SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Implicit & Explicit Learning,
Knowledge and Instruction
ATU PhD
Course: SLA theories
Professor: Dr. Khatib
Prepared by: M.Soleimani Aghchay
Distinction between Implicit/Explicit
Learning and Knowledge
Cognitive psychologists distinguish implicit and explicit
learning in two principal ways:
1) Implicit learning proceeds without making demands
on central attentional resources. Thus, the resulting
knowledge is subsymbolic, reflecting statistical
sensitivity to the structure of the learned material. In
contrast, explicit learning typically involves
memorizing a series of successive facts and thus
makes heavy demands on working memory. As a
result, it takes place consciously and results in
knowledge that is symbolic in nature (i.e. it is
represented in explicit form).
2) In the case of implicit learning, learners
remain unaware of the learning that
has taken place, although it is evident
in the behavioral responses they make.
Thus, learners cannot verbalize what
they have learned. In the case of
explicit learning, learners are aware
that they have learned something and
can verbalize what they have learned.
Rod Ellis, 2009:3
Distinction between Implicit/Explicit Learning and Knowledge
Nick Ellis’ (1994) distinction:
Some things we just come able to do, like walking,
recognizing happiness in others, knowing that th
is more common than tg in written English, or
making simple utterances in our native language.
We have little insight into the nature of the
processing involved we learn to do them implicitly
like swallows learn to fly. Other of our abilities
depend on knowing how to do them, like
multiplication, playing chess, speaking pig Latin,
or using a computer programming language. We
learn these abilities explicitly like aircraft
designers learn aerodynamics. (Ellis, 1994: 1)
Rod Ellis’ (2009) distinction:
Implicit/explicit learning and implicit/explicit
knowledge are ‘related but distinct concepts that
need to be separated’. Whereas the former refers
to the processes involved in learning, the latter
concerns the products of learning.
Schmidt also argued that learning needs to be
distinguished from instruction. It does not follow,
for instance, that implicit instruction results in
implicit learning or, conversely, that explicit
instruction leads to explicit learning. Learners have
minds of their own and may follow their own
inclinations, irrespective of the nature of the
instruction they receive (Allwright,1984)
Distinction between Implicit/Explicit Learning and Knowledge
Dornyei’s (2009) distinction:
Explicit learning refers to the learner’s
conscious and deliberate attempt to master
some material or solve a problem. In
contrast, implicit learning involves
acquiring skills and knowledge without
conscious awareness, that is,
automatically and with no conscious
attempt to learn them.
Distinction between Implicit/Explicit Learning and Knowledge
How SLA researchers have
tackled the three distinctions
Implicit/Explicit
Learning
Implicit/Explicit
Knowledge
Implicit/Explicit
Instruction
Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning
There is controversy as to whether any
learning is possible without some degree of
awareness
Schmidt (1994, 2001) distinguished two
types of awareness:
 Awareness as noticing (involving perception):
involves conscious attention to ‘surface
elements’
 Metalinguistic awareness (involving analysis):
involves awareness of the underlying abstract
rule that governs particular linguistic phenomena
Schmidt argued that there is no such thing
as complete implicit learning and so a
better definition of implicit language
learning might be ‘learning without any
metalinguistic awareness’
Explicit language learning is necessarily a
conscious process and is generally
intentional as well. It is conscious learning
‘where the individual makes and tests
hypotheses in a search for structure’ (N.
Ellis, 1994: 1)
Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
Dornyei, 2009 states that “the problem with implicit
language learning is that while it does such a great
job in generating native-speaking L1 proficiency in
infants, it does not seem to work efficiently when we
want to master an L2 at a later stage in our lives.
Evidence:
 Experiences in educational contexts that provide optimal
conditions for implicit learning and yet which typically fail to
deliver nativelike L2 proficiency
 Reviews of empirical studies that specifically compared
implicit and explicit instruction, which demonstrate a
significant advantage of explicit types of L2 instruction over
implicit types (for a seminal paper in this regard, see Norris
and Ortega, 2000).
Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
The general finding is that explicit
learning is more effective than implicit
learning (N. Ellis, 1993; Rosa & O’Neill,
1999; Gass et al., 2003). No study has
shown that implicit learning worked
better than explicit learning.
Two studies found no difference
between implicit and explicit learning
(Doughty, 1991; Shook, 1994).
Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
Dornyei (2009) concludes that:
“mere exposure to L2 input accompanied by
communicative practice is not sufficient, and,
therefore, we need explicit learning
procedures – such as focus on form or some
kind of controlled practice – to push learners
beyond communicatively effective language
toward target-like second language ability.”
In his view, the real challenge is to
maximise the cooperation of explicit
and implicit learning. (Dornyei, 2009: 36)
Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
Implicit and Explicit L2 Knowledge
What is meant by ‘linguistic knowledge’?
1) Innatist and Mentalist View: linguistic knowledge
consists of knowledge of the features of a specific
language, which are derived from impoverished input
(positive evidence) with the help of Universal
Grammar (UG)
2) Connectionist Theories of Language Learning:
linguistic knowledge is comprised of an elaborate
network of nodes and internode connections of
varying strengths that dictate the ease with which
specific sequences or ‘rules’ can be accessed. Thus,
