International Refereed Journal of Engineering and Science (IRJES)
ISSN (Online) 2319-183X, (Print) 2319-1821
Volume 6, Issue 6 (June 2017), PP.22-27
www.irjes.com 22 | Page
Experimentation in science, engineering, and education
Jürgen Paul1
, Jorge Groß1
1
(Department of Science Education, University of Bamberg, Germany)
Abstract: Experimentation is used differently in science, engineering, and science education. The aim of many
science fairs is to encourage young talent in scientific inquiry. Based on 57 interviews with participants of a
German youth science fair, this article points out typical students’ conceptions about the procedure and the
purpose of experimentation. The analysis of the interview data revealed that the derived concepts firstly depend
on each other and secondly reflect the differences in the way of thinking and working between scientists and
engineers. Since experiences with experimentation provide the basis for learning and thus for the conceptual
knowledge about science, we conclude that it is essential, for science education, to distinguish the engineer’s
and the scientist’s point of view and to implement more authentic inquiry in science lessons at school.
Keywords: conceptual change, experiments, inquiry-based learning, science education
I. INTRODUCTION
Experimentation is crucial for engineering, natural and computer science, as well as for science
education. The aim of this article is to draw attention to the difference in understanding experimentation within
these fields and to point out the difficulties for science education generated by that difference. Engineering and
science complement and inspire each other. Research in natural science provides the scientific basis for
implementing technical ideas, for inventing new technologies, for thinking and working as an engineer. Specific
technical applications and more precise devices enable scientists to measure, capture, and understand nature
more and more profoundly. In a sense, engineering and science thus form a symbiosis. As a consequence of this
merging, a problem of teaching experimentation as one of the most important scientific inquiry methods occurs
[1]. We therefore assumed that students have problems to distinguish experimentation between engineering and
science in terms of procedure and purpose.
Scientific experimentation is defined as an orderly procedure carried out with the goal of testing a
hypothesis by systematically manipulating the conditions of the observed processes or variables to be measured
[2, 3]. This scientific procedure generally, but not necessarily, contains the following steps: (1) formulating a
research question, (2) generating a theory-based hypothesis, (3) designing the experiment, (4) conducting the
experiment and collecting data, (5) preparing and evaluating the data, (6) interpretation and discussion of the
results and their conclusions, and (7) communication of findings. Of course, we do not claim that this sequence
is the one and only scientific method. However, there is a broad consensus nationally and internationally within
the science education community on the need to convey such a basic understanding of scientific thinking and
working [4, 5].
At school, experiments function in a variety of ways such as motivating students, testing hypotheses, or
illustrating concepts [6]. Here, the teacher generally knows the outcome of the experiment, whereas the result of
a research experiment is unknown. However, the key element in classroom instruction is whether the teacher has
communicated to students what the expected result should be. We therefore distinguish the typical “school
experiment” with explicit instructions and known results from the “research experiment”, which is part of the
scientific process described above.
A scientist asks a question to nature and works to get an answer to this question. Based on a theory, the
scientist’s aim is to test a hypothesis. Whereas for scientists, the purpose of experimentation is knowledge gain
(“open-ended”), engineers usually carry out experiments for technical improvements (“intended”). To fulfil the
requirements of a task, engineers want to achieve a specific effect. Since they have different goals, scientists and
engineers may additionally use different procedures for experimentation. During everyday life, most people
think and act more like an engineer than a scientist, because they daily use things, especially technical devices
as a result of social and technological change, as a means to an end and thus want to achieve a certain effect,
too.
Experimentation is also the core element of many science fairs. In order to retrace scientific inquiry,
students can compete with each other by presenting their own small research projects. In many countries,
science fairs have a long tradition to expand normal classroom teaching [7]. On the one hand, to support and
challenge students individually, and on the other hand, to enhance lessons and also to build the external profile
of the school. Researchers have investigated various aspects of science competitions in the context of science
education. Some of them focused on different factors that lead to successful participation [8]. Gender effects
Experimentation in science, engineering, and education
www.irjes.com 23 | Page
among participants were found in some motivational respects such as self-concept of own competence and
social role attributions [9] or in the choice of science field [10]. Task-oriented science olympiads reach other
types of students than project-centred science fairs [11].
In Germany, the most popular youth science fair with over 10,000 students participating annually is
“Jugend forscht” (which literally translates to “Youth Does Research”). The non-profit organisation behind this
competition sees the education and encouragement of young adults in mathematics, computer science, natural
science, and engineering as a key task to provide a basis for future research and innovation in our society. The
main goal of the competition is to encourage young talent in scientific thinking and working. The competition
follows a periodic annual structure (November 30th: closing date; February: regional competitions;
March/April: state competitions; May: national competition). For their submitted projects, students are free to
work alone or in small groups of 2-3. Scientific experimentation is most commonly at the centre of the projects.
Participants assign their projects to seven different subjects: biology, chemistry, physics, earth sciences,
mathematics or computer science, engineering, and working environment. On the competition days, the students
present their projects to a jury. This presentation in front of the project poster is usually a mixture of a short talk
and a discussion among experts.
In our nationwide study about Jugend forscht, we analysed learning processes about experimentation
and the nature of science (NOS) triggered or supported by a science fair. We used both oral interviews (n = 57)
and software-based questionnaires (n = 1,070; pre-, post-, follow-up-test). Some of these data are already
published [12].
The science fair, Jugend forscht, seems to be a suitable environment for the investigation of student
conceptions about experimentation within the fields of science and engineering, because of the defined
objectives, the focus on experimentation, and the project-centred approach of this competition. To this end, the
present article addresses the two questions: Which conceptions exist about experimentation among the
participating students in terms of procedure and purpose? Do these conceptions reflect differences in the way of
thinking between scientists and engineers? Considering the possible influence of previous experiences with
experiments at school and during everyday life, we assumed that students are used to the engineer’s point of
view and mainly understand experiments to achieve a desired effect. However, the awareness of the difference
between experimentation in science and engineering is seen as an essential basis for an advanced conceptual
knowledge about scientific inquiry and natural science in general.
II. THEORY AND METHODOLOGY
In a moderate constructivist sense, learners need to learn actively [13]. Additionally, we understand
learning by means of a revised conceptual change approach [14, 15], which considers a situated perspective
[16]. We see students as individual learners who construct their knowledge in an active and self-regulated
process on the basis of existing conceptions. These conceptions derived from everyday experiences can be
beneficial or obstructive for learning [17]. Thus, we understand conceptual changes as reconstructions of
conceptions [18], where conceptions can be further developed, changed, or newly formed, depending on the
context and the individual.
To obtain students’ conceptions about experimentation, we used guided interviews as a qualitative
research tool also applying the method of the retrospective query on learning processes (Paul, Lederman, &
Groß, 2016). A total of 57 individual interviews were conducted during regional competition days. For that
purpose, all 872 participating students of five different regional competitions in Bavaria and Thuringia were
contacted beforehand, concerning their willingness to be interviewed. From the 263 positive responses, 57
volunteering students from 10-18 years were randomly chosen for the interviews to produce an approximately
equal distribution of interviewees with respect to location, age, gender, and the topic of their work with an
emphasis on natural sciences (average age: 14,9 ± 2,6 years; 28 males, 29 females; topics: 19 biology, 18
chemistry, 14 physics, 2 engineering, 2 geoscience, 2 mathematics and computer science). All personalized data
were made anonymous.
