Learning Exercise #1
Delving into Journal Articles
Objective: To help students better understand the process of
research and research methods
Directions: You will be assigned a scholarly article. After
reading the article, you’re requied to answer all the questions
below completely.
Based upon your reading of the article, you should address the
following questions in a 1 to a 1 and ½ page (max.) typed paper:
(please refer to APA Style guidelines)
1. What is the research question?
2. What theory did these authors use?
3. What were the authors’ hypotheses?
4. Was the research deductive or inductive?
5. How were the variables operationalized?
6. What kind of relationship exists between the variables?
(correlation, cause and effect, or spurious. Define these
definitions using the text and provide examples from your
article.)
7. What method did the researchers use? (Survey, field study,
experiment, existing sources, or triangulation, or another
method? Explain.)
8. Who composed the sample? Was it representative?
Grading criteria: Your ability to communicate your thoughts in
writing to include appropriate grammar, punctuation, spelling,
syntax, evidence of appropriate editing (10 points), your ability
to critique the article’s research question, methods, and overall
argument (15), and your ability to demonstrate correct use and
application of the concepts from the text to the research article
(15 points).40 total points possible**DUE DATE: MONDAY,
February 5th (11:59 PM)
Express Yourself Language Institute
Increased competitive edge • Improved self-esteem • Enhanced
thinking skills
Background
Many colleges and universities require foreign language study,
and U.S. students from every degree-granting educational
institution recognize the importance of fluency in a foreign
language. The Express Yourself Language Institute (EYLI) is
the premier destination for students wishing to get a head start
on their language studies. Drawing on student enrollment
patterns as well as trends in language skills sought by business
recruiters, the EYLI programs continually evolve to serve our
customers’ current and future needs.
Although we started in a small office with a reception area and
only two classrooms, we have grown exponentially over the
years, expanding from a single location on the south side of
Chicago to more than 20 locations in nine cities. Although
initially we offered only courses in Spanish, we now offer
courses in eight foreign languages as well as American Sign
Language.
Express Yourself Language Institute through the Years
Year
New Cities
New Languages
2007
Chicago
Spanish
2009
Tampa
French, German
2011
Albuquerque, Boston
Chinese, Japanese
Today
Kansas City, Detroit, Huntsville
Arabic, Korean, ASL
Current Language Study Statistics
According to the Modern Language Association, the language
with the largest percentage growth has been Arabic, growing by
46.3% and becoming the third most important language to learn
for business.
Other languages with significant increases to enrollment include
the following:
· Korean 19.1%
· Chinese 18.2%
· American Sign Language 16.4%
· Portuguese 10.8%
· Japanese 10.3%
The main driver for the increased enrollment in EYLI and other
language institutes is, of course, a heightened demand by
employers for bilingual and multilingual employees. The table
below lists the industries that are more likely to hire employees
with knowledge of a second language, and the percentage of
EYLI students who have expressed an interest in those
industries.
As we have in the past, EYLI will continue to follow these
trends and respond appropriately.
In response to the latest statistics, EYLI has decided to add the
following to its course offerings:
Beginning and Intermediate Arabic Fall 2015
Because of the increased interest in Arabic language studies, we
will offer two courses in Arabic. We anticipate that 30% of
students taking Begining Arabic will continue on this language
track and sign up for Intermediate Arabic.
Advanced Chinese Spring 2016
Many students studying Chinese at the Intermediate level at our
facilities across the country have requested an advanced level
Chinese course. We hope to hire native speakers to meet this
demand.
Additional upper-level Korean, Portuguese, and Japanese
courses are also in the works. We hope to release information
about these programs as soon as we have fully assessed the
demand.
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng
Shelly Cashman Word 2013| Chapter 3: SAM Project 1a
Shelly Cashman Word 2013
Chapter 3: SAM Project 1a
Express Yourself Language Institute
FORMAT A REPORT USING TABLES AND GRAPHICS
Project Goal
M Project Name
Project Goal
PROJECT DESCRIPTION
You are working with the media director for the Express
Yourself Language Institute to develop new marketing
materials. You will create a report that summarizes the state of
foreign language learning in the United States. To clarify the
presentation and add visual interest, you will format the report
using tables, graphics, and other visual aids.GETTING
STARTED
· Download the following file from the SAM website:
· SC_Word2013_C3_P1a_FirstLastName_1.docx
· Open the file you just downloaded and save it with the name:
· SC_Word2013_C3_P1a_FirstLastName_2.docx
· Hint: If you do not see the .docx file extension in the Save file
dialog box, do not type it. Word will add the file extension for
you automatically.
· To complete this Project, you will also need to download and
save the following support files from the SAM website:
· support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_language-logo.png
· support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_classroom.png
· With the file SC_Word2013_C3_P1a_FirstLastName_2.docx
still open, ensure that your first and last name is displayed in
the footer. If the footer does not display your name, delete the
file and download a new copy from the SAM website.
PROJECT STEPS
1. Change the document margins to Moderate.
2. Move the insertion point before the word “Express” in the
headline paragraph “Express Yourself Language Institute” and
insert the image file support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_language-
logo.png available for download from the SAM website.
3. Resize the image so that it is 0.5” tall and change the text
wrap format to Through.
4. Recolor the image by applying Gold, Accent color 2 Light
(3rd column, 3rd row of the Recolor section of the Color
gallery).
5. Change the font size of the headline paragraph “Express
Yourself Language Institute” to 28 pt. and center-align the text.
(Hint: The image should remain left-aligned.)
6. Apply the Grid Table 4 – Accent 1 style to the table “Express
Yourself Language Institute through the Years” and format the
table using AutoFit Window.
7. Merge the three cells in the first row of the table and center-
align the text in the new merged cell.
8. Insert a new row in the table immediately above the row
starting with "Today" and enter the data shown in Table 1
below.
Table 1: New Table Row
© 2014 Cengage Learning.