learning is driven primarily by input and it is
necessary to posit only a relatively simple cognitive
mechanism that is capable of responding both to
positive evidence from the input and to negative
evidence available through corrective feedback.
Criteria to Distinguish Implicit and
Explicit L2 Knowledge
Implicit knowledge is tacit and intuitive whereas explicit knowledge is
conscious
Implicit knowledge is procedural whereas explicit knowledge is declarative
L2 learners’ procedural rules may or may not be target-like while their
declarative rules are often imprecise and inaccurate
Implicit knowledge is available through automatic processing whereas explicit
knowledge is generally accessible only through controlled processing
Default L2 production relies on implicit knowledge, but difficulty in performing a
language task may result in the learner attempting to exploit explicit knowledge
Implicit knowledge is only evident in learners’ verbal behavior whereas explicit
knowledge is verbalizable
There are limits on most learners’ ability to acquire implicit knowledge
whereas most explicit knowledge is learnable
The learner’s L2 implicit and explicit knowledge systems are distinct
L2 performance utilizes a combination of implicit and explicit knowledge
Implicit knowledge is tacit and intuitive
whereas explicit knowledge is conscious
*The policeman explained Wong the law.
A learner may know intuitively that there is something
ungrammatical and may even be able to identify the
part of the sentence where the error occurs, but may
have no conscious awareness of the rule that is being
broken. Such a learner has implicit but no explicit
knowledge of the feature in question.
Another learner, however, may understand that the
sentence is ungrammatical because the verb ‘explain’
cannot be followed by an indirect object without ‘to’.
A third learner (a linguist perhaps) might know that
dative verbs like ‘explain’ that are of Latin origin and
verbs like ‘give’ that are of Anglo-Saxon origin
perform differently
Implicit knowledge is procedural, explicit
knowledge is declarative
For example, for past tense verbs, learners
behave in accordance with a condition-
action rule along the lines of ‘if the action
to be referred to occurred in the past and
is completed, then add -ed to the base
form of a verb’. Explicit knowledge is
comprised of facts about the L2. These
facts are only loosely connected; they do
not constitute a ‘system’ in the same way
that the implicit knowledge of proficient L2
users does.
L2 learners’ procedural rules may or may not be
target-like while their declarative rules are often
imprecise and inaccurate
SLA research has shown that learners typically manifest
developmental sequences when they acquire implicit
knowledge
 For example, the condition-action rule for the past tense
would lead to both correct forms (e.g. ‘jumped’) and also
overgeneralized forms (e.g. ‘eated’).
In the case of explicit knowledge, learners’ knowledge is
often fuzzy.
 For example, a learner who responded to the ungrammatical
sentence (*The policeman explained Wong the law) with the
comment ‘You can’t use a proper noun after ‘‘explain’’ ’
clearly has some explicit understanding of what makes the
sentence ungrammatical, but equally clearly does not have a
very accurate notion.
Implicit knowledge is available through automatic
processing , explicit knowledge is generally
accessible only through controlled processing
The ‘procedures’ that comprise implicit knowledge
can be easily and rapidly accessed in unplanned
language use.
Explicit knowledge exists as declarative facts that
can only be accessed through the application of
attentional processes. For this reason, explicit
knowledge may not be readily available in
spontaneous language use where there is little
opportunity for careful online planning.
It is possible, however, that some learners are able
to automatize their explicit knowledge through
practice and thus access it for rapid online
processing in much the same way as they access
implicit knowledge.
Default L2 production relies on implicit
knowledge, but difficulty in performing a
language task may result in the learner
attempting to exploit explicit knowledge
When learners are asked to make and justify
grammaticality judgments in a think-aloud
or dyadic problem-solving task, they
typically try to access declarative
information to help them do so. (R. Ellis,
1991; Goss et al.,1994)
Implicit knowledge is only evident in learners’
verbal behavior whereas explicit knowledge is
verbalizable
Learners cannot explain their choice of implicit
forms. In contrast, explicit knowledge exists as
declarative facts that can be ‘stated’. It is
important to recognize that verbalizing a rule or
feature need not entail the use of
metalanguage, e.g. the error in the mentioned
sentence might be explained nontechnically by
saying ‘You can’t say ‘‘explain Wong’’. You’ve
got to say ‘‘to Wong’’ after ‘‘explain’’’.
There are limits on most learners’ ability to
acquire implicit knowledge whereas most
explicit knowledge is learnable
There are incremental deficits in our ability to
learn implicit knowledge as we age
(Birdsong, 2006). In contrast, as Bialystok
(1994: 566) pointed out, ‘explicit knowledge
can be learned at any age’, and it is not
perhaps until old age that learning deficits
become apparent.
The learner’s L2 implicit and explicit
knowledge systems are distinct
An issue of considerable importance and
controversy:
 Some researchers (Krashen,1981, Paradis, 1994:
397, 2004, Ullman, 2001: 39) have provided
evidence for the separateness of the two, some
(e.g. Dienes & Perner, 1999) have viewed the
distinction as continuous rather than dichotomous.
 Ellis (2004) holds that where representation (but not
language use) is concerned we would do better to
view the two types of knowledge as dichotomous.
L2 performance utilizes a combination
of implicit and explicit knowledge
For example, a learner may have internalized
‘jumped’ as a single item in explicit memory,
but may also have developed the procedure
for affixing -ed to the base form of the verb
in implicit memory as suggested by Ullman.
Thus, the neurological distinctiveness of the