An interview lasted for about 30 minutes and started in a time frame of 30-60 minutes after the visit of
the competition jury. We used a structured guideline to align the 57 interviews for reproducibility. Two different
researchers conducted the interviews. The interview guideline integrates two methodological approaches: firstly
problem-oriented, open and half-open questions to collect the current conceptions about scientific
experimentation and secondly the retrospective query on the individual learning process. Several basic questions
were drawn from validated questionnaires [19, 20, 21], from which subsequent questions were developed. The
interviews were captured using a voice recorder. The interrelationship between questions and answers was
validated by three different researchers based on qualitative content analysis (in line with [22, 23]). In addition,
an internal triangulation process with similar questions on the same issue was integrated into the guideline. For
reliability, coding and interpretation of students’ statements were analysed by two researchers working
independently. The findings of both were then reconciled if necessary. The organized statements of the students
Experimentation in science, engineering, and education
www.irjes.com 24 | Page
were summarized into tables, where the conceptions mentioned retrospectively were differentiated from the
current conceptions. By comparing these conceptions, we were able to construct the learning process on the
basis of the detected conceptual changes or the additionally accrued concepts. In order to reconstruct the
sequences of the learning processes, the concepts identified were linked with each other step by step according
to the conceptual changes made by the subjects.
III. RESULTS
Students’ statements about experimentation
In order to represent typical students’ experiences and their associated statements about
experimentation, three characteristic examples are quoted. Alicia, a 12-year-old, experiences experiments in
school differently from the ones at Jugend forscht (see Table 1, example 1). During the experiments in school,
she gets detailed instruction how to carry out the experiment or is only a spectator, where the teacher presents
experiments as a demonstration for the entire class. At Jugend forscht, Alicia not only takes matters in her own
hands, she also does so without explicit guidelines from the teaching staff and without written instructions. 54 of
the 57 students questioned usually carry out experiments at school as part of a group, or with a partner following
written instructions.
The 16-year-old Peter tells about his individual learning process (see Table 1, example 2). Formerly,
Peter saw experiments as entertaining, they were rather spectacular (“flash”). His additional experience was to
use experiments for trying various ways in order to construct something with a specific aim. Both conceptions
have in common, that a certain effect should be achieved via experiments. During the course of the science fair,
it became clear to him that the result of a scientific experiment is previously unknown and may be different from
the expectation. Through experiments, you can “prove things” or you can “get new findings” to answer a
question. With this, Peter is an example of the learning process from the conception that experiments serve as
entertainment or to achieve a desired effect, towards the conception that the purpose of scientific
experimentation lies in knowledge production.
Talking to other participants, the 15-year-old Dan figured out that distinct types of experimentation
exist (see Table 1, example 3). He noticed other students who pursued to construct a specific device and tried to
improve its properties. Thus, he differentiates between experiments serving the purpose of a technical
improvement and those, carried out by “scientists”, serving the purpose of knowledge gain. The possibility of
alternative appropriate conceptions of the purpose of experimentation was mentioned by 8 participants
interviewed at the science fair. The three students, Alicia, Peter, and Dan represent typical conceptions of
experimentation found in this study.
Table 1: Three representative student statements from the 57 individual interviews conducted about
experimentation (translated extracts from the original transcripts, key passages are marked as shaded text,
the time display is given in hours:minutes:seconds).
1. Experiments in School and at Jugend forscht
00:10:38
00:10:42
00:11:36
00:11:41
Interviewer: “How do you experience experiments in school?”
Alicia (12 years-old): “Sometimes we do experiments in biology. Last time we observed the germination of
cress seeds. We had a detailed instruction how to do so and worked together in groups of 3 or 4. But in most
cases, when we had experiments, we simply watched the teacher while he was doing the experiment. (...)“
Interviewer: “And how do you see experimentation at Jugend forscht?”
Alicia: “Differently. We did not have a guideline and did everything alone. (…) Without concrete instruction,
but on our own and freely.”
2. Learning processes for the purpose of experimentation
00:25:23
00:25:29
00:27:26
00:27:37
Interviewer: “What did you know about experimentation before you did your Jugend forscht assignment?”
Peter (16 years-old): “Before, I thought experiments are just entertaining, maybe a nice flash for example. (...)
Or you want to construct something and you try different ways, waiting for that what you want. (…)“
Interviewer: “Earlier on, you said that you learned something through the science fair. What did you mean?
What did you learn about science?”
Peter: “When you do real experiments, you don’t know what comes out. Experiments can run differently than
expected. And you have to accept your result, also if it is different. (…) You can prove things by yourself, and
sometimes you get new findings. You ask a question and you can find something. And there is still a lot to
explore.”
3. Difference between engineering and science
00:18:11
00:18:16
Interviewer: “Have you talked to other participants?”
Dan (15 years-old): “Yes, there was another group. They tried to construct a small rocket, just with a plastic
bottle, water, and effervescent tablets. They did a great job. (…) They used experiments for technical
improvements. We had, more or less, a scientific question. (…) As a scientist, you carry out experiments to
expand your knowledge.”
Experimentation in science, engineering, and education
www.irjes.com 25 | Page
Concepts about the procedure and the purpose of experimentation
Participants in the science fair had a comprehensive understanding of experimentation, and thus they
referred to very different features. We know from our previous research that learning processes may occur
within five distinct subdomains of learning [12]. These subdomains are procedure, purpose, material, control,
and time. The related main concepts are the following: experimentation needs a step-by-step procedure;
experiments have a purpose; experiments need materials; an experiment requires a control; experimentation
takes time.
Focusing on procedure and purpose, we found 9 different concepts regarding experimentation (Fig. 1
and 2). The concepts #1-5 are assigned to procedure, the concepts #6-9 pertain to purpose. Several of the
interviewees reported that their conceptions changed during the course of the science fair. Some of these
changes reflect replacements of previous concepts with new concepts. Alternatively, new concepts arose in
addition to the previous concepts or merely modified or complemented the existing concepts. We linked the
concepts identified with each other according to the conceptual changes made by the students and derived
typical pathways and steps of learning (Fig. 1 and 2). A comparison of the steps of learning shown in Fig. 1 with
our understanding of the scientific approach during experimentation (cf. introduction) reveals that cumulative
learning has taken place. The interview data showed that, when participants expressed a higher concept, this
always implied knowledge of the other aspects below in the sense of a sequence during experimentation
(QHERD, Fig. 1). With the final concept (# 5), the students approached the scientifically usual view of the
procedure of experimentation, including formulating a research question (Q).
We furthermore compared the two subdomains of learning with each other and found a strong
correlation between their concepts. If a participant held the concept #8 (“Experiments serve the purpose of a
technical improvement”) or below within the subdomain purpose, he never revealed the concept #5 (“Finding a
question is part of experimentation”). Typically, concept #8 was linked to concept #3 (“Experimentation means
trying out variations”). If a student reached the concept #5 and alluded to the purpose of experiments anyhow,
he always attained the concept #9 (“Experiments serve the purpose of knowledge gain”). Characteristically,
concept #7 (“Experiments are conducive to illustration and comprehension”) accompanied concept #2 (“For
experiments, detailed instruction is needed”). In summary, the two subdomains procedure and purpose
obviously depend on each other.