9. On page 2, move the insertion point to the blank line after the
paragraph “The main driver…interest in those industries.”
Insert a table with 2 columns and 5 rows and enter the data
shown in Table 2 below.
Table 2: New Table
© 2014 Cengage Learning.
10. Apply the Grid Table 5 Dark – Accent 1 style and format the
table using AutoFit Contents.
11. Center-align the text in column 2 and then center the entire
table on the page.
12. Insert the Rounded Rectangle shape in the blank line above
the paragraph “In response to….” Resize the shape to 0.3” high
and 6.75” wide.
13. Type the text Our Response into the shape you just inserted.
Left-align the text, apply bold formatting, and change the font
color to White, Background 1 (1st column, 1st row in the Theme
Colors palette).
14. Select the heading "Beginning and Intermediate Arabic Fall
2015" and add a left tab stop at 4". Move the insertion point
before the text "Fall 2015" and insert a tab so that the text is
aligned with the new tab stop.
15. Move the insertion point to the blank line after the
paragraph “Additional upper-level Korean…assessed the
demand.” and insert the image file
support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_classroom.png available for
download from the SAM website.
16. Resize the image so that it is 75% of its original size, and
then recolor the image by applying Gold, Accent color 2 Light
(3rd column, 3rd row of the Recolor section in the Color
gallery).
17. Check the Spelling & Grammar in the document to identify
and correct any spelling errors. (Hint: You should find and
correct at least 1 spelling error.)
Your document should look like the Final Figure on the
following pages. Save your changes, close the document and
exit Word. Follow the directions on the SAM website to submit
your completed project.
Final Figure
Microsoft product screenshots used with permission from
Microsoft Corporation. Logo © 2014 Cengage Learning.
Copyright © 2014 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved.
Picture source: Office.com
Copyright © 2014 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved.
2
Educational Researcher, Vol. 39, No. 1, pp. 59–68
DOI: 10.3102/0013189X09357621
© 2010 AERA. http://er.aera.net
january/February 2010 59
The gap in achievement across racial and ethnic groups has been
a
focus of education research for decades, but the
disproportionate
suspension and expulsion of Black, Latino, and American Indian
stu-
dents has received less attention. This article synthesizes
research on
racial and ethnic patterns in school sanctions and considers how
disproportionate discipline might contribute to lagging
achievement
among students of color. It further examines the evidence for
stu-
dent, school, and community contributors to the racial and
ethnic
patterns in school sanctions, and it offers promising directions
for
gap-reducing discipline policies and practices.
Keywords: achievement gap; at-risk students; classroom
management; school psychology; student behavior/
attitude; violence
A
lthough our national discourse on racial disparity tends
to focus on academic outcomes—the so-called achieve-
ment gap—in school districts throughout the United
States, Black, Latino, and American Indian students are also
sub-
ject to a differential and disproportionate rate of school
disciplin-
ary sanctions, ranging from office disciplinary referrals to
corporal
punishment, suspension, and expulsion (Krezmien, Leone, &
Achilles, 2006; Wallace, Goodkind, Wallace, & Bachman,
2008).
Ostensibly, the intent of school disciplinary interventions is to
preserve order and safety by removing students who break
school
rules and disrupt the school learning environment and, by
setting
an example of those punished students, to deter other students
from committing future rule infractions. However, schools tend
to rely heavily on exclusion from the classroom as the primary
discipline strategy (Arcia, 2006), and this practice often has a
dis-
proportionate impact on Black, Latino, and American Indian
students. The use of school exclusion as a discipline practice
may
contribute to the well-documented racial gaps in academic
achievement. This suggests that there is a pressing need for
schol-
arly attention to the racial discipline gap if efforts addressing
the
achievement gap are to have greater likelihood of success.
In this article, we synthesize the research on racial and eth-
nic patterns in school discipline, and we suggest how the racial
discipline gap influences racial patterns in achievement. We
then
review the evidence on the factors that contribute to the disci-
pline gap. Specifically, we examine the degree to which low-
income status, low achievement, and rates of misconduct
contribute to why Black, Latino, and American Indian students
are overselected and oversanctioned in the discipline system.
We
argue that such student characteristics are not adequate to
explain the large disparities, and we describe school and teacher
contributors that need to be investigated in future research.
Finally, we identify methodological challenges to the study of
disproportionality and discuss promising strategies for gap-
reducing interventions.
Safety Efforts and Racial Disproportionality
A large body of evidence shows that Black students are subject
to
a disproportionate amount of discipline in school settings, and a
smaller and less consistent literature suggests disproportionate
sanctioning of Latino and American Indian students in some
schools.1 This conclusion has been drawn across a wide array of
sanctions (e.g., suspensions, office discipline referrals) and
meth-
odology (see discussion below). The Children’s Defense Fund
(1975) first brought the issue of racial disproportionality to
national attention, showing that Black students were two to
three
times overrepresented in school suspensions compared with
their
enrollment rates in localities across the nation. National and
state
data show consistent patterns of Black disproportionality in
school discipline over the past 30 years, specifically in
suspension
(McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Raffaele Mendez, Knoff, & Ferron,
2002), expulsion (KewelRamani, Gilbertson, Fox, & Provasnik,
2007), and office discipline referrals (Skiba, Michael, Nardo, &
Peterson, 2002). According to a nationally representative study
utilizing parent reports, in 2003 Black students were
significantly
more likely to be suspended than White or Asian students (p <
.001). Specifically, almost 1 in 5 Black students (19.6%) were
suspended, compared with fewer than 1 in 10 White students
(8.8%) and Asian and Pacific Islanders (6.4%; KewelRamani et
al., 2007). A nationally representative survey of 74,000 10th
graders found that about 50% of Black students reported that
they had ever been suspended or expelled compared with about
20% of White students (Wallace et al., 2008). The study further
showed that, unlike the pattern for other racial and ethnic
groups,
suspensions and expulsions of Black students increased from
1991 to 2005 (Wallace et al., 2008).