two systems will be difficult to detect from
simply examining a learner’s linguistic
behavior.
Language
Instruction:
An attempt to
intervene in
interlanguage
development
Indirect intervention:
aims to create
conditions where
learners can learn
experientially through
learning how to
communicate in the L2
(Ellis, 2005, p. 713)
Direct intervention:
involves the pre-
emptive specification
of what it is that the
learners are supposed
to learn and, draws on
a structural syllabus.
Implicit instruction:
involves creating a
learning environment that
is ‘enriched’ with the
target feature, but without
drawing learners’ explicit
attention to it.
Explicit instruction:
involves ‘some sort of rule
being thought about during
the learning process’
(DeKeyser, 1995).
Learners are encouraged
to develop metalinguistic
awareness of the rule. Can
be achieved deductively /
inductively
Implicit and Explicit Instruction
Types of Language Instruction
Implicit
indirect
attention to
linguistic form
arises out of the
way the tasks are
performed
when
tasks are designed
to elicit the use of a
specific linguistic
target
Explicit
occurs when
teachers provide explicit
or metalinguistic
corrective feedback on
learner’ errors in the use
of the target feature
occurs when
the teacher offers a
metalinguistic
explanation of the target
rule prior to any practice
activities
Housen and
Pierrard
(2006)
Table 1.1 Implicit and explicit instruction
(Housen & Pierrard, 2006: 10)
Robinson’s (1996) operationalizations
of implicit and explicit instruction
Four instructional conditions:
1) An implicit condition, which involved asking
learners to remember sentences containing
the target structures
2) An incidental condition consisting of
exposure to sentences containing the target
structure in a meaning-centered task
3) A rule-search condition involving identifying
the rules
4) An instructed condition where written
explanations of rules were provided
The Interface Issue
Concerns the extent to which implicit
knowledge interfaces with explicit
knowledge
Addresses a number of questions:
 To what extent and in what ways are implicit
and explicit learning related?
 Does explicit knowledge convert into or
facilitate the acquisition of implicit knowledge?
 Does explicit instruction result in the
acquisition of implicit as well as explicit
knowledge?
Three very different answers to the
interface question have been offered:
1) The noninterface position
2) The strong interface position
3) The weak interface position
The Noninterface Position
Draws on research that shows that implicit and
explicit L2 knowledge involve different
acquisitional mechanisms (Krashen, 1981;
Hulstijn, 2002), are stored in different parts of
the brain (Paradis, 1994) and are accessed for
performance by means of different processes,
automatic versus controlled (R. Ellis, 1993).
This position rejects both the possibility of
explicit knowledge transforming directly into
implicit knowledge and the possibility of implicit
knowledge becoming explicit.
The strong interface position
Claims that not only can explicit knowledge
be derived from implicit knowledge, but
also that explicit knowledge can be
converted into implicit knowledge through
practice. (Sharwood Smith, 1981) and
(DeKeyser, 1998, 2007)
The weak interface position
Exists in three versions, all of which
acknowledge the possibility of explicit
knowledge becoming implicit, but posit some
limitation on when or how this can take place
1) One version posits that explicit knowledge can
convert into implicit knowledge through practice,
but only if the learner is developmentally ready to
acquire the linguistic form. This version draws on
notions of ‘learnability’ in accordance with attested
developmental sequences in L2 acquisition (e.g.
Pienemann, 1989).
2) The second version sees explicit knowledge as
contributing indirectly to the acquisition of implicit
knowledge by promoting some of the processes
believed to be responsible. N. Ellis (1994: 16), for
example, suggests that ‘declarative rules can have
‘‘top-down’’ influences on perception’. Such a
position suggests that implicit and explicit learning
processes work together in L2 acquisition and that
they are dynamic, taking place consciously but
transiently with enduring effects on implicit
knowledge (N. Ellis, 2008).
3) According to the third version, learners can use
their explicit knowledge to produce output that then
serves as ‘auto-input’ to their implicit learning
mechanisms (Schmidt & Frota, 1986; Sharwood
Smith, 1981).
Neurolinguistic studies lend some support to the
interface positions. Lee (2004: 67), for
example, suggested that neuroanatomy allows
for an interface between declarative and
procedural memory:
When (the learner) utters a sentence that violates
the rule, his or her declarative memory may send a
signal indicating that the utterance is wrong. This
signal may prevent the formation of connections
among neurons that could have represented the
incorrect rule. On the other hand, when the
speaker executes a correct sentence, this
information aligns with that of declarative memory,
and the connection that represents the sentence
or the rule involved in the sentence may become
stronger.
Lee’s account appears to lend support to
both a strong interface position (i.e.
declarative memory can convert into
procedural memory) and a weak interface
position (i.e. declarative memory can help
adjust the neural circuits in which
procedural memory is housed). Other
neuroscientific researchers, however, have
rejected the possibility of a strong interface
and emphasized the weak interface
position. Paradis (2004) and Crowell (2004)
Resources
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The Psychology of Second
Language Acquisition.
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). Communicative language
teaching in the 21st century: The 'principled
communicative approach'. Perspectives,
36(2), 33-43.
Peter Robinson: Attention and Awareness
(Volume 6)
Rod Ellis, (2009). The Study of Second
Language Acquisition, 2nd Edition
Rod Ellis: Explicit Knowledge and Second
Language Learning and Pedagogy (Volume 6)
Thank you!
Wish you a better
state of the mind!!!

