial relevance. It is less the desire to com-
municate for the healthy: this is much more easily done via keyboard, computer mouse,
speech, or gesture recognition devices. It is this additional independent channel “BCI” for
man-machine interaction (see chapters 23, 24 and 25 for first examples in this direction
of research) that has remained unexplored. Brain signals read in real-time on a single trial
basis could provide direct access to human brain states, which can then be used to adapt
the man-machine interface on the fly. One application field could be monitoring tasks such
as alertness monitoring, where the brain holds the key to access information that can oth-
erwise not be easily acquired. Signals of interest to be inferred from brain activity are
cognitive workload, alertness, task involvement, emotion, or concentration. For instance,
workload could be assessed in behavioral experiments by measuring reaction times. How-
ever, this would give very indirect and therefore imprecise measures with respect to tem-
poral resolution, quality, and context. The online monitoring of cognitive workload could
contribute to construct better systems in safety critical applications (see chapter 24). A fur-
ther direction is the direct use of brain states in computer applications or as novel features
for computer gaming (see figure 1.1). The latter is an interesting challenge since the game
interfaces should be able to compensate for the imperfect signal of a BCI. In other words,
if the classification rate of a BCI is 95 percent, then the respective computer game interface
will have to be robust with respect to the 5 percent errors that will inevitably occur. Tetris,
although already successfully played with the BBCI system, is a good example of a game
where small errors can seriously spoil the course of the game.
The current state of EEG sensor technology and the price of EEGs are major obstacles
for a broad use of BCI technology for healthy users. However, once fashionable, cheap,
contactless EEG caps are available—for example, in the shape of baseball caps—a wide
market and application perspective will immediately open.
23. 8 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
Figure 1.1 The simple game of Pong is revived in a new technological context: imagination of the
right hand moves the cursor to the right, imagination of the left hand pushes the cursor to the left. In
this manner, the ball that is reflected from the sides of the game field can be hit by the brain-racket.
Thus, the user can use his intentions to play “Brain Pong.” Dornhege (2006); Krepki (2004); Krepki
et al. (2007).
1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing
Current BCIs differ in how the neural activity of the brain is recorded, how subjects
(humans and animals) are trained, how the signals are translated into device commands,
and which application is provided to the user. An overview of current noninvasive BCIs is
provided in chapters 2–7, while invasive BCIs are discussed in chapters 8–12. An overview
of existing software packages is found in chapters 20 and 21.
1.6.1 Noninvasive Recording Methods for BCI
The electrical activity of the brain can be recorded noninvasively with electroencephalogra-
phy (EEG) (e.g., Birbaumer et al. (1999); Pfurtscheller et al. (2000b); Wolpaw et al. (2003);
Blankertz et al. (2006a)). The current produced by neural activity induces a magnetic
field that can be recorded with magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Birbaumer and Cohen
(2005)). Increased neural activity is accompanied by locally increased glucose metabolism,
resulting in increased glucose and oxygene consumption. As a consequence of glucose con-
sumption, cranial arteries dilate, allowing for increased blood flow, that results in hyper-
oxygenation of the active tissue. Imaging techniques make use of the different magnetic
and optical properties of oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin. The different mag-
netic properties of the ferrous on the heme of oxy- and deoxyhemoglobin are the basis of
the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response measured with functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) (Chen and Ogawa (2000)). Oxy- and deoxyhemoglobin have
different optical properties in the visible and near infrared range. The changes in the ratio
of oxygenated hemoglobin to blood volume due to neural activity is measured with near
infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) (Bunce et al. (2006)). In the following sections, we briefly
review noninvasive BCIs categorized according to the recording techniques.
24. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 9
Figure 1.2 Generic noninvasive BCI setup: signals are recorded, e.g., with EEG, meaningful
features are extracted and subsequently classified. Finally, a signal is extracted from the classifier
that provides the control signal for some device or machine.
1.6.1.1 Brain Signals Recorded from the Scalp (EEG-BCIs)
In a typical BCI setting, participants are presented with stimuli or are required to perform
specific mental tasks while the electrical activity of their brains is being recorded by EEG
(see figure 1.2 for a general setup of a noninvasive BCI). Extracted and relevant EEG
features can then be fed back to the user by so-called closed-loop BCIs. Specific features of
the EEG are either regulated by the BCI user (slow cortical potentials (SCP), sensorimotor
rhythms (SMR)) or are elicited by visual, tactile, or auditory stimulation (event-related
potentials, namely the P300 or steady-state [visually-]evoked potentials (SS[V]EP)). In the
following paragraphs we provide a short description of the physiology of these features
and their use for brain-computer interfacing.
Slow Cortical Potentials (SCP)
Research over the past thirty years on SCPs and their regulation led to the excitation-
threshold-regulation theory (Birbaumer et al. (1990, 2003); Strehl et al. (2006)). The
vertical arrangement of pyramidal cells in the cortex is essential for the generation of SCP
(see figure 1.3). Most apical dendrites of pyramidal cells are located in cortical layers I and
II. Depolarization of the apical dendrites giving rise to SCP is dependent on sustained
afferent intracortical or thalamocortical input to layers I and II, and on simultaneous
depolarization of large pools of pyramidal neurons. The SCP amplitude recorded from
the scalp depends upon the synchronicity and intensity of the afferent input to layers I and
II (Speckmann et al. (1984)). The depolarization of cortical cell assemblies reduces their
excitation threshold such that firing of neurons in regions responsible for specified motor or
cognitive tasks is facilitated. Negative amplitude shifts grow with increasing attentional or
cognitive resource allocation. Cortical positivity may result from active inhibition of apical
dendritic neural activity or simply from a reduction of afferent inflow and subsequent
reduced postsynaptic activity. In any case, positive SCPs are considered to increase the
excitation threshold of upper cortical layers via a negative feedback loop involving the
basal ganglia and the reticular nucleus of the thalamus. Increasing cortical negativity is
accompanied by increased activation of inhibitory striatal nuclei that leads to an increase
25. 10 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
Negative surface potential
underneath electrode
Cortical
layers
Nonspecific
thalamic
afferents
Apical dendrite
Sink
Current flow
Source
Pyramidal cell
Figure 1.3 Negative slow cortical potentials at the surface of the cortex originate from afferent
thalamic or cortical input to the apical dendrites in layers I and II. The extracellular surrounding
of the dendrites is electrically negative, leading to current flow into the cell mediated by positive
sodium ions (sink). Intracellularly, the current flows toward the soma (source). This fluctuation of
ions generates field potentials that can be recorded by electrodes on the scalp (from Kübler et al.
(2001a), figure 4, with permission).
of the excitation threshold of upper cortical layers, thereby preventing overexcitation
(Birbaumer et al. (2003); Hinterberger et al. (2003c); Strehl et al. (2006)).
A strong relationship among self-induced cortical negativity, reaction time, signal detec-
tion, and short-term memory performance has been reported in several studies in humans
and monkeys (Lutzenberger et al. (1979, 1982); Rockstroh et al. (1982)). Tasks requir-
ing attention are performed significantly better when presented after spontaneous or self-
induced cortical negativity.
Slow Cortical Potentials as Input for a BCI (SCP-BCI)
The SCP-BCI requires users to achieve voluntary regulation of brain activity. Typically,
the SCP-BCI presents users with the traditional S1-S2 paradigm, which in the sixties led
Walter and colleagues to the detection of the contingent negative variation (CNV) (Walter
et al. (1964)): a negative SCP shift seen after a warning stimulus (S1) two to ten seconds
before an imperative stimulus (S2) that requires participants to perform a task (e.g., a
26. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 11
Figure 1.4 Course of slow cortical potentials (SCP) averaged across 600 trials (amplitude as a
function of time). The grey line shows the course of SCP when cortical negativity has to be produced
to move the cursor toward the target at the top of the screen, the black line when cortical positivity is
required to move the cursor toward the bottom target. Negative and positive SCP amplitudes clearly
differ between the two tasks providing a binary response. At the beginning of a trial the task is
presented, accompanied by a high-pitched tone (S1—warning stimulus) indicating that two seconds
later the active phase will start providing SCP feedback to the user. The active phase is introduced
by a low-pitched tone (S2—imperative stimulus). Between S1 and S2 a contingent negative variation
(CNV) develops, which indicates that the user is preparing to perform the task.
button press or cursor movement). The CNV (see figure 1.4) indicates depolarization and,
thus, resource allocation for task performance as described above. Similarly, the SCP-BCI
presents users with a high-pitched tone (S1) that indicates to the user that two seconds
later, simultaneously with a low-pitched tone (S2), feedback of SCPs will start either
visually as cursor movement on a monitor or by auditory means with instrumental sounds
(Hinterberger et al. (2004a); Kotchoubey et al. (1997); Kübler et al. (1999)). Users are
presented with two tasks, for example, cursor movement into targets either at the top or
bottom of the screen, or an increase or decrease in the pitch of tones. To perform the
task, BCI users have to produce positive and negative SCP amplitude shifts as compared
to a baseline (see figure 1.4). SCP amplitude shifts must be above or below a predefined
threshold to be classified as negative or positive. Severely paralyzed patients communicated
extended messages with the SCP-BCI (Birbaumer et al. (1999); Neumann et al. (2003))
(see chapter 3).
Sensorimotor Rhythms (SMR)
Sensorimotor rhythms include an arch-shaped μ-rhythm (see figure 1.5), usually with
a frequency of 10 Hz (range 8–11 Hz), often mixed with a β (around 20 Hz) and a γ
component (around 40 Hz) recorded over somatosensory cortices, most preferably over
C3 and C4. Spreading to parietal leads is frequent and also is seen in patients with
27. 12 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
Figure 1.5 Upper trace: μ-rhythm over sensory motor areas. Lower trace: desynchronization of
μ-rhythm through movement imagery.
amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (Kübler et al. (2005a)). Recently, an ALS patient showed
left-hand movement-imagery-related SMR modulation at P4, which is in accordance with
increased parietal activation during hand movement imagery in ALS patients as measured
with functional magnetic resonance imaging (Kew et al. (1993); Lulé et al. (in press)). The
SMR is related to the motor cortex with contributions of somatosensory areas such that the
beta component arises from the motor and the alphoid μ-component from sensory cortex.
SMR is blocked by movements, movement imagery, and movement preparation; thus, it is
seen as an “idling” rhythm of the cortical sensory region. In cats, μ-rhythm-like activity has
been shown to originate from the nucleus ventralis posterior of the thalamus. Usually μ-
rhythm activity is not readily seen in scalp-recorded spontaneous EEG activity and it thus
historically has been believed to occur in only a small number of adult persons. However,
with better signal processing it has been shown to be ubiquitous in adults. Immediately,
scalp-detectable μ-rhythm may, however, be an indicator of pathology. It was reported
to accompany autonomic and emotional dysfunction such as migrane, bronchial asthma,
tinnitus, anxiety, aggressiveness, and emotional instability. It is also often seen in patients
with epilepsy. Three theories for the neurophysiological basis of the μ-rhythm exist: (1) it
could be the correlate of neuronal hyperexcitability as specifically expressed in pathology,
(2) it could be a sign of cortical inhibition, which would explain the blocking of μ-rhythm
by movement or movement imagery, or (3) it may be interpreted as somatosensory “cortical
idling,” adding the component of afferent input (summarized according to Niedermeyer
(2005b)).
Sensorimotor Rhythms as Input for a BCI (SMR-BCI)
Sensorimotor rhythms (SMR) decrease or desynchronize with movement or preparation for
movement and increase or synchronize in the postmovement period or during relaxation
(Pfurtscheller et al. (1999)). Furthermore, and most relevant for BCI use by locked-
in patients, they also desynchronize with motor imagery. Thus, to modulate the SMR
amplitude no actual movement is required. Many BCI groups choose SMR as input
signal because—at least in healthy participants—they are easy to regulate by means of
motor imagery (see figure 1.6 and chapters 2–5). Modulation of SMR can be achieved
within the first training session where the subjects are instructed to imagine left and
right hand and foot movement (e.g., Blankertz et al. (2006a), see also chapter 5). After
28. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 13
Figure 1.6 EEG frequency spectrum of an ALS patient at electrode position Cp4 as a function of
amplitude averaged over about 160 trials. The grey line shows the averaged EEG when the cursor
has to be moved into the top target at the right-hand-side margin of the screen, the black line
when the cursor has to be moved into the bottom target. Downward cursor movement is achieved
by motor imagery (in this case left-hand movement leading to SMR modulation at Cp4), upward
cursor movement by “thinking of nothing.” During downward cursor movement, the SMR amplitudes
decrease (desynchronize) in the α and β band leading to a binary signal.
subsequent machine learning (about two minutes on a standard PC) and visual inspection,
individualized spatiotemporal filters and classifiers are ready to be used for feedback.
Good subjects are then able to achieve information transfer rates of fifty bits per minute in
asynchronous BCI mode (for a comparison of different evaluation criteria, see chapter 19).
Even under the extremely challenging conditions of life demonstrations at CeBit 2006 in
Hanover, Germany, subjects were able to achieve on average a selection rate of five to eight
letters per minute in a spelling task (see chapter 5 and Müller and Blankertz (2006)).