The Achievement Gap and the Discipline Gap:
Two Sides of the Same Coin?
Anne Gregory, Russell J. Skiba, and Pedro A. Noguera
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
educational researcher60
Although disproportionality in school discipline has been
documented for Latino and American Indian students, findings
related to such disparities have been inconsistent. National data
(U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education
Statistics, 2003) show that, based on parent surveys
administered
in 1999, 20% of Latino students in Grades 7 through 12 had
ever
been suspended or expelled, which is a statistically significantly
lower rate (p < .001) than for Black students (35%) and a
statisti-
cally significantly higher rate (p < .001) than for White students
(15%). Analyzing racial disparities in discipline, Gordon, Della
Piana, and Keleher (2000) found that, in 3 of the 10 cities stud-
ied, the rates of suspended and expelled Latino students were
10% or more than 10% higher than the percentage of enrolled
Latino students. Inconsistency in findings was further
confirmed
in a study measuring disproportionality using odds ratios. Based
on state records from Maryland, Krezmien et al. (2006) found
that Latino students had similar or lower odds than White stu-
dents of being suspended for 9 successive years (1995–2003).
National and state data have also shown disproportionality in
discipline for American Indian students, although again there
appears to be some inconsistency (Wallace et al., 2008).
Krezmien
et al. (2006) showed that American Indian and White students
had a similar chance of being suspended from 1995 to 1998 in
Maryland. However, from 1998 to 2003, they found that
American Indians had significantly higher odds than Whites of
being suspended (odds ratios ranged from 1.5 to 1.8). The dis-
proportionality in American Indian suspension was again docu-
mented in nationally representative samples using school
records
(DeVoe & Darling-Churchill, 2008) and student reports
(Wallace et al., 2008). It is unclear whether the inconsistent
find-
ings on American Indian suspension is a statistical artifact
given
their relatively small numbers of suspended students (e.g.,
Krezmien et al., 2006) or if it reflects actual variability in
dispro-
portionate suspension rates across time and school districts.
Males of all racial and ethnic groups are more likely than
females to receive disciplinary sanctions. In 2004, only 1% of
Asian Pacific Islander females were suspended, compared with
11% of Asian Pacific Islander males (KewelRamani et al.,
2007).
Expulsion data from that same year showed that White females
were half as likely to be expelled as White males (p < .001), and
similarly, Black females were half as likely to be expelled as
Black
males (p < .05). Black males are especially at risk for receiving
discipline sanctions, with one study showing that Black males
were 16 times as likely as White females to be suspended (J. F.
Gregory, 1997).
Racial Disproportionality and Patterns in
Achievement
The consistent pattern of disproportionate discipline sanctions
issued to Black students and the trends in sanctions for Latino
and American Indian students, albeit less consistent, have rarely
been considered in light of the well-documented racial and
ethnic
disparities in school achievement (KewelRamani et al., 2007).
In
many schools, large proportions of a group (e.g., Black males)
receive at least one suspension, which typically results in
missed
instructional time and, for some, could exacerbate a cycle of
aca-
demic failure, disengagement, and escalating rule breaking
(Arcia, 2006). In fact, a suspended student may miss anywhere
from one class period to 10 or more school days, depending on
the violation and school policies. One of the most consistent
findings of modern education research is the strong positive
rela-
tionship between time engaged in academic learning and student
achievement (Brophy, 1988; Fisher et al., 1981; Greenwood,
Horton, & Utley, 2002). The school disciplinary practices used
most widely throughout the United States may be contributing
to lowered academic performance among the group of students
in greatest need of improvement.
Research shows that frequent suspensions appear to signifi-
cantly increase the risk of academic underperformance (Davis &
Jordan, 1994). Arcia (2006) followed two demographically
simi-
lar cohorts (matched on gender, race, grade level, family
poverty,
and limited English proficiency), contrasting a cohort that had
received at least one suspension with another that had received
no suspensions. In Year 1, suspended students were three grade
levels behind their nonsuspended peers in their reading skills,
but
were almost 5 years behind 2 years later. Although other
unmea-
sured risk factors may have contributed to cohort differences,
suspension may have initiated or maintained a process of with-
drawal from learning in the classroom. In the long term, school
suspension has been found to be a moderate to strong predictor
of dropout and not graduating on time (Ekstrom, Goertz,
Pollack, & Rock, 1986; Raffaele Mendez, 2003; Wehlage &
Rutter, 1986).
Discipline sanctions resulting in exclusion from school may
damage the learning process in other ways as well. Suspended
students may become less bonded to school, less invested in
school rules and course work, and subsequently, less motivated
to
achieve academic success. Students who are less bonded to
school
may be more likely to turn to lawbreaking activities and become
less likely to experience academic success. Consistent findings
highlight the importance of school bonding for reducing the risk
of delinquency (Hawkins, Smith, & Catalano, 2004).
Conversely,
Hemphill, Toumbourou, Herrenkohl, McMorris, and Catalano
(2006) found that taking into account previous violent and
aggressive behavior and a multitude of other risk factors (e.g.,
negative peer group, low grades), school suspension actually
increased the risk of antisocial behavior a year later. In sum,
dis-
proportionate school discipline experienced by some racial and
ethnic groups has important implications for academic out-
comes. There is a need for research to identify why racial
dispro-
portionality in discipline occurs and what types of disciplinary
practices might be less likely to exacerbate academic outcomes.
Explanations for the Racial Discipline Gap
Certain demographic characteristics that are more common
among some racial and ethnic groups have been used as a
primary
explanation for the racial discipline gap (see, e.g., National
Association of Secondary School Principals, 2000). Low-income
students with histories of low achievement, who reside in high-
crime/high-poverty neighborhoods, may be at greater risk for
engaging in behavior resulting in office disciplinary referrals
and
school suspension. A review of the literature suggests that such
characteristics likely account for some proportion of the gap in
sanctions across groups. Yet there is no evidence to suggest
demo-
graphic factors are in any way sufficient to “explain away” the
gap. Teacher and school factors need to be considered as
possible
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
january/February 2010 61
contributors to the overselection and oversanction of Black,
Latino, and American Indian students.