More Related Content

PPTX
Needs Analysis and Evaluation In English Specific Purposes
PPTX
Acculturation model
PPTX
Explicit and implicit grammar teaching
PPTX
Media based arts and design in the philippines
PPTX
Language learning strategies
PDF
Henry the last leaf and other stories
PPTX
Language and gender
PPTX
John Schumann's Acculturation Model
Needs Analysis and Evaluation In English Specific Purposes
Acculturation model
Explicit and implicit grammar teaching
Media based arts and design in the philippines
Language learning strategies
Henry the last leaf and other stories
Language and gender
John Schumann's Acculturation Model

What's hot (20)

PPTX
Second Language Acquisition (Error Analysis)
PPTX
Teaching pronunciation
PPTX
The use of technology to teach and learn English
PPT
Stages of Acquisition of first Language
PPTX
Factors that influence second language acquisition and learning
PPTX
ERROR ANALYSIS
PPTX
Krashen monitor model
PPTX
GTM method
PPTX
Multilingualism and bilingualism
PPTX
Role of motivation in language learning
PPTX
Styles and strategies in second language learning
PPT
Factors affecting second language learning
PPTX
Community Language Learning
PPTX
Language acquisition
PPTX
Stephan Krashen's five hypotheses
PPTX
Formal instruction and language learning
PPTX
Communicative language teaching
PPT
Inter-language theory
PPTX
Language and linguistics error analysis
PPTX
Teaching by principles
Second Language Acquisition (Error Analysis)
Teaching pronunciation
The use of technology to teach and learn English
Stages of Acquisition of first Language
Factors that influence second language acquisition and learning
ERROR ANALYSIS
Krashen monitor model
GTM method
Multilingualism and bilingualism
Role of motivation in language learning
Styles and strategies in second language learning
Factors affecting second language learning
Community Language Learning
Language acquisition
Stephan Krashen's five hypotheses
Formal instruction and language learning
Communicative language teaching
Inter-language theory
Language and linguistics error analysis
Teaching by principles
Ad

Viewers also liked (13)

PPT
Included 3 the critical period 2013
PPTX
Age and acquisition learning activity 2
PPTX
Age and acquisition
PPTX
neurological, cognitive, affective and linguistic considerations
PPTX
Age and acquisition
PPT
Neurological, cognitive, affective and linguistic considerations
PPTX
Ageandacquisitionnew
PPTX
John dryden
PPTX
Implicit and explicit messages
PPTX
Significance of accent
ODP
Age and acquisition
PPTX
Age and acquisition
PPT
Age and Second Language Acquisition
Included 3 the critical period 2013
Age and acquisition learning activity 2
Age and acquisition
neurological, cognitive, affective and linguistic considerations
Age and acquisition
Neurological, cognitive, affective and linguistic considerations
Ageandacquisitionnew
John dryden
Implicit and explicit messages
Significance of accent
Age and acquisition
Age and acquisition
Age and Second Language Acquisition
Ad

Similar to Implicit & Explicit learning, knowledge and instruction (20)

PPTX
learning styles .pptx
PPT
Universal grammar med
PPTX
Second language acquisition
 
PPTX
[SLA] Psycholinguistics and linguistics aspects of interlanguage.pptx
DOCX
Chapter 4 explaining second language learning
PPT
2 theoretical approaches_to_l2 (1)
PPT
Chapter three -Approaches to the study of second language acquisition-All.ppt
PPTX
Inter language
PPTX
Explaining second language learning
PPTX
Lecture 7. theories explaining second language (2)
PPTX
Psychological Basis of CA praesentation.pptx
PPTX
ERROR SONITA 6.pptx
PPT
second language acquisition
PPTX
ORTEGA PPT.pptx
PPTX
Sla bab 1ppt
PPTX
Ppt of-interlanguage-chapter-3
PPTX
Second Language Learning
PPTX
Chapter 4 how languages are learned - pasty m. lightbown and nina spada
PPTX
02. Second Language Acquisition.pptx
learning styles .pptx
Universal grammar med
Second language acquisition
 