To achieve similar EEG patterns of imagined movements as compared to actual move-
ments, it is important to instruct participants to imagine movement kinesthetically, meaning
to “feel and experience” the movement instead of simply visualizing a movement (Neuper
et al. (2005)). As with the SCP-BCI, to operate the SMR-BCI subjects are required to reg-
ulate the SMR amplitude and are thus provided with visual (Pfurtscheller et al. (2006c);
Wolpaw et al. (2003)) or auditory feedback (Hinterberger et al. (2004a); Nijboer et al. (in
press)) (see also chapter 3). Typically, subjects are shown two or more targets on a monitor
in which the cursor has to be moved by means of SMR amplitude modulation (see also
chapters 2, 4, and 5). In a recent study with four ALS patients, it was shown that SMR
regulation is possible despite considerable degeneration of cortical and spinal motor neu-
rons (Kübler et al. (2005a)). However, the SMR amplitude is much lower in patients as
compared to healthy individuals.
29. 14 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
Event-Related Potentials
Event-related potentials (ERPs) are electrocortical potentials that can be measured in the
EEG before, during, or after a sensory, motor, or psychological event. They have a fixed
time delay to the stimulus and their amplitude is usually much smaller than the ongoing
spontaneous EEG activity. The amplitudes are smaller because ERPs are more localized
in the corresponding cortical areas. They are less frequent than the spontaneous EEG
waves with similar shape and amplitude (Birbaumer and Schmid (2006)). To detect ERPs,
averaging techniques are used. An averaged ERP is composed of a series of large, biphasic
waves, lasting a total of five hundred to thousand milliseconds. Error monitoring of the
brain is also accompanied by evoked potentials referred to as error related potentials. These
deflections in the EEG may be used for error detection in a BCI (see chapter 17). In the
following two paragraphs, a short overview is provided of the P300 component of the
event-related potential and the visually (and sensorily) evoked potential for BCI use. BCIs
on the basis of visually evoked potentials and visual P300 require intact gaze.
P300
The P300 is a positive deflection in the electroencephalogram (EEG) time-locked to
auditory or visual stimuli (see figure 1.7). It is typically seen when participants are required
to attend to rare target stimuli presented within a stream of frequent standard stimuli
(Squires et al. (1977)), an experimental design referred to as the oddball paradigm (Fabiani
et al. (1987)). The P300 amplitude varies as a function of task characteristics such as
discriminability of standard and target stimuli (Johnson and Donchin (1978)), loudness of
tones (Squires et al. (1977)), overall probability of the target stimuli, the preceding stimulus
sequence (Squires et al. (1976)), and the electrode position (Squires et al. (1977)). Mostly
observed in central and parietal regions, it is seen as a correlate of an extinction process in
short-term memory when new stimuli require an update of representations.
P300 as Input Signal for a BCI (P300-BCI)
As early as the late eighties, Farwell and Donchin had shown that the P300 component of
the event-related potential can be used to select items displayed on a computer monitor
(Farwell and Donchin (1988)). The authors presented their participants with a 6 x 6 matrix
where each of the 36 cells contained a character or a symbol. This design becomes an
oddball paradigm by first intensifying resp. flashing each row and column for 100 ms in
random order and second by instructing participants to focus attention to only one of the
36 cells. Thus, in one sequence of 12 flashes (6 rows and 6 columns are highlighted), the
target cell will flash only twice, constituting a rare event compared to the 10 flashes of all
other rows and columns and therefore eliciting a P300 (see figure 1.7). Selection occurs by
detecting the row and column that elicit the largest P300 (see also chapter 2). The P300-
BCI does not require self-regulation of the EEG. All that is required from users is that they
are able to focus attention and gaze on the target letter albeit for a considerable amount of
time.
Over the past five years, the P300 has received increasing amounts of attention as a BCI
control signal. For example, a number of offline studies have been conducted to improve
30. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 15
Figure 1.7 Averaged EEG at the vertex electrode (Cz) of an ALS patient using a 7 x 7 P300 spelling
matrix. The black line indicates the EEG response to 2448 standard stimuli and the grey line to 51
target letters (oddball) that have to be selected from the spelling matrix. A positive deflection as a
response to targets can be seen in the time window between 200 and 500 ms.
the classification rate of the P300 Speller (Kaper et al. (2004); Serby et al. (2005); Xu
et al. (2004); He et al. (2001); Thulasidas et al. (2006)). Using a support vector machine
classifier, Thulasidas et al. report online selection of three characters per minute with 95
percent accuracy (Thulasidas et al. (2006)). Bayliss showed that the P300 also can be
used to select items in a virtual apartment, provided presentation of targets constitute an
oddball paradigm (Bayliss et al. (2004)). In 2003, Sellers, Schalk, and Donchin published
the first results of an ALS patient using the P300 Speller (Sellers et al. (2003)). In recent
studies, Sellers et al. and Nijboer et al. presented results of the P300 speller used by ALS
patients indicating that ALS patients are able to use the P300-BCI with accuracies up to 100
percent (Nijboer et al. (submitted); Sellers et al. (2006b)). It was also shown that the P300
response remains stable over periods of twelve to more than fifty daily sessions in healthy
volunteers as well as in ALS-patients (Nijboer et al. (submitted); Sellers and Donchin
(2006)). Piccione et al. tested the P300 as a control signal for a BCI in seven healthy and
five paralyzed patients (Piccione et al. (2006)). As in the other studies, task completion
and good performance was achieved after little time, thus there was no need for time-
consuming training. However, the patients’ performance (68.6 percent) was worse than that
of healthy participants (76.2 percent). In particular, those patients who were more impaired
performed worse than did healthy participants, whereas there was no difference between
less impaired patients and healthy participants (Piccione et al. (2006)). Recently, Vaughan
et al. introduced a P300-BCI for daily use in a patient’s home environment (Vaughan et al.
(2006)). Auditorily presented oddball paradigms may be used for patients with restricted
or lost eye movement and are currently being investigated (Sellers and Donchin (2006);
Hill et al. (2005)).
31. 16 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
SSVEP
After visual stimulation (e.g., an alternating checkerboard), evoked potentials can be
recorded from the visual cortex in the occipital lobe (O1, O2, Oz—according to the
international 10-20 system). A visually evoked potential becomes steady if the presentation
rate of stimuli is above 6 Hz (Gao et al. (2003b)). When participants focus their gaze on
a flickering target, the amplitude of the steady-state visually evoked potential (SSVEP)
increases at the fundamental frequency of the target and second and third harmonics (Wang
et al. (2006); Müller-Putz et al. (2005b)). Amplitude and phase of the SSVEP depend on
stimulus parameters such as repetition rate and contrast. The frequency resolution of the
SSVEP is about 0.2 Hz and the bandwidth in which the SSVEP can be detected reliably is
between 6 and 24 Hz (Gao et al. (2003b)).
SSVEPs as Input Signal for a BCI (SSVEP-BCI)
Like the P300-BCI, the SSVEP-BCI requires attention and intact gaze but no user train-
ing as the cortical response is elicited via external stimulation (see chapter 4). To elicit
SSVEPs, targets with different flickering frequencies are presented on a monitor (Wang
et al. (2006)) or on a board with light emitting diodes (LED) (Müller-Putz et al. (2005b);
Gao et al. (2003b)). The number of targets realized in a BCI varies from 4 (Müller-Putz
et al. (2005b)) up to 48 (Gao et al. (2003b)). Classification accuracies of more than 90
percent correct are often reported (Kelly et al. (2005); Nielsen et al. (2006); Trejo et al.
(2006)). In a 9-target SSVEP-BCI, healthy participants spelled out their phone number and
birth date with a spelling rate of 7.2–11.5 selections per minute (information transfer rate
of 18.37–27.29 bits/min) (Nielsen et al. (2006)), and in an 11-target SSVEP-BCI with an
average accuracy of 83.3 percent (23.06 bits/min) (Lee et al. (2006)).
A caveat of all SSVEP approaches to BCI control is their dependence on intact gaze,
which renders them unsuitable for patients with restricted eye movement. Two studies
address this issue. Kelly et al. investigated classification accuracies when users were not
required to focus gaze on the flickering targets but on a fixation cross between two targets—
a condition the authors refer to as covert attention (Kelly et al. (2005)). A decrease in
accuracy was observed from about 95 percent when targets were fixated directly to about
70 percent in the covert attention condition. Thus, at least a rather simple two-target SSVEP
paradigm might be used by locked-in patients albeit with reduced accuracy. A BCI based on
steady-state evoked potentials completely independent of vision was introduced by Müller-
Putz and colleagues (Müller-Putz et al. (2006)). The authors used vibratory stimulation
of left- and right-hand fingertips to elicit somatosensory steady-state evoked potentials
(SSSEP, see figure 1.8). The EEG was recorded from central electrodes (C3, Cz, and
C4—according to the international 10-20 system). In each trial, both index fingers were
stimulated simultaneously at different frequencies and participants were instructed via
arrows on a computer screen to which finger they should pay attention. Online accuracies
of four participants varied between 53 (chance level) and 83 percent correct, but offline
classification was between 65 and 88 percent correct. Albeit not yet as reliable as the
SSVEP-BCI the SSSEP-BCI may become an option for patients with impaired vision.
32. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 17
Figure 1.8 Peak at 31 Hz recorded at C3 when focusing attention on stimulation of right index
finger (left panel) and 26 Hz at C4 when focusing on stimulation of left index finger. Both peaks
reflect the stimulation frequency correctly (We thank Dr. Gernot Müller-Putz from the Laboratory of
Brain-Computer Interfaces Institute for Knowledge Discovery at the Technical University of Graz,
Austria, for this picture and the permission of reproduction).
1.6.1.2 Combinations of Signals
It is a well-known fact that different physiological phenomena, for example, slow corti-
cal potential shifts such as the premovement Bereitschaftspotenzial or differences in spa-
tiospectral distributions of brain activity (i.e., focal event-related desynchronizations), code
for different aspects of a subject’s intention to move. While papers noted the potential of
combining these multiple modalities, it was first explored systematically by Dornhege et
al. (2004a). Their work showed that BCI information transfer rates can be boosted signif-
icantly when combining different EEG features. From a theoretical point of view, feature
combination is most beneficial if the features of the single modalities have maximal statis-
tical independence. High mutual independence can be measured in EEG features and thus
subject dependent improvements of up to 50 percent relative classification performance
gain are observed when using combined features in an offline evaluation (Dornhege et al.
(2004a); Dornhege (2006)). The use of robust well-regularized classifiers is mandatory in
this “sensor-fusion” process because otherwise the model complexity is hard to control in
such high dimensional feature spaces (see chapter 13). We conjecture that not only combi-
nations between different EEG modalities but also between different recording technolo-
gies will be useful in the future, for example, between fMRI and EEG, or between local
field potentials and spike data. Machine learning will be challenged by fusing different
time-scales and their underlying statistical processes.
1.6.1.3 The Magnetic Activity of the Brain
The magnetic field generated by electrical brain activity can be measured by means of
magnetoencephalography (MEG). To date, this method is used only in laboratory settings
and is consequently not suitable for a BCI for communication and control in the patient’s
home environment. However, the advantages of MEG as compared to EEG, namely better
spatial resolution leading to a precise localization of cortical activation related to a specific
33. 18 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
task or sensory stimulation and higher signal-to-noise ratio, especially for higher frequency
activity like gamma band activity, render it a viable tool for short-term intervention and
rehabilitation (see chapter 14). In a study with three tetraplegic patients after cervical spinal
cord injury, Kauhanen et al. achieved the same classification accuracies in MEG data as
compared to EEG data (Kauhanen et al. (2006)). The patient’s task was to attempt finger
movement, and data were analyzed offline. Lal et al. showed that regulation of the magnetic
activity of the brain by means of motor imagery can be used to select letters on a computer
screen, but participants were not yet provided with online feedback of MEG activity;
instead they were provided with feedback of results, that is, a smiling face after correct
classification or selection of the correct letter (Lal et al. (2005b)). Mellinger et al. even
provide online MEG feedback for healthy participants during motor imagery (Mellinger
et al. (under revision)). Three of five participants achieved cursor control of 90 percent
accuracy or more within the first training session. Thus, learning to regulate brain activity
by means of MEG feedback and achieved accuracies were comparable to EEG (Blankertz
et al. (2006a)). MEG may be used to localize the focus of activity during motor imagery
if EEG provides no clear results (see chapter 22). Currently, MEG feedback during motor
imagery is used to train chronic stroke patients to reactivate the paralyzed limb provided
that not the entire motor cortex or pyramidal tracts are lesioned. Chronic stroke patients
undergo an MEG feedback training such that their paralyzed limb is provided with an
orthosis that opens and closes the paralyzed hand (Birbaumer and Cohen (2005)). Motor
imagery opens the orthosis whereas relaxation (thinking of nothing) closes it. This training
provides the patients with self-induced sensory feedback of the paralyzed limb. The idea
behind this being that activation of a sensorimotor network enables patients to relearn
motor functions (Braun et al. (submitted)) (see also chapter 22).
1.6.1.4 The Blood Oxygen Level Dependent Response (BOLD)
For the past approximately five years it has been possible to use the blood oxygen level de-
pendent (BOLD) response as input signal for a BCI. Local concentration of deoxygenated
hemoglobin in brain tissue depends on neuronal activity and metabolism and changes can
be measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Compared to EEG,
fMRI allows spatial resolution in the range of millimeters and a more precise allocation
of neuronal activity. Additionally, activation in subcortical areas can be recorded. Due to
recent advances in acquisition techniques, computational power, and algorithms, the func-
tional sensitivity and speed of fMRI was increased considerably (Weiskopf et al. (2004b))
and the delay of feedback could be reduced to below two seconds (Weiskopf et al. (2003)),
which allows the use of this technique as real-time fMRI. Target areas for feedback were
sensory (S1, e.g., Yoo et al. (2004)) and motor areas (M1, e.g., DeCharms et al. (2004);
SMA Weiskopf et al. (2004b)), the parahippocampal place area (Weiskopf et al. (2004b)),
the affective and cognitive subdivision of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Weiskopf
et al. (2003)), and rostral ACC (DeCharms et al. (2005)). Learning of self-regulating the
BOLD response was reported in all studies that included subject training to regulate the
BOLD response, and some reported behavioral effects in relation to activation or deacti-
vation of target areas: Increase of activation in the affective subdivision of the ACC led to
34. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 19
higher valence and arousal ratings of the subjective affective state (Weiskopf et al. (2003)).