Poverty and Neighborhood Characteristics
Race, socioeconomic status (SES), and characteristics of neigh-
borhoods associated with risk of negative outcomes are
frequently
connected in the United States (Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, &
Klebanov, 1994; McLoyd, 1998). The confluence of these
factors
makes it challenging to separate out the contributions of each to
the racial discipline gap. Many low-income students living in
urban neighborhoods may experience adversity, such as
exposure
to violence and substance abuse, which may increase the likeli-
hood of their receiving school sanctions (Brantlinger, 1991;
Bureau of Justice Statistics, 2005). Although there is no
evidence
that exposure to violence causes behavior difficulties, correla-
tional studies show links between exposure to violence and stu-
dent mental health and behavior in the classroom (e.g., Kuther
& Fisher, 1998). Many violence-exposed children suffer from
anxiety, irritability, stress, and hypervigilence (Gorman-Smith
&
Tolan, 1998). These conditions may have a negative effect upon
behavior in classrooms and result in increased discipline
referrals.
Exposure to violence may also influence how students cope in
school. One coping mechanism to ward off the threat of
violence
includes presenting a “tough front” or even arming oneself to
ward off future victimization (Anderson, 1999; Stewart,
Schreck,
& Simons, 2006). The need to negotiate what Anderson has
called the “code of the street” may contribute to behavior prob-
lems in school as students from high-crime neighborhoods
adjust
to a different set of norms in their interactions with peers and
teachers in school settings (Dance, 2002). Additional research is
needed to tease apart community effects (e.g., concentrated pov-
erty, neighborhood crime, and the stress of low SES) and their
impact on student behavior in school.
It is important to distinguish, however, between the role of
poverty in predicting disruptive behavior and the ways it may
contribute to racial and ethnic disparities in discipline. Existing
school discipline research suggests that student SES is limited
in
its explanatory power of the racial discipline gap (McCarthy &
Hoge, 1987; Wallace et al., 2008). Whether statistically control-
ling for a measure of SES at the school level (percentage of par-
ents unemployed or percentage of students enrolled in free or
reduced-cost meals; Raffaele Mendez et al., 2002; Wu, Pink,
Crain, & Moles, 1982) or at the student level (parental
education
or qualification for free or reduced-cost meals; McCarthy &
Hoge, 1987; Skiba et al., 2002), multivariate analyses have
repeatedly demonstrated that racial differences in discipline
rates
remain significant. The most recent of these analyses (Wallace
et al., 2008) used a series of logistic regressions to test
racial/ethnic
disparities in office disciplinary referrals, suspension, and
expul-
sion. Race/ethnicity remained a significant predictor of all three
disciplinary outcomes even after accounting for student-
reported
parental education, family structure (e.g., single-parent house-
hold), and urbanicity of neighborhood. In sum, being enrolled in
a school with high rates of low-income students (Raffaele
Mendez
et al., 2002; Wu et al., 1982) or being from a low-income family
(McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Skiba et al., 2002) does increase the
likelihood that a student will be subject to punitive forms of
discipline and even appears to make a mild contribution to
disproportionality (Wallace et al., 2008). Yet the highly consis-
tent finding that race/ethnicity remains a significant predictor of
discipline even after statistically controlling for measures of
fam-
ily income suggests that student SES is not sufficient to explain
the racial discipline gap.
In fact, some research has found an inverse relationship
between student demographics and rates of disproportionality in
school discipline. Rausch and Skiba (2004), examining suspen-
sion and expulsion records across one Midwestern state,
reported
that Black students are at greater risk of suspension when com-
pared with White students, not in urban schools but, rather, in
more resource-rich suburban schools. Other research suggests
that the context of school or district racial climate may have an
influence on rates of disproportionality. Thornton and Trent
(1988) reported that racial disproportionality in school suspen-
sion was greatest in schools that had been recently
desegregated,
especially if those schools had a higher SES student population.
Conversely, Eitle and Eitle (2004) found decreased rates of dis-
proportionality in school suspension in schools that became
resegregated. Such data suggest that, at the school and district
levels, financial resources, staff perceptions, and racial climate
may be as important as student demographics in predicting
racial
disparity.
Low Achievement
Low achievement is another variable that may contribute to the
racial discipline gap. A wide body of research documents a
persis-
tent pattern that Asian and White students score higher on
achievement tests compared with Black, Latino, and American
Indian students (A. Gregory & Weinstein, 2004; U.S.
Department of Education, National Center for Education
Statistics, 2003). Faced with repeated academic struggles,
under-
performing students may become frustrated and disaffected and
have lower self-confidence, all of which may contribute to a
higher rate of school disruption (Miles & Stipek, 2006). Low
literacy achievement in the elementary grades is linked to later
aggression in third and fifth grades (Miles & Stipek, 2006).
Similar patterns have been found in later grades—low achieve-
ment in middle and high school is linked with more serious
forms of aggression a year later (Choi, 2007). Although it is
clear
that low achievement is highly correlated with aggressive
behav-
ior and disciplinary infractions, such patterns in and of them-
selves do not explain disproportionality in discipline. Studies of
the relationship between achievement and student discipline
have shown that when taking into account grade point average,
race remains a predictor of suspension (Wehlage & Rutter,
1986).