[SLA] Psycholinguistics and linguistics aspects of interlanguage.pptx
Chapter 4 explaining second language learning
2 theoretical approaches_to_l2 (1)
Chapter three -Approaches to the study of second language acquisition-All.ppt
Inter language
Explaining second language learning
Lecture 7. theories explaining second language (2)
Psychological Basis of CA praesentation.pptx
ERROR SONITA 6.pptx
second language acquisition
ORTEGA PPT.pptx
Sla bab 1ppt
Ppt of-interlanguage-chapter-3
Second Language Learning
Chapter 4 how languages are learned - pasty m. lightbown and nina spada
02. Second Language Acquisition.pptx

More from aghchay (10)

PPTX
Affective factors
PPTX
Traditional ways of teaching culture
PPTX
English language teacher education curriculum
PPTX
Language teacher education by distance
PPTX
The curriculum of slte
PPTX
Traditional ways of teaching culture
PPTX
Culture and non verbal communication
PPTX
Traditional ways of teaching culture
PPTX
Learning styles
PPTX
The role of age in sla studies
Affective factors
Traditional ways of teaching culture
English language teacher education curriculum
Language teacher education by distance
The curriculum of slte
Traditional ways of teaching culture
Culture and non verbal communication
Traditional ways of teaching culture
Learning styles
The role of age in sla studies

Recently uploaded (20)

PDF
ANTIBIOTICS.pptx.pdf………………… xxxxxxxxxxxxx
PDF
2.FourierTransform-ShortQuestionswithAnswers.pdf
PDF
Insiders guide to clinical Medicine.pdf
PPTX
Pharma ospi slides which help in ospi learning
PDF
O7-L3 Supply Chain Operations - ICLT Program
PPTX
Final Presentation General Medicine 03-08-2024.pptx
PPTX
Renaissance Architecture: A Journey from Faith to Humanism
PPTX
Cell Structure & Organelles in detailed.
PDF
Mark Klimek Lecture Notes_240423 revision books _173037.pdf
PDF
Physiotherapy_for_Respiratory_and_Cardiac_Problems WEBBER.pdf
PDF
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
PDF
Basic Mud Logging Guide for educational purpose
PPTX
human mycosis Human fungal infections are called human mycosis..pptx
PPTX
master seminar digital applications in india
PPTX
Week 4 Term 3 Study Techniques revisited.pptx
PDF
Microbial disease of the cardiovascular and lymphatic systems
PDF
TR - Agricultural Crops Production NC III.pdf
PDF
01-Introduction-to-Information-Management.pdf
PPTX
Introduction_to_Human_Anatomy_and_Physiology_for_B.Pharm.pptx
PDF
Complications of Minimal Access Surgery at WLH
ANTIBIOTICS.pptx.pdf………………… xxxxxxxxxxxxx
2.FourierTransform-ShortQuestionswithAnswers.pdf
Insiders guide to clinical Medicine.pdf
Pharma ospi slides which help in ospi learning
O7-L3 Supply Chain Operations - ICLT Program
Final Presentation General Medicine 03-08-2024.pptx
Renaissance Architecture: A Journey from Faith to Humanism
Cell Structure & Organelles in detailed.
Mark Klimek Lecture Notes_240423 revision books _173037.pdf
Physiotherapy_for_Respiratory_and_Cardiac_Problems WEBBER.pdf
Classroom Observation Tools for Teachers
Basic Mud Logging Guide for educational purpose
human mycosis Human fungal infections are called human mycosis..pptx
master seminar digital applications in india
Week 4 Term 3 Study Techniques revisited.pptx
Microbial disease of the cardiovascular and lymphatic systems
TR - Agricultural Crops Production NC III.pdf
01-Introduction-to-Information-Management.pdf
Introduction_to_Human_Anatomy_and_Physiology_for_B.Pharm.pptx
Complications of Minimal Access Surgery at WLH