Better encoding of words after down regulation of the parahippocampal place area (as
compared to the supplementary motor area) and decreased reaction time in a motor task
after upregulation of the supplementary motor area (as compared to the parahippocam-
pal place area) was demonstrated (Weiskopf et al. (2004b)). Regulation of the insula, an
area involved in emotional processing, also proved possible and was shown to increase the
negative valence of participants when confronted with negative stimuli such as pictures
of violence or mutilated bodies (Sitaram et al. (2006)). Recently, specific effects on pain
perception as a function of self-regulation of the rostral part of the ACC was reported in
the first clinical study including patients with chronic pain. In healthy subjects, the au-
thors controlled for effects of repeated practice, brain region, feedback, and intervention.
In chronic pain patients, only feedback was controlled such that one group received feed-
back of the BOLD response in the rostral ACC and another of skin conductance, heart
rate, and respiration. Healthy participants were presented with nociceptive heat stimuli.
Only in those healthy participants and pain patients who received real-time feedback of the
BOLD response in the rostral ACC, an area known to be involved in pain perception, were
changes in pain ratings found (DeCharms et al. (2005)). This study already demonstates
the possible power of the fMRI-BCI for treating clinical groups if the neurobiological basis
of the disorder is known. For example, hypoactivation in orbitofrontal and limbic areas in-
volved in emotional processing were found in psychopaths (Birbaumer et al. (2005)), and
hypofunction in dorsolateral and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the pregenual part of
the ACC is consistently found in depressed patients (Davidson et al. (2002)). Even more
complex cognitive functions as needed for the game of paper, rock, and scissors could be
decoded successfully with fMRI by Kamitani and Tong (2005). Most recently, Owen et al.
successfully distinguished activation patterns to motor imagery (playing tennis) and spatial
navigation (through one’s own house starting at the front door) in a patient diagnosed with
persistent vegetative state, and could thus show that she was consciously aware (Owen
et al. (2006)). For further reference, see also the review by Haynes and Rees (2006).
1.6.1.5 Near Infrared Spectroscopy (NIRS) as a Recording Method for BCI
The advantage of functional MRI as compared to EEG is its 3D spatial resolution. How-
ever, fMRI is expensive and bound to the laboratory. Near infrared spectroscopy offers a
comparable spatial resolution albeit restricted to cortical areas (depth 1–3 cm) with much
less technical effort and costs. Moreover the NIRS-BCI is portable and could thus be used
in a patient’s home environment.
The NIRS-BCI system presented by Sitaram and colleagues incorporates the so-called
continuous wave technique. Regional brain activation is accompanied by increases in
regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and the regional cerebral oxygen metabolic rate
(rCMRO2). The increase of rCBF exceeds that of rCMRO2 resulting in a decrease of
deoxygenated hemoglobin in venous blood. Thus, the ratio of oxygenated to deoxygenated
hemoglobin is expected to increase in active brain areas and is measured with NIRS.
The continuous wave approach uses multiple pairs or channels of light sources and light
detectors operating at two or more discrete wavelengths. The light source may be a
35. 20 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
Figure 1.9 Exemplary data from a healthy participant performing motor imagery (right hand
movement). The solid line indicates course of oxygenated (Oxy HB; HB = hemoglobin) and the
dashed line of deoxygenated hemoglobin (Deoxy HB) averaged across a full session (80 trials) from
channel 7 on the contralateral (left) hemisphere (close to the C3 electrode position as per the 10-20
system) for the duration 0–140 time points after stimulus presentation. 140 time points are equal to
10 s of execution of the motor imagery task at a sampling rate of 14 Hz. (We thank Ranganatha
Sitaram from the Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioural Neurobiology, University of
Tübingen, for this picture and the permission of reproduction).
laser or a light emitting diode (LED). The optical parameter measured is attenuation of
light intensity due to absorption by the intermediate tissue. The concentration changes of
oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin are computed from the changes in the light
intensity at different wavelengths (Sitaram et al. (2007)). It has been shown already that
brain activation in response to motor movement and imagery can be readily detected with
NIRS (see figure 1.9, Coyle et al. (2004); Sitaram et al. (2005, 2007)).
1.6.2 Invasive Recording Methods for BCI
Invasive recording methods either measure the neural activity of the brain on the cortical
surface (electrocorticography, ECoG) or intracortically from within the (motor) cortex
(see figure 1.10 for general setup). These methods have strong advantages in terms of
signal quality and dimensionality. However, they require surgery and the issues of long-
term stability of implants and protection from infection arise (Hochberg et al. (2006)).
The decision of a BCI user for the one method over the other will strongly depend on
36. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 21
a
Visual, tactile, or
proprioceptive
feedback
Telemetry
(receiver)
Telemetry
(sender)
Control
logic
Robotic
arm
Signal
preprocessing
unit
Recording
system
(intracortical)
Translation to
control signal
b
Figure 1.10 Generic setup of an invasive BCI (left) and picture of an array electrode placed into
the monkey cortex (right). Figure (b) from Nicolelis (2001) by permission.
the purpose of BCI use; for example, multidirectional neuroprosthesis control may only be
possible with intracortical recordings, whereas communication at a speed of approximately
ten selections per minute can be achieved with noninvasive methods. We might speculate
that invasive methods have to proof substantially better than noninvasive methods to
become attractive for possible users.
1.6.2.1 Brain Signals Recorded from the Surface of the Cortex (ECoG)
The electrocorticogram (ECoG) uses epidural or subdural electrode grids or strips to record
the electrical activity of the cortex. It is an invasive procedure that requires craniotomy for
implantation of electrodes (see Leuthardt et al. (2006b)). However, the procedure becomes
less invasive when less electrodes are required (strips instead of grids) because strips may
be inserted via a small hole in the scalp. The main advantages of ECoG are a higher spatial
resolution than the EEG (tenths of millimeters versus centimeters), broader bandwidth (0–
200 Hz versus 0–40 Hz) that allows also recording of γ band activity, and higher amplitude
(50–100 μV versus 5–20 μV) and less vulnerability to artifacts such as electromyogram
(Leuthardt et al. (2004)). Commonly, ECoG is used to localize seizure activity in patients
with epilepsy before they undergo surgery. Studies on the feasibility of ECoG for BCI were
thus largely conducted with epilepsy patients and are reviewed in detail in chapter 8. To
our knowledge, only one ALS patient consented to grid implantation for the purpose of
controlling a BCI for communication, but communication was not achieved (Birbaumer
(2006a,b), see chapter 22). Most of these studies performed offline open-loop analysis
of ECoG data (Huggins et al. (1999); Levine et al. (1999)). Using Distinction Sensitive
Learning Vector Quantization (DSLVQ) for offline classification of data recorded during
self-paced middle finger extension, Scherer et al. reported accuracies between 85 and 91
37. 22 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
percent (Scherer et al. (2003)). Hill et al. applied autoregressive models and support vector
machine classification to data obtained during motor imagery and achieved accuracies
around 75 percent (Hill et al. (2006)). Few studies closed the loop and provided feedback
of ECoG to the participants (Felton et al. (2005); Leuthardt et al. (2004, 2006a); Wilson
et al. (2006)). In each study by Leuthardt et al., the ECoG of four patients was recorded
with electrode grids or strips over prefrontal, temporal, sensorimotor, and speech areas.
Patients were required to perform and imagine motor and speech tasks such as opening
and closing the right or left hand, protruding the tongue, shrugging shoulders, or saying
the word move. Each task was associated with a decrease in μ- and β-rhythm and an
increase of gamma-rhythm amplitudes over prefrontal, premotor, sensorimotor, or speech
areas. The spatial and spectral foci of task-related ECoG activity were similar for action
and imagery. Frequency bands in the gamma range were most often chosen for online
control, and during movement imagery accuracies achieved within a brief training of 3–
24 minutes were between 73 and 98 percent. Wilson et al. proposed to use multimodal
imagery for cursor control and showed that cursor control can be achieved with nonmotor
imagery such as auditory imagery (a favorite song, voices, phone) (Wilson et al. (2006)).
In a completely paralyzed ALS patient implanted with a 32-electrode grid, classification of
signals related to motor imagery was at the chance level (Birbaumer (2006a); Hill et al.
(2006); see also chapter 22). More than one year after implantation approximately 50
percent of the electrodes provide stable and clear signal recording (unpublished data from
first author’s affiliation).
1.6.2.2 Brain Signals Recorded from Within the Cortex
Intracortical signal acquisition can be realized with single, few, or multiple electrodes
(arrays) that capture the action potentials of individual neurons. Electrode tips have to be
in close proximity to the signal source and the arrays have to be stable over a long period
of time. With two exemplary ALS patients, Kennedy and Bakay showed that humans are
able to modulate the action potential firing rate when provided with feedback (Kennedy
and Bakay (1998)). The authors implanted into the motor cortex a single electrode with
a glass tip containing neurotrophic factors. Adjacent neurons grew into the tip and after
a few weeks, action potentials were recorded. One patient was able to move a cursor on
a computer screen to select presented items by modulating his action-potential firing rate
(Kennedy et al. (2000, 2004)). In Mehring et al. (2003), it was demonstrated that hand
movements could be estimated from local field potentials.
Multielectrode arrays for intracortical recording are still to be improved for clinical
application (Nicolelis (2003); Nicolelis et al. (2003), see figure 1.10). They have been used
in animals with stable recordings for up to two years (Nicolelis et al. (2003); Donoghue
(2002)). Recent results by Hochberg et al. (2006) for human patients show that stable
long-term recordings are possible but at the expense of losing signal at a large number
of electrodes. Several groups use multielectrode recording to detect activation patterns
related to movement execution in animals (Carmena et al. (2003); Paninski et al. (2004);
Taylor et al. (2003); Chapin et al. (1999)). The action-potential firing rate in motor areas
contains sensory, motor, perceptual, and cognitive information that allows the estimation
38. 1.6 Recording Methods, Paradigms, and Systems for Brain-Computer Interfacing 23
of a subject’s intention for movement execution, and it was shown that 3D hand trajectories
can be derived from the activity pattern of neuronal cell assemblies in the motor cortex by
appropriate decoding (Serruya et al. (2002)). For example, Taylor et al. realized brain-
controlled cursor and robot arm movement using recordings from a few neurons (18
cells) in the motor cortex only (Taylor et al. (2002)). Rhesus macaques learned first
to move a cursor into eight targets located at the corners of an imaginary cube with
real hand movements. Accompanying neural activity patterns were recorded and used to
train an adaptive movement prediction algorithm. After sufficient training of subjects and
algorithm, subjects’ arms were restricted and cursor movement was performed by brain
control. Similarly, rhesus monkeys were trained to move a brain-controlled robot arm in
virtual reality (Taylor et al. (2003)) and then to feed themselves with a real robot arm
(Schwartz (2004b)).
Recently, Musallam et al. presented data from three monkeys that were implanted with
electrode arrays in the parietal reach area, area 5, and the dorsal premotor cortex (Musallam
et al. (2004)). Subjects were first trained to reach for targets at different positions on
a screen after a delay of 1.2 to 1.8 seconds following cue presentation. Neural activity
during the memory period was correctly decoded with an accuracy of about 64 percent.
The authors then trained subjects to associate visual cues with the amount, probability, or
type of reward (orange juice versus water). Neural activity was then found to alter as a
function of expected reward and thus represented additional information for classification.
Accordingly, classification results could be improved by 12 percent.
Santhanam et al. (2006) used a 96-electrode array implanted in the monkey dorsal
premotor cortex and report selection rates of 6.5 bits per second. This astonishing high
information transfer rate was achieved in an instructed delay reach task with ultra-short
trial lengths around 250 ms. Integration over spike activity in very short time windows was
enough for these excellent decoding results.
Hochberg et al. (2006) report on a study where an array of 96 electrodes was implanted
in a human subject diagnosed with tetraplegia three years after high spinal cord injury.
With the array position being the primary motor cortex, it could be demonstrated that spike
patterns were modulated by hand movement intention. A decoding algorithm based on a
linear filter provided a “neural cursor” to the subject, who was then able to operate the
TV, to open or close a prosthetic hand even while in a conversation, or to accomplish
other tasks. The authors furthermore report a considerable loss of recorded units after 6.5
months, which again underlines the necessity to advance sensor technology. It is important
to note that this was the first pilot clinical trial with an intracortical array implantation in
humans.
These and other experimental works reveal that it is possible to derive limb or cursor
movement directly from the neural activity patterns of the cortex with appropriate decoding
algorithms (see also Lebedev and Nicolelis (2006)).
Finally, a simultaneous stimulation of the reward area and the sensory area in the rat
allowed the control over movement patterns of the rat (Talwar et al. (2002)). A more recent
work by Chapin studies how to perform a stimulation of the sensory areas to ultimately
supply artificial sensory feedback for neuroprosthetics (Chapin (2006)).