Figure 1, 2: Concepts and steps of learning about the procedure (Fig. 1, n = 54) and the purpose (Fig.
2, n = 34) of experimentation. Single concepts are numbered consecutively (1-9, at the bottom left within each
box). Learning processes are performed according to arrows by a specific number of participants (numbers
within arrows). Returning arrows imply that concepts did not change. At the bottom right within each box, dark
grey cells show the number of participants who held the respective concept before the science fair (r,
retrospective view). The light grey cells show the number of participants who revealed the respective concept on
the day of the competition (c, current concept, during the interview). White letters in Fig. 1 correspond to the
scientific procedure of experimentation: Q = question; H = hypothesis; E = execution of experiment, collecting
data; R = results, evaluating the data; D = discussion and conclusion.
IV. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
Above, we showed the various concepts about the procedure and the purpose of experimentation found
in this study. The Figures 1 and 2 illustrate typical steps of learning experimentation. Two main reasons for
conceptual developments could be detected [12]: Firstly, within the framework of the science fair, the
participating students have the opportunity to work using methodology similar to the commonly accepted
scientific path of knowledge. Secondly, due to communication processes during the science fair, a purposive
reflection of their own project is promoted.
Experimentation in science, engineering, and education
www.irjes.com 26 | Page
The most vital new finding presented in this article is that the concepts of the procedure and the
purpose of experimentation are strongly linked to each other. Learning experimentation is often described as a
straight line stepped process with different levels of complexity. Our findings reveal that concepts develop
rather according to a cross-linked map. This leads to our second research question: Do the students’ conceptions
about experimentation reflect differences in the way of thinking between scientists and engineers? Yes, they do.
The linked concepts # 3 and 8 perfectly correspond to the “engineer’s model of thinking”, since an engineer
typically wants to produce a desired and interesting phenomenon by testing variations [1]. An engineer heeds
those variables and results that seem to be relevant to achieve the goal, which often is a technical improvement.
Besides, the linked concepts # 5 and 9 cogently reflect the “scientist’s model of thinking”. A scientist usually
intends to understand cause-effect relationships to extend knowledge by designing an experiment suitable to
answer a specific question. Based on a theory, a scientist systematically tests hypotheses under controlled
conditions. Epistemologically, scientific theories cannot be verified but can only be disproved [24]. Of course,
the two ways of thinking are not rated here, rather than analysed in the sense of science education. Anyhow,
students are predominantly used to the engineer’s point of view (16 of 34 students represented the concept #8;
19 of 54 students referred to concept #3), although only 4 of 57 participants had assigned their projects to the
subjects engineering, mathematics or computer science. The projects of the other 53 participants belong to
natural science subjects. In addition, we found that concept #8 never accompanies concept #5. Thinking and
acting as an engineer differs from scientific inquiry. Some students contrasted these different approaches and
were aware of the differences (Table 1, examples 2 and 3). In order to enable a deeper comprehension of
science, a question must be in the foreground. If this concept was developed (concept #5), also the sense of
science was better understood (concept #9).
The influence of experience on learning and development is beyond any doubt [25]. Here, the previous
experiences about experimentation provide the basis for the conceptual knowledge about science. In the media
and at school, experiments often serve the purpose of entertainment (concept #6). The TV host or teacher
demonstrates an experiment as a spectacular event. At school, experiments are mostly used for illustration and
comprehension (#7). For this, a detailed instruction is needed, at least if the students carry out the experiment.
During everyday life (cf. introduction) and even at a science fair, which is supposed to encourage students in
scientific thinking and working, experimentation is experienced as it serves to achieve a specific effect or goal,
e.g. a technical improvement (#8). The experience of authentic inquiry, including a research question and a
hypothesis, conveys the insight that scientific experimentation serves the purpose of knowledge gain (#9). The
existence or the manner of the question particularly makes the difference between the engineer’s and the
scientist’s point of view. For science education, there are consequently two important steps, which should be
taken before an experiment is carried out: Firstly, a problem should be recognized and limited in order to
formulate a question. Secondly, after devising a hypothesis, the experiment should be planned with respect to
the question and to theory [24].
In conclusion, it is essential for science education, both for teachers and for students, to distinguish the
two different ways of thinking and working during experimentation, on the one hand as an engineer, and on the
other hand as a scientist. Together with a stronger anchoring of authentic inquiry in science lessons at school,
this will smooth the way for a fundamental understanding of science.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank the foundation Jugend forscht e.V. for cooperation during our research project, particularly Dr. Sven
Baszio (managing director), Sigrid Müller-Balhorn (head of competition management), and Stefan Gagel (head
of one regional competition).
REFERENCES
[1] . Hammann, and . ayer, Was lernen ch lerinnen und ch ler beim Experimentieren? Biologie in
unserer Zeit (BiuZ), 42, 2012, 284-285.
[2] R.A. Duschl, R.E. Grandy, Teaching scientific inquiry: Recommendations for research and
implementation (Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers, 2008).
[3] W.T. Griffith, and J.W. Brosing, The physics of everyday phenomena (McGraw-Hill
Science/Engineering/Math, 7th edition, 2011).
[4] W.F. McComas, and J.K. Olson, The nature of science in international science education standards
documents, in W.F. McComas (Ed.), The Nature of Science in Science Education (New York: Springer,
2002) 41-52.
[5] National Research Council, A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts,
and core ideas. (Washington, DC: The National Academy Press, 2012).
Experimentation in science, engineering, and education
www.irjes.com 27 | Page
[6] C. Hart, P. Mulhall, A. Berry, J. Loughran, and R. Gunstone, What is the purpose of this experiment? Or
can students learn something from doing experiments? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(7),
2000, 655-675.
[7] W.F. McComas, SCienCe Fair, The Science Teacher, 78(8), 2011, 34-38.
[8] D. Urhahne, L.H. Ho, I. Parchmann, and S. Nick, Attempting to predict success in the qualifying round of
the International Chemistry Olympiad, High Ability Studies, 23(2), 2012, 167-182.
[9] A.X. Feng, J.R. Campbell, and M.A. Verna, Understanding gender inequity in America: Interviews with
academic Olympians, Journal of Research in Education, 12, 2002, 93-100.
[10] G. Sonnert, P. Sadler, and M. Michaels, Gender aspects of participation, support, and success in a state
science fair, School Science and Mathematics, 113(3), 2013, 135-143.
[11] T.V. Abernathy, and R.N. Vineyard, Academic competitions in science: What are the rewards for
students? The Clearing House, 74(5), 2001, 269-276.
[12] J. Paul, N.G. Lederman, and J. Groß, Learning experimentation through science fairs, International
Journal of Science Education, IJSE, 38(15), 2016, 2367-2387.
[13] C.T. Fosnot, Constructivism: Theory, perspectives, and practice (Teachers College Press, 2013).