Implicit & Explicit learning, knowledge and instruction

  • 1. Implicit & Explicit Learning, Knowledge and Instruction ATU PhD Course: SLA theories Professor: Dr. Khatib Prepared by: M.Soleimani Aghchay
  • 2. Distinction between Implicit/Explicit Learning and Knowledge Cognitive psychologists distinguish implicit and explicit learning in two principal ways: 1) Implicit learning proceeds without making demands on central attentional resources. Thus, the resulting knowledge is subsymbolic, reflecting statistical sensitivity to the structure of the learned material. In contrast, explicit learning typically involves memorizing a series of successive facts and thus makes heavy demands on working memory. As a result, it takes place consciously and results in knowledge that is symbolic in nature (i.e. it is represented in explicit form).
  • 3. 2) In the case of implicit learning, learners remain unaware of the learning that has taken place, although it is evident in the behavioral responses they make. Thus, learners cannot verbalize what they have learned. In the case of explicit learning, learners are aware that they have learned something and can verbalize what they have learned. Rod Ellis, 2009:3
  • 4. Distinction between Implicit/Explicit Learning and Knowledge Nick Ellis’ (1994) distinction: Some things we just come able to do, like walking, recognizing happiness in others, knowing that th is more common than tg in written English, or making simple utterances in our native language. We have little insight into the nature of the processing involved we learn to do them implicitly like swallows learn to fly. Other of our abilities depend on knowing how to do them, like multiplication, playing chess, speaking pig Latin, or using a computer programming language. We learn these abilities explicitly like aircraft designers learn aerodynamics. (Ellis, 1994: 1)
  • 5. Rod Ellis’ (2009) distinction: Implicit/explicit learning and implicit/explicit knowledge are ‘related but distinct concepts that need to be separated’. Whereas the former refers to the processes involved in learning, the latter concerns the products of learning. Schmidt also argued that learning needs to be distinguished from instruction. It does not follow, for instance, that implicit instruction results in implicit learning or, conversely, that explicit instruction leads to explicit learning. Learners have minds of their own and may follow their own inclinations, irrespective of the nature of the instruction they receive (Allwright,1984) Distinction between Implicit/Explicit Learning and Knowledge
  • 6. Dornyei’s (2009) distinction: Explicit learning refers to the learner’s conscious and deliberate attempt to master some material or solve a problem. In contrast, implicit learning involves acquiring skills and knowledge without conscious awareness, that is, automatically and with no conscious attempt to learn them. Distinction between Implicit/Explicit Learning and Knowledge
  • 7. How SLA researchers have tackled the three distinctions Implicit/Explicit Learning Implicit/Explicit Knowledge Implicit/Explicit Instruction
  • 8. Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning There is controversy as to whether any learning is possible without some degree of awareness Schmidt (1994, 2001) distinguished two types of awareness:  Awareness as noticing (involving perception): involves conscious attention to ‘surface elements’  Metalinguistic awareness (involving analysis): involves awareness of the underlying abstract rule that governs particular linguistic phenomena
  • 9. Schmidt argued that there is no such thing as complete implicit learning and so a better definition of implicit language learning might be ‘learning without any metalinguistic awareness’ Explicit language learning is necessarily a conscious process and is generally intentional as well. It is conscious learning ‘where the individual makes and tests hypotheses in a search for structure’ (N. Ellis, 1994: 1) Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
  • 10. Dornyei, 2009 states that “the problem with implicit language learning is that while it does such a great job in generating native-speaking L1 proficiency in infants, it does not seem to work efficiently when we want to master an L2 at a later stage in our lives. Evidence:  Experiences in educational contexts that provide optimal conditions for implicit learning and yet which typically fail to deliver nativelike L2 proficiency  Reviews of empirical studies that specifically compared implicit and explicit instruction, which demonstrate a significant advantage of explicit types of L2 instruction over implicit types (for a seminal paper in this regard, see Norris and Ortega, 2000). Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
  • 11. The general finding is that explicit learning is more effective than implicit learning (N. Ellis, 1993; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Gass et al., 2003). No study has shown that implicit learning worked better than explicit learning. Two studies found no difference between implicit and explicit learning (Doughty, 1991; Shook, 1994). Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
  • 12. Dornyei (2009) concludes that: “mere exposure to L2 input accompanied by communicative practice is not sufficient, and, therefore, we need explicit learning procedures – such as focus on form or some kind of controlled practice – to push learners beyond communicatively effective language toward target-like second language ability.” In his view, the real challenge is to maximise the cooperation of explicit and implicit learning. (Dornyei, 2009: 36) Implicit/Explicit L2 Learning (cont.)
  • 13. Implicit and Explicit L2 Knowledge What is meant by ‘linguistic knowledge’? 1) Innatist and Mentalist View: linguistic knowledge consists of knowledge of the features of a specific language, which are derived from impoverished input (positive evidence) with the help of Universal Grammar (UG) 2) Connectionist Theories of Language Learning: linguistic knowledge is comprised of an elaborate network of nodes and internode connections of varying strengths that dictate the ease with which specific sequences or ‘rules’ can be accessed. Thus, learning is driven primarily by input and it is necessary to posit only a relatively simple cognitive mechanism that is capable of responding both to positive evidence from the input and to negative evidence available through corrective feedback.