39. 24 An Introduction to Brain-Computer Interfacing
1.7 Concluding Discussion
Brain-Machine Interfacing—be it invasive or noninvasive—has witnessed a recent explo-
sion of research. The reason for this increased activity is the wide application potential
that the field is bearing. (1) Clinical applications of BCI (such as those outlined in chap-
ter 22) become evident, and work in the invasive BMI community shows the potential for
future use in neuroprosthetics (Leuthardt et al. (2006a)). This is in particular underlined
by the first successful human clinical trial reported by Hochberg et al. or a recent monkey
study by Santhanam et al. that explores the “speed-limit” of invasive brain-computer inter-
facing (Hochberg et al. (2006); Santhanam et al. (2006)). Similar success can be seen in
noninvasive BCIs, where two-dimensional cursor control allows a richer repertoire of com-
munication and higher information transfer rates (ITR) (Wolpaw and McFarland (2004)).
The Berlin BCI system is now able to almost completely dispense with subject training, an
important progress that nevertheless has yet to be verified for disabled users. Event-related
potentials such as the P300 and the steady-state visually evoked potential provide to date
the highest ITR for noninvasive BCIs (Nijboer et al. (submitted); Nielsen et al. (2006); Lee
et al. (2006)). The Tübingen, Albany, and Graz BCIs are used for rehabilitation in exem-
plary patients. Overall, there is about a factor of ten in information transfer rate between
invasive and noninvasive BCIs and it will depend on each individual patient whether the
risk of surgery and potential inflammation incurred in the invasive methods will justify this
gain. (2) Although clinical application in rehabilitation will always serve as a main motiva-
tion and driving force for BCI research, it is the fascination for the brain itself and the urge
to better understand its function that also drives BCI researchers. In fact, BCIs are a unique
new tool that have emerged over the past years to analyze seminal questions in brain re-
search such as plasticity, dynamics, representation, neural coding, intention, planing, and
learning in a very direct manner. Invasive BMIs can now record from several hundreds
of electrodes and can thus directly study, for example, the change of neural code during
learning. Noninvasive BCIs allow researchers to watch how the brain alters and instantiates
behavior and cognition in real-time. (3) Finally, there is a variety of applications that in-
corporate advanced signal processing, such that single trial data can be classified robustly.
This step forward allows BCI researchers to contribute to general topics in the domain of
human-machine interaction. The exploration of the novel independent communication and
control channel BCI to assess the users state in a direct manner opens a broad field and
it remains to be seen how far the BCI channel will prove to be useful when considering
typical HMI applications like assessment of cognitive workload (see chapter 24), alert-
ness, task involvement, emotion, or concentration. Clearly new systems that can use BCI
for navigating or gaming in virtual worlds (see chapter 23) and for enhancing and improv-
ing man-machine interaction are on the way (see chapter 24). It is important to note that
the above applications will be limited to the noninvasive EEG based systems due to their
comparatively low risk and cost.
Many open problems remain on the path toward better brain-computer interfacing and
broader applicability of the BCI technology. As very extensively outlined by, for exam-
ple, Lebedev and Nicolelis (2006); Nicolelis (2001) and Nicolelis et al. (2003), it will be
40. 1.7 Concluding Discussion 25
important to advance recording and transmission technology such that chronical implants
become possible that can persist for a long time with very low risk and telemetrically trans-
mit signals to BCI. A better understanding of basic neuroscience issues like representation,
plasticity, and learning will allow the construction of better BMIs. Similar reasoning also
holds for noninvasive BCIs where contactless wearable sensors are a necessary condition
for a wide applicability of BCIs even outside medical domains, for example, for com-
puter gaming and general man-machine interfacing applications such as usability studies.
Overall, it will be essential to advance signal processing and machine learning technology
to build faster, better, more adaptive, and most important more robust systems. What we
defined as the phenomenon of BCI-illiteracy has to be investigated in more depth to un-
derstand whether there will always be a part of the population that is unable to operate a
BCI and for what reasons. Knowing the amazing possibility of humans to learn a task and
observing the considerable inter- and intraindividual signal variances, it seems reasonable
to make BCIs fully adaptive. Unsolved, however, is how we can get these two complex
learning systems—the machines and the human brains—in synchronicity such that stable
BCI control becomes the rule and not the exception. To investigate long-term stability of
BCI systems clearly, more long-term clinical trials are necessary. Gradual improvement in
all these directions will be indispensible for the future success of this lively and vigorous
field.
Acknowledgments
We thank Boris Kotchoubey, Michael Schröder, and Guido Dornhege for valuable com-
ments on the manuscript. This work is funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft
(DFG), the National Institutes of Health (NIH), the Bundesministerium für Bildung und
Forschung (BMBF), and the IST Programme of the European Community. This publica-
tion reflects only the authors’ views.
Notes
(1) BCI and BMI are used as synonyms.
(2) We distinguish here between brain-computer interfaces that listen to the neural code
and computer-brain interfaces that are also able to transmit information from the
computer toward the brain.
(3) Popularized under the slogan “let the machines learn” by the Berlin Brain-Computer
Interface group (BBCI).
42. Introduction
This part provides an insight into a representative variety of BCI systems that are currently
being pursued in research labs.
A distinctive feature in BCI studies is the paradigm used for the interaction between
user and computer. On one hand there are systems that require an active and voluntary
strategy for generating a specific regulation of an EEG parameter such as the motor-related
μ-rhythm or the self-regulation of slow cortical potentials (SCP). On the other hand there
are passive paradigms, where participants only have to passively view an item for selection.
Those systems detect the evoked responses such as P300 as presented in chapter 2 or make
use of steady-state evoked potentials (SSVEP) as presented in chapter 4.
Finally, one distinction between BCI labs is based on the realization of the system. Most
groups, as introduced in chapters 2, 3, and 7, use extensive subject training. So, users have
to adapt their brain signals to a fixed decoding algorithm, that is, the learning is on the
subject side. Over the past five years, the Berlin group has established a paradigm change,
where learning is now done by the computer, following the motto “let the machines learn.”
Now several groups have adopted this principle. Examples for this approach are discussed
in chapters 4, 5, and 6. Note that even if a pure machine learning approach was intended,
the subject will inevitably learn once feedback has started, so in principle BCI systems will
always have both aspects: subject and machine training.
In this section six major BCI labs introduce their systems. For further ideas we refer to
Babiloni et al. (2004), Gao et al. (2003b), Sykacek et al. (2003), Thulasidas et al. (2006),
Kauhanen et al. (2006), and Kaper et al. (2005). Note that this list can never be complete.
Chapter 2 outlines the Albany BCI, where a user is trained to manipulate his μ and
β rhythms to control a cursor in 1- or 2D. Furthermore, BCI control based on the P300
paradigm is shown.
Similar to Albany, the Tübingen BCI, outlined in chapter 3, train their subjects to
adapt to the system using slow cortical potentials. The group uses BCI as a means for
communication of ALS patients with the outside world and as the design of this interaction.
Further BCI systems discussed in the chapter are P300 and μ-rhythm-based BCIs, an
interesting new BCI paradigm based on auditory stimulation and the use of invasive
techniques like ECoG for BCI.
In chapter 4 the main research directions of the Graz BCI are depicted. The group is
broadly exploring the whole BCI field from sensors, feedback strategies, and cognitive
aspects to novel signal processing methods, with excellent results. The Graz BCI is shown
to be not only of use for patients but also it contributes to general man-machine interaction
as demonstrated for a moving in a VR environment. Typically, only a few electrodes and
43. 30 BCI Systems and Approaches
machine learning techniques combined with user adaptation are employed to achieve BCI
control.
Chapter 5 introduces the Berlin BCI. Compared to training times of weeks or even
months in other BCIs, the BBCI allows for subject control after 30 minutes. This drastic
decrease in training time became possible by virtue of advanced machine learning and
signal processing technology. The chapter presents online feedback studies based on the
physiological signals’ preparatory potential and μ-rhythm modulation. The study shows
that after less than one hour, five of six untrained subjects were able to achieve high
performances when operating a variety of different feedbacks.
Similar to the Berlin approach, the Martigny BCI introduced in chapter 6 tries to relocate
the effort from the subject training to the machine by using machine learning techniques
and online adaptation to realize a BCI. In particular, online adaptation is an important
direction to compensate for the intrinsic nonstationarities found in EEG signals.
Finally, the ideas of the Vancouver BCI are introduced in chapter 7. The main focus
here is to establish an asynchronous BCI for patients, that is, a system that detects whether
a user is intending something or not. To achieve this goal, the authors also use machine
learning techniques that adapt the machine to the user.
The cheapest, most popular, and thus most commonly used measuring device for non-
invasive BCI is certainly EEG, but recently also BCI experiments using fMRI (cf., e.g.,
Weiskopf et al. (2004a); Kamitani and Tong (2005)) and MEG were conducted success-
fully (cf. Mellinger et al. (2005); Kauhanen et al. (2006)). So far fMRI and MEG are too
expensive for a broad use in BCI, but they have been very important for a better under-
standing of the physiological phenomena in the context of BCI control (cf. Haynes and
Rees (2006)).
Thilo Hinterberger, Guido Dornhege, and Klaus-Robert Müller
44. 2 Noninvasive Brain-Computer Interface
Research at the Wadsworth Center
Eric W. Sellers, Dean J. Krusienski, Dennis J. McFarland, and Jonathan R. Wolpaw
Laboratory of Nervous System Disorders
Wadsworth Center
New York State Department of Health
Albany, NY 12201-0509
2.1 Abstract
The primary goal of the Wadsworth Center brain-computer interface (BCI) program is to
develop electroencephalographic (EEG) BCI systems that can provide severely disabled
individuals with an alternative means of communication and/or control. We have shown
that people with or without motor disabilities can learn to control sensorimotor rhythms
recorded from the scalp to move a computer cursor in one or two dimensions and we have
also used the P300 event-related potential as a control signal to make discrete selections.
Overall, our research indicates there are several approaches that may provide alternatives
for individuals with severe motor disabilities. We are now evaluating the practicality and
effectiveness of a BCI communication system for daily use by such individuals in their
homes.
2.2 Introduction
Many people with severe motor disabilities require alternative methods for communica-
tion and control because they are unable to use conventional means that require voluntary
muscular control. Numerous studies over the past two decades indicate that scalp-recorded
EEG activity can be the basis for nonmuscular communication and control systems, com-
monly called brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) (Wolpaw et al. (2002)). EEG-based BCI
systems measure specific features of EEG activity and translate these features into de-
vice commands. The most commonly used features have been sensorimotor rhythms (Wol-
paw et al. (1991, 2002); Wolpaw and McFarland (2004); Pfurtscheller et al. (1993)), slow
cortical potentials (Birbaumer et al. (1999, 2000); Kübler et al. (1998)), and the P300
event-related potential (Farwell and Donchin (1988); Donchin et al. (2000); Sellers and
45. 32 Noninvasive Brain-Computer Interface Research at the Wadsworth Center
Donchin (2006)). Systems based on sensorimotor rhythms or slow cortical potentials use
components in the frequency or time domain that are spontaneous in the sense that they
are not dependent on specific sensory events. Systems based on the P300 response use
time-domain EEG components that are elicited by specific stimuli.
At the Wadsworth Center, our goal is to develop a BCI that is suitable for everyday use by
severely disabled people at home or elsewhere. Over the past 15 years, we have developed
a BCI that allows people, including those who are severely disabled, to move a computer
cursor in one or two dimensions using μ and/or β rhythms recorded over sensorimotor
cortex. More recently, we have expanded our BCI to include use of the P300 response that
was originally described by Farwell and Donchin (1988). Fundamental to the efficacy of
our system has been BCI2000 (Schalk et al. (2004)), the general-purpose software system
that we developed and that is now used by more than one hundred BCI laboratories around
the world (see chapter 21 for a complete description of the BCI2000 system).
2.3 Sensorimotor Rhythm-Based Cursor Control
Users learn during a series of training sessions to use sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) am-
plitudes in the μ (8–12 Hz) and/or β (18–26 Hz) frequency bands over left and/or right
sensorimotor cortex to move a cursor on a video screen in one or two dimensions (Wolpaw
and McFarland (1994, 2004); McFarland et al. (2003)). This is not a normal function of this
brain signal, but rather the result of training. The SMR-based system uses spectral features
extracted from the EEG that are spontaneous in the sense that the stimuli presented to the
subject provide only the possible choices and the contingencies are arbitrary.
The SMR-based system relies on improvement of user performance as a result of prac-
tice (McFarland et al. (2003)). This approach views the user and system as the interaction
of two dynamic processes (Taylor et al. (2002); Wolpaw et al. (2000a)), and can be best
conceptualized as coadaptive. By this view, the goal of the BCI system is to vest control in
those signal features that the user can most accurately modulate and optimize the transla-
tion of these signals into device control. This optimization is presumed to facilitate further
learning by the user.