[14] M.T. Chi, Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and
categorical shift, in S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change
(Taylor & Francis, 2008) 61-82.
[15] K.A. Strike, and G.J. Posner, A revisionist theory of conceptual change, in R.A. Duschl, and R.J.
Hamilton (Eds.), Philosophy of science, cognitive psychology, and educational theory and practice (State
University of New York Press, 1992) 147-176.
[16] J.D. Novak, Meaningful learning: The essential factor for conceptual change in limited or inappropriate
propositional hierarchies leading to empowerment of learners, Science Education, 86(4), 2002, 548-571.
[17] R. Duit, and D.F. Treagust, Learning in science: From behaviourism towards social constructivism and
beyond, in B.J. Fraser, K.G. Tobin (Eds.), International handbook of science education (London: Kluwer
Academic Publishers, 1998) 3-25.
[18] U. Kattmann, Learning biology by means of anthropomorphic conceptions? in M. Hammann, M. Reiss,
C. Boulter, and S.D. Tunnicliffe (Eds.), Biology in context: learning and teaching for the twenty-first
century (London: Institute of Education, 2008) 7-17.
[19] E. Gaigher, N.G. Lederman, and J. Lederman, Knowledge about Inquiry: A study in South African high
schools, International Journal of Science Education, 36(18), 2014, 3125-3147.
[20] N.G. Lederman, F. Abd-El-Khalick, R.L. Bell, and R.S. Schwartz, Views of nature of science
questionnaire: Toward valid and meaningful assessment of learners' conceptions of nature of science,
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(6), 2002, 497-521.
[21] R.S. Schwartz, N.G. Lederman, and B.A. Crawford, Developing views of nature of science in an
authentic context: An explicit approach to bridging the gap between nature of science and scientific
inquiry, Science Education, 88(4), 2004, 610-645.
[22] P. Mayring, Qualitative Inhaltsanalyse, Grundlagen und Techniken (Weinheim: Beltz, 2010).
[23] I. Seidman, Interviewing as qualitative research: A guide for researchers in education and the social
sciences (Teachers College Press, 2012).
[24] D. Klahr, Exploring Science: The Cognition and Development of Discovery Processes (Cambridge: MIT
Press, 2002).
[25] D.A. Kolb, Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development (FT press,
2014).

More Related Content

PPTX
Introduction of research
PDF
Case Study
PPTX
aiou code 837
PPTX
How do Scholars Evaluate and Promote Research Outputs? An NTU Case Study
PPTX
nursing research Chapter 5 for pbn 3rd year pu PU nepal
PPTX
Research methodology part 1
PDF
An abstraction of research findings for policy formulation and decision makin...
Introduction of research
Case Study
aiou code 837
How do Scholars Evaluate and Promote Research Outputs? An NTU Case Study
nursing research Chapter 5 for pbn 3rd year pu PU nepal
Research methodology part 1
An abstraction of research findings for policy formulation and decision makin...

What's hot (17)

DOCX
Rmic 725 master syllabus
PPTX
Research methodology
PPT
Business Research, MBA Nguyễn Đình Như Hà
PDF
Guidelines to Qualitative Researches
PDF
EMERGING TRENDS ON THE TOPIC OF INFORMATION TECHNOLOGY IN THE FIELD ...
PPTX
EBP & Health Sciences Librarianship
DOCX
Chapter 1 done editing
PDF
Introduction to research
PPTX
Research problem
PDF
INTRODUCTION TO HEALTHCARE RESEARCH METHODS
PDF
Thesis Writing
PPT
Bornali poster final
PDF
Unit 1-introduction to research methods -dr m r n babu
PDF
Nursing research introduction
PDF
Relevance of experimental design
PDF
Research trough design and transdisciplinarity
PDF
Nursing Research Designs
Rmic 725 master syllabus
Research methodology
Business Research, MBA Nguyễn Đình Như Hà
Guidelines to Qualitative Researches
EMERGING TRENDS ON THE TOPIC OF INFORMATION TECHNOLOGY IN THE FIELD ...
EBP & Health Sciences Librarianship
Chapter 1 done editing
Introduction to research
Research problem
INTRODUCTION TO HEALTHCARE RESEARCH METHODS
Thesis Writing
Bornali poster final
Unit 1-introduction to research methods -dr m r n babu
Nursing research introduction
Relevance of experimental design
Research trough design and transdisciplinarity
Nursing Research Designs
Ad

Similar to Experimentation in science, engineering, and education (20)

DOCX
Research-giatay.docx
PDF
Science Fair Warmup Teachers Guide Learning The Practice Of Scientists 1st Ed...
PDF
Students Developing More Complex Understanding in Science and Mathematics
PDF
Making authentic science accesible to students
PDF
Research trends in science education
PDF
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
DOC
Current Assessment Practices 2008-09.pdf
PDF
[7 16]ghanaian primary school pupils’ conceptual framework of energy
PDF
Attitude of science teachers towards teaching Integrated Science in Junior Hi...
PDF
Monitoring The Status Of Students' Journey Towards Science And Mathematics Li...
PDF
3rd grade of elementary student perceptions of science
PDF
A Summary Of Research In Science Education 1987. Part 1
DOC
2016 scig7 q1
DOCX
Introduction of Science lab
PDF
Radians School News Letter, Issue 14 science fair edition!!
PPT
Presentation sc techno-anglais-1
PDF
Guyana -Understanding Science to Improve Teaching & Learning
PDF
Yager, stuart o the role of student and teacher creativity nfaerj v25 n3 2012
PDF
Ijsrp p90115
PDF
Executive Summary Project Review of Mobile Science Laboratories June 2012
Research-giatay.docx
Science Fair Warmup Teachers Guide Learning The Practice Of Scientists 1st Ed...
Students Developing More Complex Understanding in Science and Mathematics
Making authentic science accesible to students
Research trends in science education
West, jeff science literacy is classroom instruction enough nftej v20 n 3 2010
Current Assessment Practices 2008-09.pdf
[7 16]ghanaian primary school pupils’ conceptual framework of energy
Attitude of science teachers towards teaching Integrated Science in Junior Hi...
Monitoring The Status Of Students' Journey Towards Science And Mathematics Li...
3rd grade of elementary student perceptions of science
A Summary Of Research In Science Education 1987. Part 1
2016 scig7 q1
Introduction of Science lab
Radians School News Letter, Issue 14 science fair edition!!
Presentation sc techno-anglais-1
Guyana -Understanding Science to Improve Teaching & Learning
Yager, stuart o the role of student and teacher creativity nfaerj v25 n3 2012
Ijsrp p90115
Executive Summary Project Review of Mobile Science Laboratories June 2012
Ad

More from IRJESJOURNAL (20)

PDF
Drying of agricultural products using forced convection indirect solar dryer
PDF
The Problems of Constructing Optimal Onboard Colored RGB Depicting UAV Systems
PDF
Flexible Design Processes to Reduce the Early Obsolescence of Buildings
PDF
Study on Performance Enhancement of Solar Ejector Cooling System
PDF
Flight Safety Case Study: Adi Sucipto Airport Jogjakarta - Indonesia
PDF
A Review of Severe Plastic Deformation
PDF
Annealing Response of Aluminum Alloy AA6014 Processed By Severe Plastic Defor...