Moreover, it is also possible that any relationship between the
achievement gap and the discipline gap is in fact the product of
other variables, such as educational disadvantage. Ladson-
Billings
(2006) argues that what is widely viewed as an achievement gap
between White and Black students could more properly be
termed an “education debt” in that educational opportunities in
the Unites States have historically never been equalized for
differ-
ent groups. McLloyd (1998) notes that poverty’s effects on stu-
dents are mediated not simply by family or community risk
factors but also by poor school conditions in disadvantaged
neighborhoods. Poor students of color are more likely to attend
schools with lower quality resources and facilities (Kozol,
2005),
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
educational researcher62
higher teacher turnover, and a lower percentage of highly quali-
fied teachers (Darling-Hammond, 2004). Discrepancies in the
quality of resources available to rich and poor districts are well
documented, but there is a need for sound policy research that
can specify how to address resource disparities in order to posi-
tively affect both the achievement gap and the discipline gap.
Differential Behavior
Another explanation for the racial discipline gap is that students
from certain racial and ethnic groups misbehave or contribute to
a lack of safety in schools more than students from other racial
and ethnic groups. Studies using both measures of student self-
report and extant school disciplinary records have examined this
premise and have generally failed to find evidence of racial
differ-
ences in student behavior (e.g., Skiba et al., 2002; Wehlage &
Rutter, 1986). In one of the earliest longitudinal studies of stu-
dent race and school sanctions for misbehavior, Wehlage and
Rutter examined predictors of school sanctions for 7th, 9th, and
11th graders over a 3-year period and reported that Black stu-
dents did not consistently report more misbehavior than White
students. This failure to find consistently large racial and ethnic
differences in student self-reported behavior has been corrobo-
rated in the literature (McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Wu et al.,
1982). A recent study using a nationally representative sample
showed few and generally small differences in self-reported
unsafe
behavior across racial groups compared with the racial discrep-
ancy in discipline sanctions (Dinkes, Cataldi, & Lin-Kelly,
2007). There were, for example, no differences in self-reported
weapon carrying among Black, White, and American Indian stu-
dents. Some of the most recent data on school safety (Bauer,
Guerino, Nolle, & Tang, 2008) show that victimization by vio-
lence or theft is not statistically differentiated by race, with
simi-
lar percentages of White (4.7%), Black (3.8%), and Latino
(3.9%) students reporting that they had been victimized in the
past 6 months in school.
The use of self-report data, however, can raise questions about
the accuracy of the student reporters and hence the validity of
the
results. Hindelang, Hirschi, and Weis (1979) hypothesized that
the failure to find differences between Black and White self-
report of serious delinquent behavior could be due to underre-
porting by Black youth. Studies examining this hypothesis,
however, have failed to find support for it. McCarthy and Hoge
(1987) examined whether Black students, more than White stu-
dents, underreported their rule-breaking behavior. Comparing
student self-report with a sample of teacher reports of rule
break-
ing from a sample of 1,125 7th and 11th graders, the researchers
found no clear pattern that teacher reports were more highly
cor-
related with either White or Black self-reports of misconduct,
and they concluded that neither group tended to systematically
under- or over-report their misconduct.
The findings of self-report data have also been corroborated
by studies using extant school data on office referrals, which
have
also failed to find substantial differences in rates of disruptive
school behavior by race. McFadden, Marsh, Price, and Hwang
(1992), studying discipline records in a single Florida school
dis-
trict, found no general differences in behavior between White
and
Black students and indeed found that White students engaged in
a higher level of those behaviors (e.g., defiance, fighting, and
bothering others) that tended to result in suspension or corporal
punishment. Similarly, Shaw and Braden (1990) reported that
White children in a single school district were significantly
more
likely than Black children to be referred for disciplinary action
for
severe rule violations, despite the overrepresentation of Black
stu-
dents in that district in corporal punishment. Finally, Skiba et
al.
(2002) set out specifically to test the differential behavior
hypoth-
esis, using disciplinary referrals from all 19 middle schools in a
single large urban district. They found no evidence that either
Black or White students were referred to the office for more
seri-
ous behaviors. The analyses did show, however, that reasons for
referring White students tended to be for causes that were more
objectively observable (smoking, vandalism, leaving without
per-
mission, obscene language), whereas office referrals for Black
stu-
dents were more likely to occur in response to behaviors
(loitering,
disrespect, threat, excessive noise) that appear to be more
subjec-
tive in nature. In short, there appears to be a notable paucity of
evidence that could support a hypothesis that the racial
discipline
gap can be explained through differential rates of misbehavior.
Differential Selection
In juvenile justice research, there has been a similar focus on
exploring disproportionate minority contact in the justice
system
(Piquero, 2008). Some of this research has sought to identify
whether the high incarceration rates of ethnic minority youth,
compared with the rates of White youth, are due to their higher
rates of illegal behavior or due to institutional practices such as
patterns in police surveillance, racial profiling, or biased
sentenc-
ing (Piquero, 2008). This research provides a useful framework
for understanding discrimination as a contributor to the racial
discipline gap in schools. Specifically, the “differential
selection”
hypothesis asserts that ethnic minorities are more likely to be
arrested because they are more likely to be picked out for
wrong-
doing despite similar levels of infractions (Piquero, 2008). This
hypothesis is useful when applied to the school setting; that is,
despite relatively similar rates of disruption, Black, Latino, or
American Indian students may be more likely to be
differentially
selected for discipline consequences.
There is a fairly substantial research base suggesting that dif-
ferential selection at the classroom level contributes in some
way
to racial/ethnic disproportionality in school disciplinary out-
comes. Consistent findings of disproportionality in office refer-
rals (Skiba et al., 2002; Skiba et al., 2008; Wallace et al., 2008)
suggest that racial/ethnic disparities in discipline begin at the
classroom level. In an ethnographic observational study of
urban
classrooms, Vavrus and Cole (2002) found that many office
refer-
rals leading to school suspension were due to what the authors
described as a student’s “violation of implicit interactional
codes,”
most often a student calling into question established classroom
practices or the teacher’s authority. Those students singled out
in
this way were disproportionately students of color. Skiba et al.