Our first reports of SMR use to control a BCI used a single feature to control cursor
movement in one dimension to hit a target located at the top or bottom edge of a video
monitor (Wolpaw et al. (1991)). In 1993 we demonstrated that users could learn to control
the same type of cursor movement to intercept targets starting at a variable height and
moving from left to right across the screen (McFarland et al. (1993)). Subsequently, we
used two channels of EEG to control cursor movement independently in two dimensions
so users could hit targets located at one of the four corners of the monitor (Wolpaw and
McFarland (1994)). We also evaluated using one-dimensional cursor control with two to
five targets arranged along the right edge of the monitor (McFarland et al. (2003)). This
task is illustrated in figure 2.1a. Cursor control in these examples was based on a weighted
sum of one or two spectral features for each control dimension. For example, an increase in
the amplitude of the 10Hz μ rhythm, located over the sensorimotor cortex (electrode C3),
could move the target up and a decrease in the amplitude of this μ-rhythm could serve to
46. 2.3 Sensorimotor Rhythm-Based Cursor Control 33
a
b
Figure 2.1 (a) One-dimensional four-target SMR control task (McFarland et al. (2003)). (b) Two-
dimensional eight target SMR control task (Wolpaw and McFarland (2004)). (1) The target and cursor
are present on the screen for 1 s. (2a) The cursor moves steadily across the screen for 2 s with its
vertical movement controlled by the user. (2b) The cursor moves in two dimensions with direction
and velocity controlled by the user until the user hits the target or 10 s have elapsed. (3) The target
flashes for 1.5 s when it is hit by the cursor. If the cursor misses the target, the screen is blank for 1.5
s. (4) The screen is blank for a 1-s interval. (5) The next trial begins.
move the target down. In this case, feature selection was based on inspection of univariate
statistics.
We found that a regression approach is well suited to SMR cursor movement since
it provides continuous control in one or more dimensions and generalizes well to novel
target configurations. The utility of a regression model is illustrated in the recent study of
SMR control of cursor movement in two dimensions described in Wolpaw and McFarland
(2004). An example trial is shown in figure 2.1b. A trial began when a target appeared at
one of eight locations on the periphery of the screen. Target location was block-randomized
(i.e., each occurred once every eight trials). One second later, the cursor appeared in the
middle of the screen and began to move in two dimensions with its movement controlled
by the user’s EEG activity. If the cursor reached the target within 10 s, the target flashed
as a reward. If it failed to reach the target within 10 s, the cursor and the target simply
disappeared. In either case, the screen was blank for one second, and then the next trial
began. Users initially learned cursor control in one dimension (i.e., horizontal) based on a
regression function. Next they were trained on a second dimension (i.e., vertical) using a
different regression function. Finally the two functions were used simultaneously for full
two-dimensional control. Topographies of Pearson’s r correlation values for one user are
shown in figure 2.2, where it can be seen that two distinct patterns of activity controlled
cursor movement. Horizontal movement was controlled by a weighted difference of 12-Hz
μ-rhythm activity between the left and right sensorimotor cortex (see figure 2.2, left
topography). Vertical movement was controlled by a weighted sum of activity located
47. 34 Noninvasive Brain-Computer Interface Research at the Wadsworth Center
Figure 2.2 Scalp topographies (nose at top) of Pearson’s r values for horizontal (x) and vertical (y)
target positions. In this user, horizontal movement was controlled by a 12-Hz μ-rhythm and vertical
movement by a 24-Hz β-rhythm. Horizontal correlation is greater on the right side of the scalp,
whereas vertical correlation is greater on the left side of the scalp. The topographies are for R rather
than R2 to show the opposite (i.e., positive and negative, respectively) correlations of right and left
sides with horizontal target level (Wolpaw and McFarland (2004)).
over left and right sensorimotor cortex in the 24-Hz β-rhythm (see figure 2.2, right
topography). This study illustrated the generalizability of regression functions to varying
target configurations.
This 2004 study also determined how well users could move the cursor to novel loca-
tions. Targets were presented at sixteen possible locations consisting of the original eight
targets and eight additional targets that were on the periphery in the spaces between the
original eight and not overlapping with them. Target location was block-randomized (i.e.,
each occurred once in sixteen trials). The average movement times to the original locations
was compared with the average movement times to the novel locations. In the first of these
sessions, movement time was slightly but not significantly longer for the novel targets,
and this small difference decreased with practice. These results illustrated that ordinary
least-squares regression procedures provide efficient models that generalize to novel tar-
get configurations. Regression provides an efficient means to parameterize the translation
algorithm in an adaptive manner that smoothly transfers to different target configurations
during the course of multistep training protocols. This study clearly demonstrated strong
simultaneous independent control of horizontal and vertical movement. This control was
comparable in accuracy and speed to that reported in studies using implanted intracortical
electrodes in monkeys (Wolpaw and McFarland (2004)).
We have also evaluated various regression models for controlling cursor movement
acquired from a four-choice, one-dimensional cursor movement task (McFarland and
Wolpaw (2005)). We found that using more than one EEG feature improved performance
(e.g., C4 at 12Hz and C3 at 24Hz). In addition, we evaluated nonlinear models with linear
regression by including cross-product (i.e., interaction) terms in the regression function.
While the translation algorithm could be based on either a classifier or a regression
function, we concluded that a regression approach was more appropriate for the cursor
48. 2.3 Sensorimotor Rhythm-Based Cursor Control 35
Figure 2.3 Comparison of regression and classification for feature translation. For the two-target
case, both methods require only one function. For the five-target case, the regression approach still
requires only a single function, while the classification approach requires four functions (see text for
full discussion).
movement task. Figure 2.3 compares the classification and regression approaches. For the
two-target case, both the regression approach and the classification approach require that
the parameters of a single function be determined. For the five-target case, the regression
approach still requires only a single function when the targets are distributed along a single
dimension (e.g., vertical position on the screen). In contrast, for the five-target case the
classification approach requires that four functions be parameterized. With even more and
variable targets, the advantage of the regression approach becomes increasingly apparent.
For example, the positioning of icons in a typical mouse-based graphical user interface
would require a bewildering array of classifying functions, while with the regression
approach, two dimensions of cursor movement and a button selection serve all cases.
We have conducted preliminary studies that suggest users are also able to accurately
control a robotic arm in two dimensions by applying the same techniques used for cursor
control. A more recent study shows that after encountering a target with the cursor, users
are able to select or reject the target by performing or withholding hand-grasp imagery
(McFarland et al. (2005)). This imagery evokes a transient response that can be detected
and used to improve the overall accuracy by reducing unintended target selections. As
these results illustrate, training of SMRs has the potential to be extended to a variety of
applications, and the control obtained for one task can transfer directly to another task.
Our current efforts toward improving the SMR paradigm are refining the one- and two-
dimensional control procedures with the intention of progressing to more choices and
to higher dimensional control. This includes the identification or transformation of EEG
features so that the resulting control signals are as independent, trainable, stable, and
49. 36 Noninvasive Brain-Computer Interface Research at the Wadsworth Center
a b
Figure 2.4 (a) A 6 × 6 P300 matrix display. The rows and columns are randomly highlighted as
indicated by column 3. (b) Average waveforms for each of the 36 cells contained in the matrix from
electrode Pz. The target letter “O” (thick waveform) elicited the largest P300 response, and a smaller
P300 response is evident for the other characters in column 3 or row 3 (medium waveforms) because
these stimuli are highlighted simultaneously with the target. All other cells indicate nontarget stimuli
(thin waveforms). Each response is the average of 30 stimulus presentations.
predictable as possible. With control signals possessing these traits, the user and system
adaptations should be superior, and thus the required training time should be reduced and
overall performance improved.
2.4 P300-Based Communication
We have also begun to use and further develop the potential of the P300 class of BCI
systems. In the original P300 matrix paradigm introduced by Farwell and Donchin (1988),
the user is presented with a 6 × 6 matrix containing 36 symbols. The user focuses
attention on the desired symbol in the matrix while the rows and columns of the matrix
are highlighted in a random sequence of flashes. A P300 response occurs when the desired
symbol is highlighted. To identify the desired symbol, the classifier determines the row and
the column that the user is attending to (i.e., the symbol that elicited a P300) by weighting
specific spatiotemporal features that are time-locked to the stimulus. The intersection of
this row and column defines the selected symbol. Figure 2.4 shows a typical P300 matrix
display and the averaged event-related potential responses to the intensification of each
cell. The cell containing the letter “O” was the target cell and elicited the largest P300
response when highlighted. To a lesser extent the other characters in the row or the column
containing the O also elicited a P300 because these cells are simultaneously highlighted
with the target cell.
Our focus has been on improving matrix speller classification. These studies examined
variables related to stimulus properties, presentation rate, classification parameters, and
classification methods. Sellers et al. (2006a) examined the effects of matrix size and
interstimulus interval (ISI) on classification accuracy using two matrix sizes (3 × 3 and
50. 2.4 P300-Based Communication 37
Figure 2.5 Montages used to derive SWDA classification coefficients. Data were collected from
all 64 electrodes; only the indicated electrodes were used to derive coefficients (see text).
6 × 6), and two ISIs (175 and 350 ms). The results showed that the amplitude of the
P300 response for the target items was larger in the 6 × 6 matrix condition than in the
3 × 3 matrix condition. These results are consistent with a large number of studies that
show increased P300 amplitude with reduced target probability (e.g., Duncan-Johnson and
Donchin (1977)).
Our lab has tested several variables related to classification accuracy using the stepwise
discriminant analysis (SWDA) method (Krusienski et al. (2005)). We examined the effects
of channel set, channel reference, decimation factor, and the number of model features
on classification accuracy (Krusienski et al. (2005)). The factor of channel set was the
only factor to have a statistically significant effect on classification accuracy. Figure 2.5
shows examples of each electrode set. Set 1 (Fz, Cz, and Pz) and set 2 (PO7, PO8, and
Oz) performed equally, and significantly worse than set 3 (set 1 and set 2 combined). In
addition, set 4 (which contained 19 electrodes) was no better than set 3 (which contained
6 electrodes).
These results demonstrate at least two important points: First, 19 electrode locations
appear to provide no more useful information beyond that provided by the 6 electrodes
contained in set 3. Second, electrode locations other than those traditionally associated with
the P300 response provide unique information for classification of matrix data. Occipital
51. 38 Noninvasive Brain-Computer Interface Research at the Wadsworth Center
a
b
Time (ms)
Figure 2.6 (a) Example waveforms for target (black) and nontarget (grey) stimuli for electrodes
PO7, Pz, and PO8. The target waveform represents the average of 480 stimuli and the nontarget
waveform represents the average of 2400 stimuli. The P300 response is evident at Pz and a negative
deflection preceding the P300 is evident at PO7 and PO8. (b) r2
values that correspond to the
waveforms shown in panel a.
electrodes (e.g., Oz, PO7, and PO8) have previously been included in matrix speller data
classification (Kaper et al. (2004); Meinicke et al. (2002)). In addition, Vaughan et al.
(2003a) showed that these electrode locations discriminate target from nontarget stimuli, as
measured by r2
, but the nature of the information provided by the occipital electrodes has
not been rigorously investigated. Examination of the waveforms suggests that a negative
deflection preceding the P300 response provides this additional unique information (see
figure 2.6a).
While a relationship to gaze cannot be ruled out at this time, it is likely that the essential
classification-specific information recorded from the occipital electrodes is not produced
because the user fixates the target item. An exogenous response to a stimulus occurs
within the first 100 ms of stimulus presentation and appears as a positive deflection in
the waveform (Skrandies (2005)). In contrast, the response observed at PO7 and PO8
is a negative deflection that occurs after 200 ms. The r2
values remain near zero until
approximately 200 ms, also suggesting a negligible exogenous contribution. Moreover,
whether or not this negativity is specific to the matrix style display or also present in
standard P300 tasks is yet to be determined.
While it is reasonable to assume that the user must be able to fixate for the response
to be elicited, Posner (1980) has shown that nonfixated locations can be attended to. To
our knowledge, P300-BCI studies that examine the consequences of attending to a location
other than the fixated location have not been conducted. Furthermore, one may also assume
that fixating a nontarget location may have a deleterious effect on performance because it
is harder to ignore distractor items located at fixation than it is to ignore distractor items
located in the periphery (Beck and Lavie (2005)). At the same time, fixation alone is not
sufficient to elicit a P300 response. Evidence for this is provided by studies that present
target and nontarget items at fixation in a Bernoulli series (e.g., Fabiani et al. (1987)).
If fixation alone were responsible for the P300, both the target and nontarget items would
52. 2.5 A Portable BCI System 39
produce equivalent responses because all stimuli are presented at fixation. Hence, we argue
that a visual P300-BCI is not classifying gaze in a fashion analogous to the Sutter (1992)
steady-state visually evoked potential system.
To be useful a BCI must be accurate. Accurate classification depends on feature extrac-
tion and the translation algorithm being used for classification (Krusienski et al. (2005)).
Currently, we are testing several alternative classification methods in addition to SWDA.
To date, we have tested classifiers derived from linear support vector machines, Gaussian
support vector machines, Pearson’s correlation method, Fisher’s linear discriminant, and
SWDA. The preliminary results reveal minimal differences among several different classi-
fication algorithms. The SWDA method we have been using for our online studies perform
as well as, or better than, any of the other solutions we have tested offline (Krusienski et al.
(2006)).
2.5 A Portable BCI System
In addition to refining and improving SMR- and P300-BCI performance we are also
focused on developing clinically practical BCI systems. We are beginning to provide
severely disabled individuals with BCI systems to use in their daily lives. Our goals are
to demonstrate that the BCI systems can be used for everyday communication and that
using a BCI has a positive impact on the user’s quality of life (Vaughan et al. (2006)).
In collaboration with researchers at the University of Tübingen and the University of
South Florida, we have conducted many experimental sessions at the homes of disabled
individuals (e.g., Kübler et al. (2005a); Sellers and Donchin (2006); Sellers et al. (2006c)).
This pilot work has identified critical factors essential for moving out of the lab and into
a home setting where people can use a BCI in an autonomous fashion. The most pressing
needs for a successful home BCI system are developing a more compact system, making
the system easy to operate for a caregiver, and providing the user with effective and reliable
communication applications.