PDF
Evaluation of Thresholding Based Noncontact Respiration Rate Monitoring using...
PDF
Correlation of True Boiling Point of Crude Oil
PDF
Combined Geophysical And Geotechnical Techniques For Assessment Of Foundation...
PDF
ICT governance in Higher Education
PDF
Gobernanzade las TIC en la Educacion Superior
PDF
The Analysis and Perspective on Development of Chinese Automotive Heavy-duty ...
PDF
Research on The Bottom Software of Electronic Control System In Automobile El...
PDF
Evaluation of Specialized Virtual Health Libraries in Scholar Education Evalu...
PDF
Linking Ab Initio-Calphad for the Assessment of the AluminiumLutetium System
PDF
Thermodynamic Assessment (Suggestions) Of the Gold-Rubidium System
PDF
Elisa Test for Determination of Grapevine Viral Infection in Rahovec, Kosovo
PDF
Modelling of Sealing Elements Wearing
PDF
Determining Loss of Liquid from Different Types of Mud by Various Addictives ...
Drying of agricultural products using forced convection indirect solar dryer
The Problems of Constructing Optimal Onboard Colored RGB Depicting UAV Systems
Flexible Design Processes to Reduce the Early Obsolescence of Buildings
Study on Performance Enhancement of Solar Ejector Cooling System
Flight Safety Case Study: Adi Sucipto Airport Jogjakarta - Indonesia
A Review of Severe Plastic Deformation
Annealing Response of Aluminum Alloy AA6014 Processed By Severe Plastic Defor...
Evaluation of Thresholding Based Noncontact Respiration Rate Monitoring using...
Correlation of True Boiling Point of Crude Oil
Combined Geophysical And Geotechnical Techniques For Assessment Of Foundation...
ICT governance in Higher Education
Gobernanzade las TIC en la Educacion Superior
The Analysis and Perspective on Development of Chinese Automotive Heavy-duty ...
Research on The Bottom Software of Electronic Control System In Automobile El...
Evaluation of Specialized Virtual Health Libraries in Scholar Education Evalu...
Linking Ab Initio-Calphad for the Assessment of the AluminiumLutetium System
Thermodynamic Assessment (Suggestions) Of the Gold-Rubidium System
Elisa Test for Determination of Grapevine Viral Infection in Rahovec, Kosovo
Modelling of Sealing Elements Wearing
Determining Loss of Liquid from Different Types of Mud by Various Addictives ...

Recently uploaded (20)

PPT
INTRODUCTION -Data Warehousing and Mining-M.Tech- VTU.ppt
PDF
Influence of Green Infrastructure on Residents’ Endorsement of the New Ecolog...
PDF
Exploratory_Data_Analysis_Fundamentals.pdf
PDF
null (2) bgfbg bfgb bfgb fbfg bfbgf b.pdf
PDF
Abrasive, erosive and cavitation wear.pdf
PDF
August 2025 - Top 10 Read Articles in Network Security & Its Applications
PPTX
Current and future trends in Computer Vision.pptx
PDF
Improvement effect of pyrolyzed agro-food biochar on the properties of.pdf
PDF
Design Guidelines and solutions for Plastics parts
PPTX
Fundamentals of safety and accident prevention -final (1).pptx
PDF
Artificial Superintelligence (ASI) Alliance Vision Paper.pdf
PPTX
CyberSecurity Mobile and Wireless Devices
PDF
August -2025_Top10 Read_Articles_ijait.pdf
PDF
BIO-INSPIRED HORMONAL MODULATION AND ADAPTIVE ORCHESTRATION IN S-AI-GPT
PPTX
ASME PCC-02 TRAINING -DESKTOP-NLE5HNP.pptx
PPTX
Module 8- Technological and Communication Skills.pptx
PDF
Visual Aids for Exploratory Data Analysis.pdf
PDF
Level 2 – IBM Data and AI Fundamentals (1)_v1.1.PDF
PPTX
communication and presentation skills 01
PDF
Accra-Kumasi Expressway - Prefeasibility Report Volume 1 of 7.11.2018.pdf
INTRODUCTION -Data Warehousing and Mining-M.Tech- VTU.ppt
Influence of Green Infrastructure on Residents’ Endorsement of the New Ecolog...
Exploratory_Data_Analysis_Fundamentals.pdf
null (2) bgfbg bfgb bfgb fbfg bfbgf b.pdf
Abrasive, erosive and cavitation wear.pdf
August 2025 - Top 10 Read Articles in Network Security & Its Applications
Current and future trends in Computer Vision.pptx
Improvement effect of pyrolyzed agro-food biochar on the properties of.pdf
Design Guidelines and solutions for Plastics parts
Fundamentals of safety and accident prevention -final (1).pptx
Artificial Superintelligence (ASI) Alliance Vision Paper.pdf
CyberSecurity Mobile and Wireless Devices
August -2025_Top10 Read_Articles_ijait.pdf
BIO-INSPIRED HORMONAL MODULATION AND ADAPTIVE ORCHESTRATION IN S-AI-GPT
ASME PCC-02 TRAINING -DESKTOP-NLE5HNP.pptx
Module 8- Technological and Communication Skills.pptx
Visual Aids for Exploratory Data Analysis.pdf
Level 2 – IBM Data and AI Fundamentals (1)_v1.1.PDF
communication and presentation skills 01
Accra-Kumasi Expressway - Prefeasibility Report Volume 1 of 7.11.2018.pdf

Experimentation in science, engineering, and education

  • 1. International Refereed Journal of Engineering and Science (IRJES) ISSN (Online) 2319-183X, (Print) 2319-1821 Volume 6, Issue 6 (June 2017), PP.22-27 www.irjes.com 22 | Page Experimentation in science, engineering, and education Jürgen Paul1 , Jorge Groß1 1 (Department of Science Education, University of Bamberg, Germany) Abstract: Experimentation is used differently in science, engineering, and science education. The aim of many science fairs is to encourage young talent in scientific inquiry. Based on 57 interviews with participants of a German youth science fair, this article points out typical students’ conceptions about the procedure and the purpose of experimentation. The analysis of the interview data revealed that the derived concepts firstly depend on each other and secondly reflect the differences in the way of thinking and working between scientists and engineers. Since experiences with experimentation provide the basis for learning and thus for the conceptual knowledge about science, we conclude that it is essential, for science education, to distinguish the engineer’s and the scientist’s point of view and to implement more authentic inquiry in science lessons at school. Keywords: conceptual change, experiments, inquiry-based learning, science education I. INTRODUCTION Experimentation is crucial for engineering, natural and computer science, as well as for science education. The aim of this article is to draw attention to the difference in understanding experimentation within these fields and to point out the difficulties for science education generated by that difference. Engineering and science complement and inspire each other. Research in natural science provides the scientific basis for implementing technical ideas, for inventing new technologies, for thinking and working as an engineer. Specific technical applications and more precise devices enable scientists to measure, capture, and understand nature more and more profoundly. In a sense, engineering and science thus form a symbiosis. As a consequence of this merging, a problem of teaching experimentation as one of the most important scientific inquiry methods occurs [1]. We therefore assumed that students have problems to distinguish experimentation between engineering and science in terms of procedure and purpose. Scientific experimentation is defined as an orderly procedure carried out with the goal of testing a hypothesis by systematically manipulating the conditions of the observed processes or variables to be measured [2, 3]. This scientific procedure generally, but not necessarily, contains the following steps: (1) formulating a research question, (2) generating a theory-based hypothesis, (3) designing the experiment, (4) conducting the experiment and collecting data, (5) preparing and evaluating the data, (6) interpretation and discussion of the results and their conclusions, and (7) communication of findings. Of course, we do not claim that this sequence is the one and only scientific method. However, there is a broad consensus nationally and internationally within the science education community on the need to convey such a basic understanding of scientific thinking and working [4, 5]. At school, experiments function in a variety of ways such as motivating students, testing hypotheses, or illustrating concepts [6]. Here, the teacher generally knows the outcome of the experiment, whereas the result of a research experiment is unknown. However, the key element in classroom instruction is whether the teacher has communicated to students what the expected result should be. We therefore distinguish the typical “school experiment” with explicit instructions and known results from the “research experiment”, which is part of the scientific process described