(2002) reported on findings of referrals based on objective
versus
subjective reasons by race. Together with findings that Black
stu-
dents are more likely than White students to be referred to the
office for defiance (A. Gregory & Weinstein, 2008) or noncom-
pliance (Skiba et al., 2008), these results strongly suggest that
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
january/February 2010 63
some process of differential selection at the classroom level
may
contribute to disparities in discipline.
Explanations for the overselection of certain students for dis-
cipline may include cultural mismatch, implicit bias, or
negative
expectations in classrooms and schools. The cultural mismatch
hypothesis suggests that the classroom culture or the teacher’s
culture is at odds with the culture of ethnic minority students
(Irvine, 2002; Townsend, 2000). For instance, Boykin and col-
leagues argued that Western European–based individualism and
competitiveness are the dominant underlying ideologies guiding
classroom activities (Boykin, Tyler, & Miller, 2005)—an
orienta-
tion that may clash with a stronger emphasis on communal val-
ues in Black, Latino, and American Indian culture (Gay, 2006).
Gay further suggested that communicative tensions can arise
through cultural difference. Specifically, differences in ways of
communicating between Blacks (e.g., animated, interpersonal)
and Whites (e.g., dispassionate, impersonal) may lead to
conflict
(Kochman, 1981). In a study of 62 White elementary teachers
who taught in two predominantly Black schools, Tyler, Boykin,
and Walton (2006) found that teachers were more likely to rate
vignettes of students who exhibited competitive and individual-
istic behavior as motivated and achievement oriented than stu-
dents who exhibited more communal and vervistic (e.g.,
collaborative and multitasking) behaviors. Such findings, if
vali-
dated in actual classroom settings, would indicate a differential
perception on the part of teachers that could well advantage
White students exhibiting competitive behaviors and disadvan-
tage Black students exhibiting a more active and community-
oriented learning style.
Other scholars have focused on ways in which negative teacher
beliefs and expectations can contribute to racially related
author-
ity conflicts (R. S. Weinstein, 2002; R. S. Weinstein, Gregory,
&
Strambler, 2004). In her ethnography of school discipline in an
elementary school, Ferguson (2000) observed patterns in nega-
tive teacher–student interactions and argued that these events
were fueled by White teachers’ overreacting and relying on
stereo-
types to interpret Black students’ language and physical expres-
sion. Given stereotypes and media portrayals of Black youth as
dangerous and aggressive (Devine & Elliot, 2000; Noguera &
Akom, 2000), teacher expectations for behavior may also influ-
ence whether these students are selected for discipline
sanctions.
A related area of research examines how implicit beliefs may
neg-
atively affect Black and Latino students. Implicit racial bias,
according to social psychologists, operates out of conscious
awareness yet influences decision making (e.g., Dovidio, Glick,
& Rudman, 2005). Although no studies have been conducted on
the implicit bias of teachers and how race may activate stereo-
types, Graham and Lowery (2004) conducted an analogous
experimental study with police and probation officers. They
found that, compared with officers who were subliminally
primed with neutral, non-race-related words, officers who had
been subliminally primed with words related to the category
Black were more likely to recommend harsher punishments for
adolescents who had committed crimes, as presented in
standard-
ized, written vignettes.
Taken together, research on classroom processes suggests that
Black students are differentially selected for discipline referral
(e.g., Skiba et al., 2002), although there is insufficient data to
establish why this may occur. Several reasons may include
societal
stereotypes, implicit bias, or cultural mismatch between
teachers
and Black students. To advance research in this area, a
systematic
line of mixed-methods research is needed, using observational
studies of classroom interactions and interviews of teachers and
students concerning the process of school discipline. Coding of
teacher–student interactions could help identify whether teach-
ers are more or less tolerant of racially specific deviations from
implicit behavioral standards in the classroom.
Differential Processing
The differential processing hypothesis asserts that
discrimination
occurs in the courts and correctional systems, which leads to a
disproportionate arrest and incarceration rate of minorities
(Piquero, 2008). Subjective judgments in sanctioning may be
det-
rimental to Black, Latino, and American Indian youth. Morrison
(Morrison et al., 2001; Morrison & Skiba, 2001) noted that the
application of school consequences such as suspension and
expul-
sion represents less a discrete event than a complex process
whose
outcome is influenced simultaneously by student behavior,
teacher classroom management, administrator perspectives, and
school policy. There is tremendous local flexibility in the types
of
infractions that move forward from the classroom to the office
and in the types of consequences issued by administrators. The
Gun-Free Schools Act of 1994 mandates a 1-year expulsion for
the possession of firearms at school, but such consequences can
be
modified based on the discretion of the district administration.
Thus, in general, there is considerable flexibility in the type and
length of sanction students receive for an infraction. For the
same
offense, one administrator may decide to mandate a conference
with parents or guardians; a different administrator may
mandate
a 5-day suspension (Noguera & Yonemura Wing, 2006).
The most well-documented gap in sanctions is between Black
and White students. Wehlage and Rutter (1986) found that
Black students were more likely than White students to report
being sent to the principal’s office and were more likely than
White students to report being suspended even though they did
not report higher incidents of misbehavior, across 2 years of
study. These findings suggest a discrepancy between sanctions
and student-reported behavior. Indeed, it may be that Black stu-
dents are suspended and punished for behavior that is less
serious
than the behavior of other students. McFadden, Marsh, Price,
and Hwang (1992) reported that Black pupils in a Florida school
district were more likely than White students to receive severe
punishments (e.g., corporal punishment, school suspension) and
less likely to receive milder consequences (e.g., in-school
suspen-
sion). These results are consistent with findings that Black stu-
dents were referred for corporal punishment for less serious
behavior than were other students (Shaw & Braden, 1990).
These findings, as a whole, suggest harsh sanctions issued to
Black students may contribute to their overrepresentation in dis-
cipline data.