  • 14. Criteria to Distinguish Implicit and Explicit L2 Knowledge Implicit knowledge is tacit and intuitive whereas explicit knowledge is conscious Implicit knowledge is procedural whereas explicit knowledge is declarative L2 learners’ procedural rules may or may not be target-like while their declarative rules are often imprecise and inaccurate Implicit knowledge is available through automatic processing whereas explicit knowledge is generally accessible only through controlled processing Default L2 production relies on implicit knowledge, but difficulty in performing a language task may result in the learner attempting to exploit explicit knowledge Implicit knowledge is only evident in learners’ verbal behavior whereas explicit knowledge is verbalizable There are limits on most learners’ ability to acquire implicit knowledge whereas most explicit knowledge is learnable The learner’s L2 implicit and explicit knowledge systems are distinct L2 performance utilizes a combination of implicit and explicit knowledge
  • 15. Implicit knowledge is tacit and intuitive whereas explicit knowledge is conscious *The policeman explained Wong the law. A learner may know intuitively that there is something ungrammatical and may even be able to identify the part of the sentence where the error occurs, but may have no conscious awareness of the rule that is being broken. Such a learner has implicit but no explicit knowledge of the feature in question. Another learner, however, may understand that the sentence is ungrammatical because the verb ‘explain’ cannot be followed by an indirect object without ‘to’. A third learner (a linguist perhaps) might know that dative verbs like ‘explain’ that are of Latin origin and verbs like ‘give’ that are of Anglo-Saxon origin perform differently
  • 16. Implicit knowledge is procedural, explicit knowledge is declarative For example, for past tense verbs, learners behave in accordance with a condition- action rule along the lines of ‘if the action to be referred to occurred in the past and is completed, then add -ed to the base form of a verb’. Explicit knowledge is comprised of facts about the L2. These facts are only loosely connected; they do not constitute a ‘system’ in the same way that the implicit knowledge of proficient L2 users does.
  • 17. L2 learners’ procedural rules may or may not be target-like while their declarative rules are often imprecise and inaccurate SLA research has shown that learners typically manifest developmental sequences when they acquire implicit knowledge  For example, the condition-action rule for the past tense would lead to both correct forms (e.g. ‘jumped’) and also overgeneralized forms (e.g. ‘eated’). In the case of explicit knowledge, learners’ knowledge is often fuzzy.  For example, a learner who responded to the ungrammatical sentence (*The policeman explained Wong the law) with the comment ‘You can’t use a proper noun after ‘‘explain’’ ’ clearly has some explicit understanding of what makes the sentence ungrammatical, but equally clearly does not have a very accurate notion.
  • 18. Implicit knowledge is available through automatic processing , explicit knowledge is generally accessible only through controlled processing The ‘procedures’ that comprise implicit knowledge can be easily and rapidly accessed in unplanned language use. Explicit knowledge exists as declarative facts that can only be accessed through the application of attentional processes. For this reason, explicit knowledge may not be readily available in spontaneous language use where there is little opportunity for careful online planning. It is possible, however, that some learners are able to automatize their explicit knowledge through practice and thus access it for rapid online processing in much the same way as they access implicit knowledge.
  • 19. Default L2 production relies on implicit knowledge, but difficulty in performing a language task may result in the learner attempting to exploit explicit knowledge When learners are asked to make and justify grammaticality judgments in a think-aloud or dyadic problem-solving task, they typically try to access declarative information to help them do so. (R. Ellis, 1991; Goss et al.,1994)
  • 20. Implicit knowledge is only evident in learners’ verbal behavior whereas explicit knowledge is verbalizable Learners cannot explain their choice of implicit forms. In contrast, explicit knowledge exists as declarative facts that can be ‘stated’. It is important to recognize that verbalizing a rule or feature need not entail the use of metalanguage, e.g. the error in the mentioned sentence might be explained nontechnically by saying ‘You can’t say ‘‘explain Wong’’. You’ve got to say ‘‘to Wong’’ after ‘‘explain’’’.
  • 21. There are limits on most learners’ ability to acquire implicit knowledge whereas most explicit knowledge is learnable There are incremental deficits in our ability to learn implicit knowledge as we age (Birdsong, 2006). In contrast, as Bialystok (1994: 566) pointed out, ‘explicit knowledge can be learned at any age’, and it is not perhaps until old age that learning deficits become apparent.
  • 22. The learner’s L2 implicit and explicit knowledge systems are distinct An issue of considerable importance and controversy:  Some researchers (Krashen,1981, Paradis, 1994: 397, 2004, Ullman, 2001: 39) have provided evidence for the separateness of the two, some (e.g. Dienes & Perner, 1999) have viewed the distinction as continuous rather than dichotomous.  Ellis (2004) holds that where representation (but not language use) is concerned we would do better to view the two types of knowledge as dichotomous.
  • 23. L2 performance utilizes a combination of implicit and explicit knowledge For example, a learner may have internalized ‘jumped’ as a single item in explicit memory, but may also have developed the procedure for affixing -ed to the base form of the verb in implicit memory as suggested by Ullman. Thus, the neurological distinctiveness of the two systems will be difficult to detect from simply examining a learner’s linguistic behavior.