above. A scientist asks a question to nature and works to get an answer to this question. Based on a theory, the scientist’s aim is to test a hypothesis. Whereas for scientists, the purpose of experimentation is knowledge gain (“open-ended”), engineers usually carry out experiments for technical improvements (“intended”). To fulfil the requirements of a task, engineers want to achieve a specific effect. Since they have different goals, scientists and engineers may additionally use different procedures for experimentation. During everyday life, most people think and act more like an engineer than a scientist, because they daily use things, especially technical devices as a result of social and technological change, as a means to an end and thus want to achieve a certain effect, too. Experimentation is also the core element of many science fairs. In order to retrace scientific inquiry, students can compete with each other by presenting their own small research projects. In many countries, science fairs have a long tradition to expand normal classroom teaching [7]. On the one hand, to support and challenge students individually, and on the other hand, to enhance lessons and also to build the external profile of the school. Researchers have investigated various aspects of science competitions in the context of science education. Some of them focused on different factors that lead to successful participation [8]. Gender effects
  • 2. Experimentation in science, engineering, and education www.irjes.com 23 | Page among participants were found in some motivational respects such as self-concept of own competence and social role attributions [9] or in the choice of science field [10]. Task-oriented science olympiads reach other types of students than project-centred science fairs [11]. In Germany, the most popular youth science fair with over 10,000 students participating annually is “Jugend forscht” (which literally translates to “Youth Does Research”). The non-profit organisation behind this competition sees the education and encouragement of young adults in mathematics, computer science, natural science, and engineering as a key task to provide a basis for future research and innovation in our society. The main goal of the competition is to encourage young talent in scientific thinking and working. The competition follows a periodic annual structure (November 30th: closing date; February: regional competitions; March/April: state competitions; May: national competition). For their submitted projects, students are free to work alone or in small groups of 2-3. Scientific experimentation is most commonly at the centre of the projects. Participants assign their projects to seven different subjects: biology, chemistry, physics, earth sciences, mathematics or computer science, engineering, and working environment. On the competition days, the students present their projects to a jury. This presentation in front of the project poster is usually a mixture of a short talk and a discussion among experts. In our nationwide study about Jugend forscht, we analysed learning processes about experimentation and the nature of science (NOS) triggered or supported by a science fair. We used both oral interviews (n = 57) and software-based questionnaires (n = 1,070; pre-, post-, follow-up-test). Some of these data are already published [12]. The science fair, Jugend forscht, seems to be a suitable environment for the investigation of student conceptions about experimentation within the fields of science and engineering, because of the defined objectives, the focus on experimentation, and the project-centred approach of this competition. To this end, the present article addresses the two questions: Which conceptions exist about experimentation among the participating students in terms of procedure and purpose? Do these conceptions reflect differences in the way of thinking between scientists and engineers? Considering the possible influence of previous experiences with experiments at school and during everyday life, we assumed that students are used to the engineer’s point of view and mainly understand experiments to achieve a desired effect. However, the awareness of the difference between experimentation in science and engineering is seen as an essential basis for an advanced conceptual knowledge about scientific inquiry and natural science in general. II. THEORY AND METHODOLOGY In a moderate constructivist sense, learners need to learn actively [13]. Additionally, we understand learning by means of a revised conceptual change approach [14, 15], which considers a situated perspective [16]. We see students as individual learners who construct their knowledge in an active and self-regulated process on the basis of existing conceptions. These conceptions derived from everyday experiences can be beneficial or obstructive for learning [17]. Thus, we understand conceptual changes as reconstructions of conceptions [18], where conceptions can be further developed, changed, or newly formed, depending on the context and the individual. To obtain students’ conceptions about experimentation, we used guided interviews as a qualitative research tool also applying the method of the retrospective query on learning processes (Paul, Lederman, & Groß, 2016). A total of 57 individual interviews were conducted during regional competition days. For that purpose, all 872 participating students of five different regional competitions in Bavaria and Thuringia were contacted beforehand, concerning their willingness to be interviewed. From the 263 positive responses, 57 volunteering students from 10-18 years were randomly chosen for the interviews to produce an approximately equal distribution of interviewees with respect to location, age, gender, and the topic of their work with an emphasis on natural sciences (average age: 14,9 ± 2,6 years; 28 males, 29 females; topics: 19 biology, 18 chemistry, 14 physics, 2 engineering, 2 geoscience, 2 mathematics and computer science). All personalized data were made anonymous. An interview lasted for about 30 minutes and started in a time frame of 30-60 minutes after the visit of the competition jury. We used a structured guideline to align the 57 interviews for reproducibility. Two different researchers conducted the interviews. The interview guideline integrates two methodological approaches: firstly problem-oriented, open and half-open questions to collect the current conceptions about scientific experimentation and secondly the retrospective query on the individual learning process. Several basic questions were drawn from validated questionnaires [19, 20, 21], from which subsequent questions were developed. The interviews were captured using a voice recorder. The interrelationship between questions and answers was validated by three different researchers based on qualitative content analysis (in line with [22, 23]). In addition, an internal triangulation process with similar questions on the same issue was integrated into the guideline. For reliability, coding and interpretation of students’ statements were analysed by two researchers working independently. The findings of both were then reconciled if necessary. The organized statements of the students