The current home system includes a laptop computer, a flat panel display, an eight-
channel electrode cap, and an amplifier with a built in A/D board. The amplifier has been
reduced to 15 × 4 × 9 cm, and we anticipate a smaller amplifier in the future. We have
addressed making the system more user-friendly by automating some of the processes
in the BCI2000 software and employing a novice user level that allows the caregiver to
start the program with a short series of mouse clicks. Thus, the caregiver’s major task
is placing and injecting gel into the electrode cap, which takes about five minutes. We
have also modified the BCI2000 software to include a menu-driven item selection structure
that allows the user to navigate various hierarchical menus to perform specific tasks (e.g.,
basic communication, basic needs, word processing, and environmental controls) in a more
expedient manner than earlier versions of the SMR (Vaughan et al. (2001)) and P300
(Sellers et al. (2006c)) software. In addition, we incorporated a speech output option for
users who desire this ability. A more complete description of the system is provided in
Vaughan et al. (2006).
53. 40 Noninvasive Brain-Computer Interface Research at the Wadsworth Center
Finally, we have provided one severely disabled user with an in-home P300 system that
he uses for daily work and communication tasks. He is a 48-year-old man with amyotrophic
lateral sclerosis (ALS) who is totally paralyzed except for some eye movement. Since
installation, the BCI has been used at least five times per week for up to eight hours per
day. The format is a 9 × 8 matrix of letters, numbers, and function calls that operates as
a keyboard and makes the computer and Windows-based programs (e.g., Eudora, Word,
Excel, PowerPoint, Acrobat) completely accessible via EEG control. The system uses an
ISI of 125 ms with a stimulus duration of 62.5 ms, and each series of intensifications lasts
for 12.75 s. On a weekly basis the data is uploaded to an ftp site and analyzed in the lab,
and classification coefficients are updated via our previously described SWDA procedure
(Krusienski et al. (2005); Sellers and Donchin (2006); Sellers et al. (2006a)). The user’s
average classification accuracy for all experimental sessions has been 88 percent. These
results have demonstrated that a P300-BCI can be of practical value for individuals with
severe motor disabilities, and that caregivers who are unfamiliar with BCI devices and EEG
signals can be trained to operate and maintain a BCI (Sellers et al. (2006c)). We plan to
enroll additional users in the coming months.
2.6 Discussion
The primary goal of the Wadsworth BCI is to provide a new communication channel for
severely disabled people. As demonstrated here, the SMR and P300 systems employ very
different approaches to achieve this goal. The SMR system relies on EEG features that
are spontaneous in the sense that the stimuli presented to the user provide information
regarding SMR modulation. In contrast, the P300 response is elicited by a stimulus con-
tained within a predefined set of stimuli and depends on the oddball paradigm (Fabiani
et al. (1987)). The SMR system uses features extracted by spectral analysis while the P300
system uses time-domain features. While the P300 can be characterized in the frequency
domain (e.g., Cacace and McFarland (2003)), to our knowledge, this has not been done for
P300-BCI use.
We use regression analysis with the SMR system and classification for the P300 system.
The regression approach is well suited to the SMR cursor movement application since it
provides continuous control in one or more dimensions and generalizes well to novel target
configurations (McFarland and Wolpaw (2005)). In contrast, the classification approach
is well suited to the P300 system where the target is treated as one class and all other
alternatives are treated as the other class. Done in this way, a single discriminant function
generalizes well to matrices of differing sizes.
Finally, these two BCI systems differ in terms of the importance of user training. BCI
users can learn to control SMRs to move a computer cursor to hit targets located on a
computer screen. This is not a normal function of this brain signal, but, rather, is the
result of training. In contrast, the P300 can be used for communication purposes without
extensive training. The SMR system relies on improvement of user performance as a result
of practice (McFarland et al. (2003)), while the P300 system uses a response that appears
to remain relatively constant across trials in terms of waveform morphology (Cohen
54. 2.6 Discussion 41
User User User
BCI system BCI system
BCI system
Machine
learning conditioning
Operant
coadaptation
Optimized
Figure 2.7 Three concepts of BCI operation. The arrows through the user and/or the BCI system
indicate which elements adapt in each concept.
and Polich (1997); Fabiani et al. (1987); Polich (1989)) and classification coefficient
performance (Sellers and Donchin (2006); Sellers et al. (2006a)). An SMR-BCI system
is more suitable for continuous control tasks such as moving a cursor on a screen; although
Piccione et al. (2006) have shown that a P300 system can be used to move a cursor in
discrete steps, albeit more slowly than with an SMR system.
While most BCI researchers agree that coadaptation between user and system is a central
concept, BCI systems have been conceptualized in at least three ways. Blankertz et al.
(e.g., Blankertz et al. (2003)) view BCI to be mainly a problem of machine learning;
this view implicitly sees the user as producing a predictable signal that needs to be
discovered. Birbaumer et al. (e.g., Birbaumer et al. (2003)) view BCI to be mainly an
operant conditioning paradigm, in which the experimenter, or trainer, guides or leads the
user to encourage the desired output by means of reinforcement. Wolpaw et al. (2000a)
and Taylor et al. (2002) view the user and BCI system as the coadaptive interaction of two
dynamic processes. Figure 2.7 illustrates these three views of BCI. The Wadsworth Center
SMR system falls most readily into the coadaptive class, while the Wadsworth Center P300
system is most analogous to the machine learning model. Ultimately, determining which
of these views (or other conceptualizations of BCI systems) is most appropriate must be
empirically evaluated for each BCI paradigm.
We feel that one should allow the characteristics of the EEG feature(s) to dictate the
BCI system design and this will determine the most effective system for a given user.
We currently test users on the SMR- and P300-based BCI systems and then select the
most appropriate system based on analyses of speed, accuracy, bit rate, usefulness, and
likelihood of use (Nijboer et al. (2005)). This may prove to be the most efficient model as
we move BCI systems into people’s homes.
56. filled with stagnant water which would have done no discredit to
that of the lower Thames. Here we outspanned, and here we
breakfasted.
These breakfasts under the dome of heaven are not to be
looked back upon with rapture. Picnicking is an excellent relaxation
in England, but a picnic without shade, without cooling drinks,
without pasties and salads and jellies and pies, without white
tablecloths and bright knives, without even shelter from incessant
dust, lacks much. Tinned provisions are, no doubt, excellent and
nourishing, but oh, the weariness of them! And oh, the squalor of
the single tin mug, which never loses the taste of what it last had in
it! And oh, the meanness of the one tin plate which does duty for
every meal, and every phase of it! Perhaps of all unappetising
adjuncts to a breakfast the tin of preserved milk, which has been
opened two days and is already becoming disgustingly familiar, is the
most aggressive. The hot climate and the indefatigable ant and the
fly do little to make the items of a meal attractive.
What does not rapidly decompose promptly dries up. On one
occasion a roasted fowl was brought up reverently to Frere in a tin
box, but when it came to be eaten it had dried into a sort of papier
mâchê roast fowl, and was like the viands which are thrown at the
police at pantomimes. We brought many varieties of preserved food
with us, and of much of it the question could not fail to arise as to
whether it had ever been worth preserving.
Many had experience, too, of the inventive art of the
shopkeeper, as shown in the evolution of canteens and pocket table
knives. The canteen, when unstrapped, tends to fall into a hundred
parts, and can never be put together again. It is a prominent or
57. generic feature of most canteens that the kettle should look as little
like a kettle as possible, and that everything should pack into a
frying-pan. The pocket picnicking knife contains a knife, a fork, a
spoon, and a corkscrew. The fork runs into everything and prevents
the knife from being carried in the pocket. The spoon and fork are
jointed for more convenient stowing, and at crises in a meal they are
apt to bend weakly in the middle and then to incontinently shut up.
The outspanning and the inspanning at Pretorius' Farm occupied
over two hours, and then the march was resumed. A better country
was reached as we neared the river, and it was a pleasant sight to
see the tumbling stream of the Lesser Tugela, and to find in one
valley the pretence of a garden and a house among trees. This was
at Springfield, which place we reached at 2.30 P.M. The march of
some eighteen miles had therefore been effected in two treks.
At Springfield the camping ground was the least dreary of any
the hospital had experience of, and the proximity of the Lesser
Tugela made bathing possible.
After a few days at Springfield we moved on to Spearman's
Farm, where we camped by the hill called Mount Alice.
The return from Spearman's after Spion Kop and Vaal Krantz
was even more monotonous than the going forth. The first journey
was to Springfield, which was reached at sundown, and where we
bivouacked for the night. Springfield was left at dawn, and the next
night was spent in a bivouac at Frere. On the following day, before
the sun was well up, we took the last stage of the march and
reached Chieveley. Here all enjoyed once more the luxury of having
tents overhead, for during the crawling journey over the veldt we
slept in wagons, on wagons, or under wagons.
58. XV
SPEARMAN'S FARM
In a lonely valley under the mimosa-covered heights which dominate
the Great Tugela is the lonely homestead of Spearman's Farm. Those
who built it and made a home in it could have had little thought that
it would one day figure in the annals of history. The farmhouse and
the farm buildings and the garden were enclosed by a rough stone
wall, and upon this solitary homestead the hand of the Boer had
fallen heavily. The house had been looted, and what was breakable
in it had been broken. The garden had been trampled out of
recognition, the gates were gone, the agricultural implements had
been wantonly destroyed, and the unpretending road which led to
the farm was marked by the wheels of heavy guns. The house was
small and of one story, and was possessed of the unblushing
ugliness which corrugated iron alone can provide. The door swung
open, and any could enter who would, and through the broken
windows there was nothing to be seen but indiscriminate wreckage.
There was about the little house and its cluster of outbuildings a
suggestion of the Old Country, and it wanted but a rick or so, and a
pond with white ducks to complete a picture of a small English farm.
The garden had evidently been the subject of solicitous care, and
was on that account all the more desolate, and what delight it ever
had had been trampled out of it by countless hoofs or obliterated by
the rattling passage over it of a battery or so of artillery.
59. At the back of the farm, and at the foot of a green kopje, was a
quaint little burial ground--little because it held but two graves, and
quaint because these were surmounted by unexpected stone
memorials of a type to be associated with a suburban English
cemetery. These monuments were fitly carved, and were distinctly
the product of no mean town, and they were to the memory
respectively of George Spearman and of Susan Spearman. For some
undefinable reason these finished memorials, so formal and so
hackneyed in their design, appeared inappropriate and even
unworthy of the dignity of the lonely graves at the foot of the kopje.
Some more rugged emblem, free from artificiality and from any
suggestion of the crowded haunts of men, would have covered more
fittingly the last resting-place of these two pioneers. A few trees,
almost the only trees within sight, shaded the little graveyard, and
the trees and the monuments were enclosed by a very solid iron
railing. It was in the shadow of this oasis that the dead from our
hospital were buried.
XVI
THE HOSPITAL AT SPEARMAN'S
The hospital reached Spearman's on January 16th, and was pitched
at the foot of the hill, upon the summit of which the naval gun was
firing. We were, therefore, close to those scenes of fighting which
were to occupy the next few weeks, and too close for comfort to the
60. great 4.7 gun, the repeated booming of which often became a
trouble to those who were lying ill in the hospital.
The heights that dominated the southern bank of the Tugela
were very steep on the side that faced the river, but on the side that
looked towards Spearman's the ground sloped gradually down into a
wide plain which, like other stretches of veldt, was dotted with
kopjes and slashed with dongas. Anyone who mounted the hill at the
back of the hospital would come by easy steps to an abrupt ridge,
beyond which opened a boundless panorama.
In the valley below this crest was the winding Tugela, and just
across the dip rose the solemn ridge of Spion Kop. Far away in the
distance were the purple hills which overshadowed Ladysmith. If the
crest were followed to the right the ground rose until at last the
summit of the naval hill was reached, and here were the handy
men and their big gun. From this high eminence a splendid view
was obtained of the country we desired once more to possess. The
Tugela glistened in the sun like a band of silver, and over the plain
and in and out among the kopjes and round the dongas the brown
road wound to Ladysmith. The road was deserted, and the few
homesteads which came into view showed no signs of life. At the
foot of the hill was Potgieter's Drift, while above the ford was a
splashing rapid, and below was the pont which our men had seized
with such daring.
The face of the hill towards the river was covered with mimosa
trees and with cactus bushes and aloes, and this unexpected wealth
of green almost hid the red and grey boulders which clung to the
hill-side. Among the rocks were many strange flowers, many
unfamiliar plants, and creeping things innumerable. This was a
61. favourite haunt of the chameleon, and I believe it was here that the
hospital chameleon was captured.
The quiet of the place, when the guns had ceased, was
absolute, and was only broken by the murmur of the numerous
doves which occupied the mimosa woods. The whole place seemed
a paradise of peace, and there was nothing to suggest that there
were some thousands of grimy men beyond the river who were busy
with the implements of death. On looking closely one could see
brown lines along many of the hillsides, and these said lines were
trenches, and before the hubbub began men in their shirt-sleeves
could be seen working about them with pickaxes and shovels.
I should imagine that few modern battles have been viewed by
the casual onlooker at such near proximity and with such
completeness in detail as were the engagements of Spion Kop and
Vaal Krantz, when viewed from the high ground above our hospital.