  • 24. Language Instruction: An attempt to intervene in interlanguage development Indirect intervention: aims to create conditions where learners can learn experientially through learning how to communicate in the L2 (Ellis, 2005, p. 713) Direct intervention: involves the pre- emptive specification of what it is that the learners are supposed to learn and, draws on a structural syllabus. Implicit instruction: involves creating a learning environment that is ‘enriched’ with the target feature, but without drawing learners’ explicit attention to it. Explicit instruction: involves ‘some sort of rule being thought about during the learning process’ (DeKeyser, 1995). Learners are encouraged to develop metalinguistic awareness of the rule. Can be achieved deductively / inductively Implicit and Explicit Instruction
  • 25. Types of Language Instruction Implicit indirect attention to linguistic form arises out of the way the tasks are performed when tasks are designed to elicit the use of a specific linguistic target Explicit occurs when teachers provide explicit or metalinguistic corrective feedback on learner’ errors in the use of the target feature occurs when the teacher offers a metalinguistic explanation of the target rule prior to any practice activities Housen and Pierrard (2006)
  • 26. Table 1.1 Implicit and explicit instruction (Housen & Pierrard, 2006: 10)
  • 27. Robinson’s (1996) operationalizations of implicit and explicit instruction Four instructional conditions: 1) An implicit condition, which involved asking learners to remember sentences containing the target structures 2) An incidental condition consisting of exposure to sentences containing the target structure in a meaning-centered task 3) A rule-search condition involving identifying the rules 4) An instructed condition where written explanations of rules were provided
  • 28. The Interface Issue Concerns the extent to which implicit knowledge interfaces with explicit knowledge Addresses a number of questions:  To what extent and in what ways are implicit and explicit learning related?  Does explicit knowledge convert into or facilitate the acquisition of implicit knowledge?  Does explicit instruction result in the acquisition of implicit as well as explicit knowledge?
  • 29. Three very different answers to the interface question have been offered: 1) The noninterface position 2) The strong interface position 3) The weak interface position
  • 30. The Noninterface Position Draws on research that shows that implicit and explicit L2 knowledge involve different acquisitional mechanisms (Krashen, 1981; Hulstijn, 2002), are stored in different parts of the brain (Paradis, 1994) and are accessed for performance by means of different processes, automatic versus controlled (R. Ellis, 1993). This position rejects both the possibility of explicit knowledge transforming directly into implicit knowledge and the possibility of implicit knowledge becoming explicit.
  • 31. The strong interface position Claims that not only can explicit knowledge be derived from implicit knowledge, but also that explicit knowledge can be converted into implicit knowledge through practice. (Sharwood Smith, 1981) and (DeKeyser, 1998, 2007)
  • 32. The weak interface position Exists in three versions, all of which acknowledge the possibility of explicit knowledge becoming implicit, but posit some limitation on when or how this can take place 1) One version posits that explicit knowledge can convert into implicit knowledge through practice, but only if the learner is developmentally ready to acquire the linguistic form. This version draws on notions of ‘learnability’ in accordance with attested developmental sequences in L2 acquisition (e.g. Pienemann, 1989).
  • 33. 2) The second version sees explicit knowledge as contributing indirectly to the acquisition of implicit knowledge by promoting some of the processes believed to be responsible. N. Ellis (1994: 16), for example, suggests that ‘declarative rules can have ‘‘top-down’’ influences on perception’. Such a position suggests that implicit and explicit learning processes work together in L2 acquisition and that they are dynamic, taking place consciously but transiently with enduring effects on implicit knowledge (N. Ellis, 2008). 3) According to the third version, learners can use their explicit knowledge to produce output that then serves as ‘auto-input’ to their implicit learning mechanisms (Schmidt & Frota, 1986; Sharwood Smith, 1981).
  • 34. Neurolinguistic studies lend some support to the interface positions. Lee (2004: 67), for example, suggested that neuroanatomy allows for an interface between declarative and procedural memory: When (the learner) utters a sentence that violates the rule, his or her declarative memory may send a signal indicating that the utterance is wrong. This signal may prevent the formation of connections among neurons that could have represented the incorrect rule. On the other hand, when the speaker executes a correct sentence, this information aligns with that of declarative memory, and the connection that represents the sentence or the rule involved in the sentence may become stronger.
  • 35. Lee’s account appears to lend support to both a strong interface position (i.e. declarative memory can convert into procedural memory) and a weak interface position (i.e. declarative memory can help adjust the neural circuits in which procedural memory is housed). Other neuroscientific researchers, however, have rejected the possibility of a strong interface and emphasized the weak interface position. Paradis (2004) and Crowell (2004)
  • 36. Resources Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The Psychology of Second Language Acquisition. Dörnyei, Z. (2009). Communicative language teaching in the 21st century: The 'principled communicative approach'. Perspectives, 36(2), 33-43. Peter Robinson: Attention and Awareness (Volume 6) Rod Ellis, (2009). The Study of Second Language Acquisition, 2nd Edition Rod Ellis: Explicit Knowledge and Second Language Learning and Pedagogy (Volume 6)
  • 37. Thank you! Wish you a better state of the mind!!!