  • 3. Experimentation in science, engineering, and education www.irjes.com 24 | Page were summarized into tables, where the conceptions mentioned retrospectively were differentiated from the current conceptions. By comparing these conceptions, we were able to construct the learning process on the basis of the detected conceptual changes or the additionally accrued concepts. In order to reconstruct the sequences of the learning processes, the concepts identified were linked with each other step by step according to the conceptual changes made by the subjects. III. RESULTS Students’ statements about experimentation In order to represent typical students’ experiences and their associated statements about experimentation, three characteristic examples are quoted. Alicia, a 12-year-old, experiences experiments in school differently from the ones at Jugend forscht (see Table 1, example 1). During the experiments in school, she gets detailed instruction how to carry out the experiment or is only a spectator, where the teacher presents experiments as a demonstration for the entire class. At Jugend forscht, Alicia not only takes matters in her own hands, she also does so without explicit guidelines from the teaching staff and without written instructions. 54 of the 57 students questioned usually carry out experiments at school as part of a group, or with a partner following written instructions. The 16-year-old Peter tells about his individual learning process (see Table 1, example 2). Formerly, Peter saw experiments as entertaining, they were rather spectacular (“flash”). His additional experience was to use experiments for trying various ways in order to construct something with a specific aim. Both conceptions have in common, that a certain effect should be achieved via experiments. During the course of the science fair, it became clear to him that the result of a scientific experiment is previously unknown and may be different from the expectation. Through experiments, you can “prove things” or you can “get new findings” to answer a question. With this, Peter is an example of the learning process from the conception that experiments serve as entertainment or to achieve a desired effect, towards the conception that the purpose of scientific experimentation lies in knowledge production. Talking to other participants, the 15-year-old Dan figured out that distinct types of experimentation exist (see Table 1, example 3). He noticed other students who pursued to construct a specific device and tried to improve its properties. Thus, he differentiates between experiments serving the purpose of a technical improvement and those, carried out by “scientists”, serving the purpose of knowledge gain. The possibility of alternative appropriate conceptions of the purpose of experimentation was mentioned by 8 participants interviewed at the science fair. The three students, Alicia, Peter, and Dan represent typical conceptions of experimentation found in this study. Table 1: Three representative student statements from the 57 individual interviews conducted about experimentation (translated extracts from the original transcripts, key passages are marked as shaded text, the time display is given in hours:minutes:seconds). 1. Experiments in School and at Jugend forscht 00:10:38 00:10:42 00:11:36 00:11:41 Interviewer: “How do you experience experiments in school?” Alicia (12 years-old): “Sometimes we do experiments in biology. Last time we observed the germination of cress seeds. We had a detailed instruction how to do so and worked together in groups of 3 or 4. But in most cases, when we had experiments, we simply watched the teacher while he was doing the experiment. (...)“ Interviewer: “And how do you see experimentation at Jugend forscht?” Alicia: “Differently. We did not have a guideline and did everything alone. (…) Without concrete instruction, but on our own and freely.” 2. Learning processes for the purpose of experimentation 00:25:23 00:25:29 00:27:26 00:27:37 Interviewer: “What did you know about experimentation before you did your Jugend forscht assignment?” Peter (16 years-old): “Before, I thought experiments are just entertaining, maybe a nice flash for example. (...) Or you want to construct something and you try different ways, waiting for that what you want. (…)“ Interviewer: “Earlier on, you said that you learned something through the science fair. What did you mean? What did you learn about science?” Peter: “When you do real experiments, you don’t know what comes out. Experiments can run differently than expected. And you have to accept your result, also if it is different. (…) You can prove things by yourself, and sometimes you get new findings. You ask a question and you can find something. And there is still a lot to explore.” 3. Difference between engineering and science 00:18:11 00:18:16 Interviewer: “Have you talked to other participants?” Dan (15 years-old): “Yes, there was another group. They tried to construct a small rocket, just with a plastic bottle, water, and effervescent tablets. They did a great job. (…) They used experiments for technical improvements. We had, more or less, a scientific question. (…) As a scientist, you carry out experiments to expand your knowledge.”
  • 4. Experimentation in science, engineering, and education www.irjes.com 25 | Page Concepts about the procedure and the purpose of experimentation Participants in the science fair had a comprehensive understanding of experimentation, and thus they referred to very different features. We know from our previous research that learning processes may occur within five distinct subdomains of learning [12]. These subdomains are procedure, purpose, material, control, and time. The related main concepts are the following: experimentation needs a step-by-step procedure; experiments have a purpose; experiments need materials; an experiment requires a control; experimentation takes time. Focusing on procedure and purpose, we found 9 different concepts regarding experimentation (Fig. 1 and 2). The concepts #1-5 are assigned to procedure, the concepts #6-9 pertain to purpose. Several of the interviewees reported that their conceptions changed during the course of the science fair. Some of these changes reflect replacements of previous concepts with new concepts. Alternatively, new concepts arose in addition to the previous concepts or merely modified or complemented the existing concepts. We linked the concepts identified with each other according to the conceptual changes made by the students and derived typical pathways and steps of learning (Fig. 1 and 2). A comparison of the steps of learning shown in Fig. 1 with our understanding of the scientific approach during experimentation (cf. introduction) reveals that cumulative learning has taken place. The interview data showed that, when participants expressed a higher concept, this always implied knowledge of the other aspects below in the sense of a sequence during experimentation (QHERD, Fig. 1). With the final concept (# 5), the students approached the scientifically usual view of the procedure of experimentation, including formulating a research question (Q). We furthermore compared the two subdomains of learning with each other and found a strong correlation between their concepts. If a participant held the concept #8 (“Experiments serve the purpose of a technical improvement”) or below within the subdomain purpose, he never revealed the concept #5 (“Finding a question is part of experimentation”). Typically, concept #8 was linked to concept #3 (“Experimentation means trying out variations”). If a student reached the concept #5 and alluded to the purpose of experiments anyhow, he always attained the concept #9 (“Experiments serve the purpose of knowledge gain”). Characteristically, concept #7 (“Experiments are conducive to illustration and comprehension”) accompanied concept #2 (“For experiments, detailed instruction is needed”). In summary, the two subdomains procedure and purpose obviously depend on each other. Figure 1, 2: Concepts and steps of learning about the procedure (Fig. 1, n = 54) and the purpose (Fig. 2, n = 34) of experimentation. Single concepts are numbered consecutively (1-9, at the bottom left within each box). Learning processes are performed according to arrows by a specific number of participants (numbers within arrows). Returning arrows imply that concepts did not change. At the bottom right within each box, dark grey cells show the number of participants who held the respective concept before the science fair (r, retrospective view). The light grey cells show the number of participants who revealed the respective concept on the day of the competition (c, current concept, during the interview). White letters in Fig. 1 correspond to the scientific procedure of experimentation: Q = question; H = hypothesis; E = execution of experiment, collecting data; R = results, evaluating the data; D = discussion and conclusion. IV. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION Above, we showed the various concepts about the procedure and the purpose of experimentation found in this study. The Figures 1 and 2 illustrate typical steps of learning experimentation. Two main reasons for conceptual developments could be detected [12]: Firstly, within the framework of the science fair, the participating students have the opportunity to work using methodology similar to the commonly accepted scientific path of knowledge. Secondly, due to communication processes during the science fair, a purposive reflection of their own project is promoted.