The hospital, though now more than twenty-five miles from the
railway, was very well supplied with almost every necessity and with
the amplest stores of food. Bread was not to be obtained, or only on
occasion, when it would be brought up by an ambulance on its
return from Frere. We had with us, however, our flocks and herds,
and were thus able to supply the sick and wounded with fresh milk,
and the whole hospital with occasional fresh meat. We were a little
short of water, and fuel was not over abundant. As a result the
washing of clothes, towels and sheets presented the same type of
problem as is furnished by the making of bricks without straw. The
aspect of a flannel shirt that has been washed by a Kaffir on the
remote veldt leaves on the mind the impression that the labour of
the man has been in vain.
62. Our stay at Spearman's was extended to three weeks, and we
dealt with over a thousand wounded during that period, and I am
sure that all those who came within our lines would acknowledge
that at No. 4 they found an unexpected degree of comfort and
were in every way well done for.
On the Sunday after our arrival the wounded began to come in.
Thirteen only came from the division posted at Potgieter's Drift, the
rest came from Sir Charles Warren's column. Increasing numbers of
wounded came in every day in batches of from fifty to one hundred
and fifty. They were all attended to, and were sent on to Frere as
soon as possible. All the serious cases, however, were kept in the
hospital.
XVII
THE TWO WHITE LIGHTS
Many of the wounded who were brought in between the 18th and
the 24th of January came in after sundown. The largest number
arrived on the night of Monday, the 22nd. It was a very dark night.
The outline of the tents and marquees was shadowy and faint. The
camp was but the ghost of a camp. Here and there a feeble light
would be shining through the fly of a marquee, and here and there
an orderly, picking his way among the tent ropes by the aid of a
lantern, would light up a row or two in the little canvas town. In the
front of the camp was the flagstaff, high up upon which were
suspended the two white lights which marked the situation of the
63. hospital. These lamps only sufficed to illumine a few of the tents in
the first line. The flaps of these tents were probably secured and the
occupants asleep.
It was a weary journey to the hospital, and one can imagine
with what eagerness the tired, hungry, aching wounded would look
ahead for the two white lights. Rocking in pain on a crawling ox
wagon, or jolted in the rigid fabric of an ambulance, the way must
have seemed unending. Tumbling along in the dark, with no sound
but the creaking of the wagon and the incessant moans of the
shapeless, huddled figures who were lying in the cart, the journey
might well have been one never to be forgotten. How many a time a
tired head must have been lifted up from the straw to see if there
were yet any sign of the two white lights. Would the journey never
end, and the pain never cease? and was the broken limb to be
wrenched every time the blundering wagon pitched and rolled? And
why had the man who had talked so much ceased to speak--and
indeed to breathe? Would they drive through the dark for eternity?
and would they never come in view of the two white lights?
It was a miserable sight to see these belated wagons come in,
and they would often rumble in all night. They emerged one by one
out of the darkness and drew up in the open space between the two
central lines of tents, and between the few uplifted lanterns held by
the sergeants and the men on duty. After they had deposited their
load they moved away and vanished again into the night.
Some of the wounded in the wagons were sitting up, but the
majority were lying on the straw with which the wagon would be
littered. Some were asleep and some were dead; and by the light of
the lanterns the wagon seemed full of khaki-coloured bundles, vague
64. in outline and much stained with blood, with here and there an
upraised bandage, and here and there a wandering hand, or a leg in
crude splints, or a bare knee. And round about all a medley of rifles,
boots, haversacks, helmets, cartridge pouches and tin canteens.
What the journey must have been to many I could gather from
an incident of one of these dreary nights. A wagon had reached the
hospital lines and was waiting to be unloaded. A man with a
shattered arm in a sling was sitting up, and at his feet a comrade
was lying who had been very hard hit, and who had evidently
become weaker and less conscious as the wagon had rolled along.
The apparently sleeping man moved, and, lifting his head to look at
his pal, who was sitting above him, asked wearily, for probably the
fiftieth time, Don't you see nothing yet, Bill, of the two white
lights?
XVIII
AFTER SPION KOP
On Wednesday, January 24th, came the terrible affair of Spion Kop.
On the previous day some hint of what was expected was
foreshadowed in the order that an additional hundred bell tents were
to be erected in No. 4 Field Hospital. These tents were obtained
from a brigade who were bivouacking, and were all pitched by
Wednesday afternoon. They represented accommodation for an
additional number of five hundred wounded, and it was, therefore,
evident that an important engagement was at hand.
65. On Thursday the wounded came pouring in, and they came in
the whole day and until late at night, until the hospital was full. The
number admitted on that day was nearly six hundred. Those who
were deposited in the bell tents had to lie on stretchers. All were
provided with blankets. In spite of the immense number of the
wounded, they were all got under shelter by Thursday night, and
had had their more serious injuries attended to, and were made as
comfortable as circumstances would admit. Some of the staff went
round with water and food, and others with morphia, while a third
party made it their business to see that every man was bestowed as
comfortably as extemporised pillows or change of posture could
make him. The pillows were represented by helmets, or by the
happy combination of helmet and boot, or by haversacks or rolled-
up tunics.
The volunteer ambulance corps and the coolie bearers did
excellent service. The larger number of the wounded were on the
top of Spion Kop. The path down was about two miles, was steep,
and in places very difficult. The carriage of the wounded down the
hill had all to be by hand. From the foot of the hill to the hospital the
carriage was by ambulance wagons and in some cases by bearers.
All the stretchers had hoods. There was no doubt that the wounded
suffered much on account of the tedious transport, but it was
rendered as little distressing as possible.
The surgeons who went after the wounded on the top of the hill
told us that the sight of the dead and injured was terrible in the
extreme, the wounds having been mostly from shell and shrapnel;
some men had been blown almost to pieces. The weather on
Wednesday was warm, but was not to be compared with the intense
66. heat on the day of the battle of Colenso. The temperature was that
of a hot summer's day in England. Thursday was fortunately cloudy
and much cooler.
As to the wounded, there was the usual proportion of minor
injuries, but on the whole the wounds were much more severe than
those received at Colenso. This is explained by the large number of
wounds from shell and shrapnel. The men, however, were much
exhausted by the hardships they had undergone. In many instances
they had not had their clothes off for a week or ten days. They had
slept in the open without great-coats, and had been reduced to the
minimum in the matter of rations. The nights were cold, and there
was on nearly every night a heavy dew. Fortunately there was little
or no rain. The want of sleep and the long waiting upon the hill had
told upon them severely. There is no doubt also that the incessant
shell fire must have proved a terrible strain. Some of the men,
although wounded, were found asleep upon their stretchers when
brought in. Many were absolutely exhausted and worn out
independently of their wounds.
In spite of all their hardships the wounded men behaved
splendidly, as they always have done. They never complained. They
were quite touching in their unselfishness and in their anxiety not to
give trouble; but it was evident enough that they were much
depressed at the reverse.
The shell wounds were the most terrible and the most difficult
to treat. One man had most of his face shot away, including both
eyes. Another had the forearm shot off and two fearful wounds of
each thigh dividing the anterior muscles to the bone. In one case a
shrapnel had opened a main artery in the forearm, and the man
67. came down safely with a tourniquet on his brachial artery composed
of a plug of cake tobacco and the tape of a puttie. I cannot help
thinking that this ingenious tourniquet was the work of one of the
handy men.
XIX
THE STORY OF THE RESTLESS MAN
The following incident may serve to illustrate the often-expressed
unselfishness of the soldier, and his anxiety to do what he can for a
comrade in trouble.
Among the wounded who came down from Spion Kop was a
private, a native of Lancashire, who had been shot in the thigh. The
thigh-bone was broken, and the fracture had been much disturbed
by the journey to the hospital. The man was given a bedstead in one
of the marquees; the limb was adjusted temporarily, and he was told
to keep very quiet and not to move off his back. Next morning,
however, he was found lying upon his face, with his limb out of
position and his splints, as he himself confessed, all anyhow. He
was remonstrated with, but excused himself by saying, But you see,
doctor, I am such a restless man.
The limb was more elaborately adjusted, and everything was
left in excellent position. Next morning, however, the restless man
was found lying on the floor of the marquee, and in his bed was a
man who had been shot through the chest. The marquee was
crowded and the number of beds were few, and those who could not
68. be accommodated on beds had to lie on stretchers on the ground.
The man who was shot in the chest had come in in the night, and
had been placed on the only available stretcher. The restless man
proceeded to explain that the newcomer seemed worse off than he
was, and that he thought the man would be easier on the bed, so he
had induced the orderlies to effect the change. The man who was
shot in the chest died suddenly, and in due course the restless man
was back in his own bed once more.
It was not, however, for long, for on another morning visit the
Lancashire lad was found on the floor again, and again beamed forth
an explanation that one of the wounded on the ground, who had
come in late, seemed to be very bad, and so he had changed over.
The present occupant of the bed was in a few days moved down to
the base, and the restless man was in his own bed again. But not
many days elapsed before he discovered among the fresh arrivals an
old chum, who longed to lie on a bed, and thus the good-hearted
North-countryman found himself once more on the floor.
The moving of a man with a broken thigh from a bed to the
ground and back again means not only such disordering of splints
and bandages, but much pain to the patient and no little danger to
the damaged limb. So this generous lad was talked to seriously, and
with a faintly veiled sternness was forbidden to give up his bed again
on any pretence. In the little attempt he made to excuse himself he
returned once more to his original joke and said, with a broad grin:
But you see, doctor, I am such a restless man.
XX
69. DID WE WIN?
One instance of the indomitable pluck of the British soldier deserves
special notice. A private in the King's Royal Rifles, of the name of
Goodman, was brought from Spion Kop to No. 4 Field Hospital in an
ambulance with many others. He was in a lamentable plight when
he arrived. He had been lying on the hill all night. He had not had
his clothes off for six days. Rations had been scanty, and he had
been sleeping in the open since he left the camp. He had been
struck in the face by a fragment of shell, which had carried away his
right eye, the right upper jaw, the corresponding part of the cheek
and mouth, and had left a hideous cavity, at the bottom of which his
tongue was exposed. The rest of his face was streaked with blood,
which was now dried and black--so black that it looked as if tar had
been poured on his head and had streamed down his cheek and
neck. Eight hours had been occupied on the journey to the hospital,
and eight hours is considered to be long even for a railway journey
in a Pullman car.
He was unable to speak, and as soon as he was settled in a tent
he made signs that he wanted to write. A little memorandum book
and a pencil were handed to him, and it was supposed that his
inquiry would be as to whether he would die--what chance he had?
Could he have something to drink? Could anything be done for his
pain? After going through the form of wetting his pencil at what had
once been a mouth, he simply wrote: Did we win? No one had the
heart to tell him the truth.
His memorandum-book--which is in my possession--was used by
him while he remained speechless in the hospital, and certain of the
70. notes he made in it, and which are here appended, speak for
themselves:
Water.
I haven't done bleeding yet.
I've got it this time. I think my right eye is gone, and I can
hardly swallow.
There are no teeth in front.
It aches a lot.
I'm lying the wrong way for my wound.
I found the trenches.
I've had all the officers over to see me.
He is pleased, the doctor.
Did my haversack come with me? If it did, there is some
tobacco in it. You can give it to them that smoke.
Poor Goodman, he had no mouth to smoke with himself. I am
glad to say he reached England, is in good health, and is as cheery
as ever.
XXI
THE FIGHTING SPIRIT
The circumstances under which men enlist in the Army are, no
doubt, varied enough. But not a few find their place under the
colours in obedience to that fighting spirit which has for centuries
been strong in the hearts of the islanders from Great Britain and
Ireland. That spirit has anyhow carried the colours over the world.
71. Among the wounded there are many who, to use an expression
common on the soldiers' lips, were fed up with the war: they had
had enough of it. There were others who were eager to be at it
again, who felt that they had a score to wipe off; and even among
the desperately hurt there would be here and there a man keen for
revenge, and full of a passionate desire to have another go at 'em.
These men, ill as they often were, would describe with a savage
delight, and in savage language, the part they had played in the
battle out of which they had been finally dragged on a stretcher. A
little success, a victory however small, did much to lessen the
torment of a wound and to gild the contemplation of a life
henceforth to be spent as a cripple. One gallant lad had been
paralysed by a Mauser at short range, and had little prospect of
other than permanent lameness. He had been in the assault on Vaal
Krantz, had escaped without hurt until just towards the end, and
was shot as his victorious company were rushing the last trench.
After he had been examined, and while he was still lying on his
stretcher, I could not avoid the remark, This is a bad business. To
which he replied, Yes, but we took the bally trench.
To many and many of the dying the last sound of which they
were conscious must have belonged to the clamour of war, and it
was well for those who heard, or fancied they heard, above the roar
of guns the shout of victory. One officer, dying in the hospital at
Spearman's, had his last moments made happy by the sound of
battle. He had sunk into a state of drowsiness, and was becoming
gradually unconscious. Every now and then the boom of the 4.7 gun,
firing from the hill above us, would rattle through the tents, and with
each shot a smile would come over his face, and he would mutter
72. with great satisfaction, They are getting it now. He repeated these
words many times, and they were, indeed, the last he uttered.
Things were evidently going better with the army in his dream than
they were at that moment with the real regiments by the river.
Some most vivid suggestions of what may pass through the
soldier's mind during the actual circumstances of war were afforded
by the utterances of more or less unconscious men when passing
under the influence of chloroform in the operation-tent. Before they
fell into the state of sleep, it was evident that the drug, with its
subtle intoxicating power, brought back to the fading sense some
flash of a scene which may have been real, but which was rendered
lurid, spectral, and terrifying by the action of the poison. Under this
condition incoherent words of command would be uttered in rapid
tones, full of an agony of eagerness and haste; and cries for help
would be yelled forth in what seemed to be a maniacal frenzy. Many
of the actual utterances that escaped these unconscious lips, and
gave glimpses of a phantom war as seen through the vapor of
chloroform, were too fragmentary to be remembered, but two at
least were muttered with such an emphasis of horror that I took
note of them.