Methodological Issues and Recommendations
Although the concept of disproportionate representation seems
straightforward, its measurement can be complex, as demon-
strated in special education research (Skiba et al., 2008). The
com-
position index (Donovan & Cross, 2002) compares the
proportion
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
educational researcher64
of those served in special education represented by a given
ethnic
group with the proportion that group represents in the popula-
tion or in school enrollment. For example, Black students
account
for 33% of students identified as mentally retarded at the
national
level, clearly discrepant from their 17% representation in the
school-aged population (Donovan & Cross, 2002). Although an
intuitive measure, problems with interpretation and scaling of
the composition index measure have led the field toward use of
the risk index and risk ratio (Coutinho & Oswald, 2000; Skiba
et
al., 2008; Westat, 2005). The risk index is the proportion of a
given group in a given category; at the national level, 2.64% of
all Black students enrolled in the public schools are identified
as
mentally retarded (Donovan & Cross, 2002). To interpret the
risk index, a ratio of the risk of the target group to one or more
groups may be constructed, termed a risk ratio (Hosp &
Reschly,
2003; Parrish, 2002). Comparison of Black student risk for
iden-
tification as mentally retarded (2.64%) with the White risk
index
of 1.18% for that category yields a risk ratio of 2.24
(2.64/1.18),
suggesting that Black students are over two times more likely to
be served in the category mental retardation than White
students.
The same data can also be used to compute an odds ratio (Finn,
1982), often drawn from logistic regression (Wallace et al.,
2008).
In contrast to the risk ratio, the odds ratios assesses both occur-
rence and nonoccurrence data.
Methodological issues in the measurement of disproportion-
ality remain outstanding, including criteria for determining a
significant level of disproportionality (Bollmer, Bethel,
Garrison-
Mogren, & Brauen, 2007; Skiba et al., 2008), the appropriate
comparison group when calculating risk ratios (Westat, 2004),
and the comparability of risk and odds ratios (Davies, Crombie,
& Tavakoli, 1998). In the face of national special education law
mandating the identification of significant disproportionality at
the local level, however, criteria for making that determination
are necessary. Thus, the U.S. Department of Education Office of
Special Education Programs issued policy guidance to state and
local education agencies regarding the calculation and
interpreta-
tion of risk indices and risk ratios (Westat, 2004, 2005), which
has
implications for how disproportionality in discipline sanctions
could be identified. The Office of Special Education Programs
recommends that a risk ratio can be used to understand the rela-
tive risk of students receiving special education services for
differ-
ent racial and ethnic groups (Westat, 2005). The office cautions,
however, that risk ratios are difficult to interpret when based on
small numbers of students in a racial and ethnic group. It
further
describes the benefits of a weighted risk ratio, which takes into
account differences in the size of racial and ethnic groups. This
allows for comparison of risk ratios across districts with
varying
racial and ethnic composition.
Improved measurement of the racial discipline gap should
advance substantive areas of inquiry. One important area relates
to the unique contributions of student, teacher, school, and fam-
ily and neighborhood to the racial discipline gap. As of yet,
there
have been no comprehensive studies or systematic lines of
research that have disentangled the unique effects of these con-
tributors. Education researchers might follow the lead of a
recent
study by Sampson, Morenoff, and Raudenbush (2005) on the
gap in community violence between White, Black, and Latino
young adults, which offers a guide for ecologically sensitive
research on race and discipline. Using data from almost 3,000
young adults in 180 Chicago neighborhoods, Sampson and col-
leagues identified the unique contributions of individual, home,
and neighborhood variables to the relative odds of self-reported
violence for each racial and ethnic group. The apparent multi-
level causation of disciplinary disproportionality strongly sug-
gests that multivariate procedures, in particular hierarchical
approaches (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), will be most appropri-
ate in future research. The next generation of research could
simultaneously consider the effects of student attitude and
behav-
ior, teacher tolerance and classroom management skills,
adminis-
trative leadership, school climate, and school and community
demographics on the racial discipline gap.
Following the lead from research on the juvenile justice system
(Piquero, 2008), systematic lines of research on the chain of
events that culminate in suspension and expulsion are needed.
Unfair selection and sanction at various points in the discipline
process could additively contribute to the discipline gap.
Another
crucial area of research needs to test mechanisms and develop
theory regarding the conscious and unconscious processes that
result in differential treatment of some racial and ethnic groups.
Previous research has shown that cultural mismatch between
teachers and students can contribute to misunderstandings, fear,
and conflict with respect to pedagogy (Irvine, 2002; Ladson-
Billings 1995; Pollack, 2008); further research is needed on the
extent to which such processes also contribute to inequitable
dis-
ciplinary practices. Social class, immigrant status, racial and
eth-
nic identity, neighborhood and familial diversity, and educator
training and perspectives may all affect student behavior,
teacher
responses, or their interaction. Clearly, conducting research that
could truly sort out the numerous and interacting sources of
vari-
ance contributing to disciplinary disproportionality is challeng-
ing. Subtle and implicit processes related to racial bias,
negative
expectations, or stereotypes are not easily detected outside of
con-
trolled laboratory conditions, and it is not a simple matter to
observe the complex and interactive social processes that can
con-
tribute to an escalating sequence of actions and reactions during
actual discipline encounters.
Identifying the characteristics of resilient schools is another
important next step in research on racial and ethnic disparities
in
school discipline. In the field of public health, research has
estab-
lished a strong link between community violence and manifesta-
tions of school violence (Ozer, 2005). Not surprisingly, schools
in areas with a high incidence of crime and violence also tend to
experience higher rates of violence and disorder (Noguera,
2003).
Yet the presence of schools that demonstrate positive outcomes
despite their location in high-risk neighborhoods (e.g., Welsh,
Greene, & Jenkins, 1999) strongly suggests that neighborhood
and family disadvantage be approached in research and practice
as conditions that increase educational challenge, rather than as
limiting conditions. In particular, there is a need for additional
research on the types of strategies schools can implement to
reduce the effects of violence in neighboring communities.