  • 5. Experimentation in science, engineering, and education www.irjes.com 26 | Page The most vital new finding presented in this article is that the concepts of the procedure and the purpose of experimentation are strongly linked to each other. Learning experimentation is often described as a straight line stepped process with different levels of complexity. Our findings reveal that concepts develop rather according to a cross-linked map. This leads to our second research question: Do the students’ conceptions about experimentation reflect differences in the way of thinking between scientists and engineers? Yes, they do. The linked concepts # 3 and 8 perfectly correspond to the “engineer’s model of thinking”, since an engineer typically wants to produce a desired and interesting phenomenon by testing variations [1]. An engineer heeds those variables and results that seem to be relevant to achieve the goal, which often is a technical improvement. Besides, the linked concepts # 5 and 9 cogently reflect the “scientist’s model of thinking”. A scientist usually intends to understand cause-effect relationships to extend knowledge by designing an experiment suitable to answer a specific question. Based on a theory, a scientist systematically tests hypotheses under controlled conditions. Epistemologically, scientific theories cannot be verified but can only be disproved [24]. Of course, the two ways of thinking are not rated here, rather than analysed in the sense of science education. Anyhow, students are predominantly used to the engineer’s point of view (16 of 34 students represented the concept #8; 19 of 54 students referred to concept #3), although only 4 of 57 participants had assigned their projects to the subjects engineering, mathematics or computer science. The projects of the other 53 participants belong to natural science subjects. In addition, we found that concept #8 never accompanies concept #5. Thinking and acting as an engineer differs from scientific inquiry. Some students contrasted these different approaches and were aware of the differences (Table 1, examples 2 and 3). In order to enable a deeper comprehension of science, a question must be in the foreground. If this concept was developed (concept #5), also the sense of science was better understood (concept #9). The influence of experience on learning and development is beyond any doubt [25]. Here, the previous experiences about experimentation provide the basis for the conceptual knowledge about science. In the media and at school, experiments often serve the purpose of entertainment (concept #6). The TV host or teacher demonstrates an experiment as a spectacular event. At school, experiments are mostly used for illustration and comprehension (#7). For this, a detailed instruction is needed, at least if the students carry out the experiment. During everyday life (cf. introduction) and even at a science fair, which is supposed to encourage students in scientific thinking and working, experimentation is experienced as it serves to achieve a specific effect or goal, e.g. a technical improvement (#8). The experience of authentic inquiry, including a research question and a hypothesis, conveys the insight that scientific experimentation serves the purpose of knowledge gain (#9). The existence or the manner of the question particularly makes the difference between the engineer’s and the scientist’s point of view. For science education, there are consequently two important steps, which should be taken before an experiment is carried out: Firstly, a problem should be recognized and limited in order to formulate a question. Secondly, after devising a hypothesis, the experiment should be planned with respect to the question and to theory [24]. In conclusion, it is essential for science education, both for teachers and for students, to distinguish the two different ways of thinking and working during experimentation, on the one hand as an engineer, and on the other hand as a scientist. Together with a stronger anchoring of authentic inquiry in science lessons at school, this will smooth the way for a fundamental understanding of science. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank the foundation Jugend forscht e.V. for cooperation during our research project, particularly Dr. Sven Baszio (managing director), Sigrid Müller-Balhorn (head of competition management), and Stefan Gagel (head of one regional competition). REFERENCES [1] . Hammann, and . ayer, Was lernen ch lerinnen und ch ler beim Experimentieren? Biologie in unserer Zeit (BiuZ), 42, 2012, 284-285. [2] R.A. Duschl, R.E. Grandy, Teaching scientific inquiry: Recommendations for research and implementation (Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers, 2008). [3] W.T. Griffith, and J.W. Brosing, The physics of everyday phenomena (McGraw-Hill Science/Engineering/Math, 7th edition, 2011). [4] W.F. McComas, and J.K. Olson, The nature of science in international science education standards documents, in W.F. McComas (Ed.), The Nature of Science in Science Education (New York: Springer, 2002) 41-52. [5] National Research Council, A framework for K-12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. (Washington, DC: The National Academy Press, 2012).
  • 6. Experimentation in science, engineering, and education www.irjes.com 27 | Page [6] C. Hart, P. Mulhall, A. Berry, J. Loughran, and R. Gunstone, What is the purpose of this experiment? Or can students learn something from doing experiments? Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(7), 2000, 655-675. [7] W.F. McComas, SCienCe Fair, The Science Teacher, 78(8), 2011, 34-38. [8] D. Urhahne, L.H. Ho, I. Parchmann, and S. Nick, Attempting to predict success in the qualifying round of the International Chemistry Olympiad, High Ability Studies, 23(2), 2012, 167-182. [9] A.X. Feng, J.R. Campbell, and M.A. Verna, Understanding gender inequity in America: Interviews with academic Olympians, Journal of Research in Education, 12, 2002, 93-100. [10] G. Sonnert, P. Sadler, and M. Michaels, Gender aspects of participation, support, and success in a state science fair, School Science and Mathematics, 113(3), 2013, 135-143. [11] T.V. Abernathy, and R.N. Vineyard, Academic competitions in science: What are the rewards for students? The Clearing House, 74(5), 2001, 269-276. [12] J. Paul, N.G. Lederman, and J. Groß, Learning experimentation through science fairs, International Journal of Science Education, IJSE, 38(15), 2016, 2367-2387. [13] C.T. Fosnot, Constructivism: Theory, perspectives, and practice (Teachers College Press, 2013). [14] M.T. Chi, Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation, and categorical shift, in S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change (Taylor & Francis, 2008) 61-82. [15] K.A. Strike, and G.J. Posner, A revisionist theory of conceptual change, in R.A. Duschl, and R.J. Hamilton (Eds.), Philosophy of science, cognitive psychology, and educational theory and practice (State University of New York Press, 1992) 147-176. [16] J.D. Novak, Meaningful learning: The essential factor for conceptual change in limited or inappropriate propositional hierarchies leading to empowerment of learners, Science Education, 86(4), 2002, 548-571. [17] R. Duit, and D.F. Treagust, Learning in science: From behaviourism towards social constructivism and beyond, in B.J. Fraser, K.G. Tobin (Eds.), International handbook of science education (London: Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1998) 3-25. [18] U. Kattmann, Learning biology by means of anthropomorphic conceptions? in M. Hammann, M. Reiss, C. Boulter, and S.D. Tunnicliffe (Eds.), Biology in context: learning and teaching for the twenty-first century (London: Institute of Education, 2008) 7-17. [19] E. Gaigher, N.G. Lederman, and J. Lederman, Knowledge about Inquiry: A study in South African high schools, International Journal of Science Education, 36(18), 2014, 3125-3147. [20] N.G. Lederman, F. Abd-El-Khalick, R.L. Bell, and R.S. Schwartz, Views of nature of science questionnaire: Toward valid and meaningful assessment of learners' conceptions of nature of science, Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(6), 2002, 497-521. [21] R.S. Schwartz, N.G. Lederman, and B.A. Crawford, Developing views of nature of science in an authentic context: An explicit approach to bridging the gap between nature of science and scientific inquiry, Science Education, 88(4), 2004, 610-645. [22] P. Mayring, Qualitative Inhaltsanalyse, Grundlagen und Techniken (Weinheim: Beltz, 2010). [23] I. Seidman, Interviewing as qualitative research: A guide for researchers in education and the social sciences (Teachers College Press, 2012). [24] D. Klahr, Exploring Science: The Cognition and Development of Discovery Processes (Cambridge: MIT Press, 2002). [25] D.A. Kolb, Experiential learning: Experience as the source of learning and development (FT press, 2014).