One of the wounded from Spion Kop had evidently engraved
upon his mind the hideous scene of slaughter which the trenches on
that hill presented. As he was being anæsthetised it was apparent
that in his dream he was back again in the trenches, and was once
more among his dead and mangled comrades. The vision of one
wounded man especially haunted him and fascinated him, and at
last he screamed out: There goes that bloke again whose leg was
shot away; blimy, if he ain't crawling now!
73. Another poor fellow had before his eye the spectre of an awful
kopje. His fragmentary utterances made vivid the unearthly land he
was traversing. All who stood by could picture the ghostly kopje, and
could almost share in his anguish when he yelled: There they are
on the hill! For God's sake, shoot! Why don't we shoot?
XXII
THE BODY-SNATCHERS
Early in the campaign Colonel Gallwey, the P.M.O., organised a
volunteer ambulance corps. Two thousand bearers were wanted, and
in a few days two thousand were enrolled. Their duties were to carry
the wounded off the field, to transport serious cases from the
advanced hospitals or dressing stations to the stationary field
hospital, and thence, if need be, to the railway. There were to be
twelve on a stretcher.
This corps contained examples of all sorts and conditions of
men--labourers, mechanics, gentlemen, dock loafers, seamen,
dentists, a chemist or two, a lawyer or two, tram drivers, clerks,
miners, and shop assistants. Many were refugees from the
Transvaal, and the majority had been thrown out of work of some
kind or another by the war. A chance of getting employment had, no
doubt, induced many to enlist, while probably the greater number
were attracted by a spirit of adventure, by a desire to get to the
front and to see something of the pomp and circumstance of war.
74. They formed a strange company when they mustered at
Pietermaritzburg--a section of a street crowd in their everyday
clothes, or in such clothes as were selected for roughing it. There
was immense variety in the matter of hats. Belts were a feature. The
flannel shirt, which was practically de rigueur, was replaced in an
instance or two by a jersey. Collars were not worn; neckties were
optional. There was no fixed fashion in the matter of boots; they
varied from canvas shoes, worthy of a dandy at the seaside, to top
boots fit for a buccaneer.
As to the men themselves, they were of all ages, heights,
shapes, and sizes--the men of a crowd. Some were sunburned, and
some were pale. Some were indifferent, but most were eager. Some
were disposed to assume a serious military bearing, while others
appeared to regard the venture as a silly joke of which they were
beginning to be a little ashamed.
There is no doubt that the corps was in appearance not
impressive. They were wild and shabby looking, disordered,
unsymmetrical, and bizarre. They were scoffed at; and acquired the
not unkindly meant title of the body-snatchers. Later on the
exuberant invention of the soldier dignified them by the titles of the
catch-'em-alive-oh's or the pick-me-ups.
It is needless to say that a good number of unsuitable and
undesirable men had found their way into the ranks. These were
gradually weeded out, and under the discreet command of Major
Wright the corps improved day by day, until the time Spearman's
was reached they formed a very efficient, reliable, and handy body
of men. They did splendid service, and one which was keenly
appreciated. They were the means of saving many lives and an
75. infinite amount of pain. Their longest tramp, of which I had
knowledge, was from Spearman's to Frere, a distance of twenty-five
miles. They showed the usual British indifference under fire, and
went without hesitancy wherever they were led. Unfortunately it
happened that many of the worthy body-snatchers were wounded,
and not a few of them were killed.
In the early days of their career the catch-'em-alive-oh's fell
upon bad times. They knew little of camp life, and less of the art of
getting the most out of it. They had no organisation among
themselves, and many were incompetent to shift alone. They began
as a mob, and they tried to live as a mob, and the result was that
about the time of Colenso they had little comfort but that which is
said by the moralist to be derived from labour. In their camp after
the battle they had time to settle down. They entered the camp a
thriftless crowd, and came out of it a company of handy men.
They were popular with the soldiers. They had the gift of
tongues of a kind, and could compete with most in the matter of
lurid language. Their incessant hunger and indiscriminate thirst were
a matter for admiration. They were good-hearted, and, although
they looked wild, they meant well. Many a wounded man has been
rocked to sleep on their stretchers, and on more than one dying ear
the last sound that fell was the tramp of their untidy feet.
XXIII
SEEING THEM OFF
76. On the afternoon of Thursday, February 8th, the news came to the
hospital at Spearman's that the army was once more to retire, and
signs were already abroad to show that the retreat had commenced.
At the same time an order arrived to the effect that all the wounded
were to be moved at sunrise on the following day to Frere. Our stay
at Spearman's--extended now to three weeks--had therefore come
to an end.
Among those left in the hospital were 150 patients whose
condition was more or less serious. They had been kept under care
as long as possible in order to avoid or postpone the danger of the
long journey to the base. It was determined that these 150 men
should be carried down to Frere on stretchers and by hand. And this
was done, and well done, by the much-ridiculed corps of body-
snatchers.
It was no light undertaking, for the distance was twenty-five
miles, and the road was dusty and not of the best. Every step had to
be tramped under a glaring sun, and the heat of that day was great.
Allowing twelve men to a stretcher, 1,800 men would be required.
This number was forthcoming at sunrise, and they accomplished the
march in the day, reaching Frere at sundown. This was a splendid
piece of work.
It is not hard to surmise what would have happened to many of
those who were the most ill if their journey to Frere had been by the
ox-wagon, or by the still less easy ambulance. As it was, the whole
convoy went down with comfort, and only one man died on the way,
and he had indeed just reached his journey's end when his life
ebbed away.
77. Long before sunrise on the morning of the departure from
Spearman's the hospital was astir; and while it was yet dark lights
could be seen in most of the tents, and lanterns carried by orderlies
or coolies were moving here and there among the grey lines. The
two white lights which hung from the flag-pole in front of the
hospital were still shining. By the time the shadows had vanished
and the light of the dawn fell upon No. 4, it was in a state of
untidy turmoil. Everyone was on the alert to see them off.
In the marquees the last dressings were being carried out by
candle-light. Clothes were being got together; helpless men were
being dressed; blankets were being rolled up, and such comforts as
the hospital could provide were being packed for the wounded to
take with them on their journey. Cherished possessions were being
dragged out from under pillows, to be safely disposed in a haversack
or a boot. The grey light fell upon orderlies in their shirt sleeves
bustling from tent to tent; upon piles of provision cases and of
forage which were being turned out; upon heaps of stretchers; upon
the rolled-up kit of the Army Medical Corps men; upon melancholy
coolies who had been up all night, and were still crawling about, and
were still in their night attire. This night outfit would consist,
probably, of a turban, a mealie sack round the neck, and a decayed
army mackintosh on the body; or of a turban, a frock-coat, which
might at one time have graced Bond Street, and bare legs. Here and
there in the indistinct light would be seen the white apron and trim
dress of a nurse, who still carried the lantern she had had with her
since the small hours of the morning. All were anxious to be up in
time to see them off.
78. In due course, and even yet before the sun could be seen, the
Volunteer Ambulance Corps began to form up outside the camp.
They were nearly two thousand strong, and they were a wild-looking
company. There was, however, more uniformity in their clothing
now, because they had been supplied with khaki tunics, and with
occasional khaki trousers. Some wore putties, some gaiters, and
some had tucked their trousers inside their socks. A few had cut
their trousers off about the knee and were distinguished by bare
legs. A gaiter on one leg and a puttie on the other was not
considered to be in any way démodé. Their hats were still very
varied, but many had possessed themselves of helmets which had
been picked up on the field. Uniformity and smartness could,
however, not be expected if one man wore a helmet and the next a
tam-o'-shanter, the third a bowler hat, and the fourth a squasher
or a headpiece of his own designing. They had red-cross brassards
on their left arms, but these had become merely fluttering bits of
colouring.
This weird corps carried their possessions with them, and it was
evident that in transporting their impedimenta they had appreciated
the value of the division of labour. Many had military water-bottles,
which they had probably picked up. Others carried their water in
glass bottles, which dangled from their waists. Hanging about their
bodies by strings or straps would be various useful domestic articles.
Attached to one man would be a bundle of firewood, to another a
saucepan, to a third a kettle and a lantern. Here a man would have
in the place of a sabre-tache a biscuit tin suspended by a cord, or a
hatchet and a tin-opener, or a spare pair of boots, which swung
bravely as he marched. A popular vade mecum was an empty jam
79. tin (much blackened by the smoke of the camp fire) with a wire
handle, and evidence that it represented a cooking-pot. Belts,
knives, sticks, overcoats, rolled-up mackintoshes, and a general tint
of sunburn and dirt completed the uniform of this strange company.
Before they entered the camp the wounded had been brought
out on stretchers. The stretchers were placed on the grass, side by
side, in long rows which extended across the breadth of the hospital.
The men lying on them were not pleasant to look at. They formed a
melancholy array of bad cases. Each man was covered by a brown
blanket, and within the hood of the stretcher were his special
belongings, his boots and his haversack, and, with them, such
delicacies for the journey as a pot of jam, a chunk of bread, some
biscuits, a lump of tinned meat in a newspaper, and bottles (mostly
with paper corks) containing water or milk or tea. Those on the
stretchers presented bandaged legs and bandaged arms, splints of
all kinds, covered-up eyes and bound-up heads, and the general
paraphernalia of an accident ward. Some of the faces were very
pinched and pale, for pain and loss of blood and exhaustion had
caused the sunburn to fade away.
The light of the dawn fell upon this woe-begone line, and
dazzled the eyes of many with the unaccustomed glare. Those who
were not too ill were in excellent spirits, for this was the first step on
the journey homewards. Such were excited, garrulous and jocular,
and busy with pipes and tobacco. A few were already weary, and
had on their lips the oft-repeated expression that they were fed up
with the war. Many a head was lifted out of the hood to see if any
old chum could be recognised along the line, and from those would
come such exclamations as: Why are you here, Tom? Where have
80. you been hit? Ain't this a real beanfeast? Thought you were stiff.
We're on the blooming move at last.
Many of the men on the stretchers were delirious, and some
were almost unmanageable. One poor fellow was babbling about the
harvest and the time they were having. He was evidently in his
dream once more among the cornfields of England, and among
plenteous beer. Another shook the canvas hood of his stretcher and
declared with vehemence that he would not go in any bally sailing
boat, he was going in a steamer, and the colonel would never let his
men go in a rotten sailing ship. Whereupon he affirmed that he
was going to chuck it, and proceeded to effect his purpose by
rolling off his stretcher.
When the Volunteer Ambulance Corps marched along the line of
stretchers they were the subject of much chaff, and many comments
such as these burst forth: You're being paraded before the General.
So buck up! Pull up yer socks. You with the kettle! Do you take
yourself for a gipsy van? We ain't buying no hardware to-day--go
home. You know there's a Government handicap on this job, and
half a crown to the man who gets in first, so you had better hurry
my stretcher along. And so on; in the dialect of London, of Dublin,
of Lancashire, and of Devon, with infinite variety and with apparent
good spirits.
There were many anxious cases among this crowd on the
stretchers. One, for example, was an Irishman named Kelly, a
private in the Lancashire Fusiliers. He was as plucky a soldier as the
plucky soil of Ireland has ever produced. His right arm had been
smashed on Spion Kop. He had been on the hill two nights; and
when the darkness fell had spent his time in crawling about on the
81. ground, holding the sleeve of his shattered arm between his teeth,
dragging his rifle with his left hand, and searching the bodies of the
dead for any water that may have been left in their water-bottles. He
had lost an incredible amount of blood, and when he reached the
hospital it was necessary to amputate the whole upper limb,
including the shoulder-blade and the collar-bone. He went through
this ordeal with infinite courage and with irrepressible good humour.
He had been the strong man of his regiment and a great boxer, and,
as he casually said, He should miss his arm.
Kelly's spirits were never damped, and he joked on all topics
whenever he had the strength to joke. He was a little difficult to
manage, but was as docile as a lamb in the hands of the Sister who
looked after him, and for whom he had a deep veneration. Nothing
in the ordinary way upset this gallant Irishman, but just before the
convoy started he did for once break down. Two bottles of English
beer had found their way into the camp as a precious gift. Kelly was
promised these bottles to take with him on his journey. In due
course they were deposited in the hood of his stretcher. When his
eyes fell upon the delectable vision of English beer he could stand no
more, and Kelly wept.
I little thought when I saw Kelly off at Spearman's that the next
time I should say good-bye to him would be in a hansom cab in Pall
Mall; but so it was.
When all was ready the stretchers were lifted off the ground in
order, and the bearers filed out of the camp and on to the dusty
track. The morning was like that of a summer's day in England, and
we watched the long convoy creep along the road until it was nearly
82. Welcome to our website – the perfect destination for book lovers and
knowledge seekers. We believe that every book holds a new world,
offering opportunities for learning, discovery, and personal growth.
That’s why we are dedicated to bringing you a diverse collection of
books, ranging from classic literature and specialized publications to
self-development guides and children's books.
More than just a book-buying platform, we strive to be a bridge
connecting you with timeless cultural and intellectual values. With an
elegant, user-friendly interface and a smart search system, you can
quickly find the books that best suit your interests. Additionally,
our special promotions and home delivery services help you save time
and fully enjoy the joy of reading.
Join us on a journey of knowledge exploration, passion nurturing, and
personal growth every day!
ebookbell.com