Disciplinary Practices, Prevention Programming, and
School Reform
Existing research on the racial discipline gap suggests that,
similar to
efforts that address the achievement gap or the disproportionate
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
january/February 2010 65
number of Black students placed in special education (Skiba et
al.,
2008), no single causal factor can fully explain racially
disparate
discipline, and no single action will therefore be sufficient to
ameliorate it. Multifaceted strategies may offer promise, but
there
is as yet no empirical research testing specific interventions for
reducing the discipline gap.
Given the lack of systematic research addressing the effective-
ness of gap-reducing interventions, promising directions must
be
extrapolated from other intervention research. Freiberg and
Lapointe (2006) reviewed 40 school-based programs targeting
the reduction of behavior problems in schools. Of those, 29
were
implemented with Black, Latino, urban, and low-income stu-
dents and offered some evidence for their success in increasing
student problem solving and/or reducing difficulties in
classroom
management for participants as a whole. Freiberg and Lapointe
identified commonalities among those effective programs. The
programs move beyond discipline, emphasizing student learning
and self-regulation, not simply procedures for addressing rule
infractions. They encourage “school connectedness” and “caring
and trusting relationships” between teachers and students.
Overall, the programs try to increase students’ positive
experience
of schooling and to move away from a reliance on punitive reac-
tions to misbehavior.
The programmatic commonalities described by Freiberg and
Lapointe (2006) offer a promising direction for lowering the
oversanctioning of Black, Latino, and American Indian students.
Yet universal approaches to educational practice have
frequently
been critiqued for not specifically addressing the racial
dynamics,
economic stressors, or other influences on the racial discipline
gap (Goldstein & Noguera, 2006). In a national sample of
schools at the elementary and middle school level that imple-
mented positive behavior supports for at least a year, Skiba et
al.
(2008) reported generally positive findings before
disaggregation
by race but significant disciplinary disproportionality for Black
and Latino students in both office disciplinary referrals and
administrative consequences when the data were disaggregated.
Explicit attention to issues of race and culture may be necessary
for sustained change in racial and ethnic disciplinary
disparities.
Studies of successful teachers of Black students support the
idea that teachers differ from one another in their ability to
elicit
cooperation and diffuse conflict. A. Gregory and Weinstein
(2008) found that teachers who elicited trust and cooperation
with their Black students tended to use an authoritative style of
teaching—one in which teachers showed both caring and high
expectations. These “warm demanders” (Irvine, 2002) may pro-
vide cultural synchronization between authority in the home and
in the school. Teachers’ use of humor, emotions, and colloquial
expressions are other avenues through which cultural synchrony
may occur (Monroe & Obidah, 2004; C. S. Weinstein,
Tomlinson-Clarke, & Curran, 2004). Additional research on
preservice teacher training and professional development is
needed to ascertain if an increase in teacher cultural responsive-
ness or synchrony with students is linked to lower discipline
referrals for Black, Latino, and American Indian students.
Overall, little is known about the types of interventions that
reduce the racial discipline gap. Given the research on possible
con-
tributors to the gap, a variety of strategies may be needed,
includ-
ing (a) increasing the awareness of teachers and administrators
of
the potential for bias when issuing referrals for discipline, (b)
utilizing a range of consequences in response to behavior prob-
lems, (c) treating exclusion as a last resort rather than the first
or
only option, (d) making a concerted effort to understand the
roots of behavior problems, and (e) finding ways to reconnect
students to the educational mission of schools during
disciplinary
events (Noguera, 2007).
Summary
The racial and ethnic disparity in discipline sanctions has not
received the attention it deserves. Few studies have examined
where and why disproportionality between Black and White stu-
dents is on the increase, especially for Black females (Wallace
et al., 2008). Discipline trends for Latinos have been inconsis-
tently documented. Given the diversity of Latinos in the United
States (e.g., immigrant status, country of origin), in-depth
exam-
inations of different Latino groups is needed (e.g., first-genera-
tion Mexican American, third-generation Cuban American).
Moreover, comparisons of schools with racial diversity versus
racial homogeneity would be informative. Such research would
then lend itself to inquiry about why such trends exist in school
discipline.
Unfortunately, the discourse on racial and ethnic dispropor-
tionality seems to be constrained by simplistic dichotomies that
artificially pit individual student characteristics (e.g., student
aggression, disengagement from school) against systemic
factors
(e.g., school administrators’ implicit bias, community violence)
as the reason why some groups are overrepresented in
suspension
or expulsion (Skiba et al., 2008). The multiple and interacting
variables that appear to contribute to racial and ethnic
disparities
in discipline demand a more comprehensive and nuanced
approach. More sophisticated statistical methodologies such as
hierarchical linear modeling or sequential analysis (Gottman &
Roy, 1990) may prove to be better suited for modeling the com-
plexity of inequitable outcomes in school discipline.
At this time, however, little is known about the efficacy or
effectiveness of possible “gap-reducing” interventions. What
types
of interventions might successfully increase teacher and
adminis-
trator awareness of the potential for bias when issuing referrals
for
discipline? Do interventions aimed at using exclusion as a last
resort rather than the first or only option reduce the gap in
refer-
rals across racial and ethnic groups? Will interventions aimed at
reducing the achievement gap, such as access to rigorous
curricu-
lum and caring teacher–student relationships, be accompanied
by
a narrowed discipline gap? Can gap-reducing interventions draw
on universal approaches, or do they need targeted, culturally
spe-
cific approaches that respond to the students’ cultural and
socio-
economic contexts? Effectively addressing these questions
poses a
serious challenge to researchers, as it necessarily involves
attention
to the complex, politically charged, and often personally
threaten-
ing topic of race. Yet creativity and perseverance will be
necessary
to craft such research if we are to understand and develop inter-
ventions that can effectively reduce the racial discipline gap.
NoTE
1Rarely does research differentiate between expulsion resulting
in
alternative educational services or exclusion from such services.
As a
result, this review must rely on a broad usage of the term
expulsion.
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
educational researcher66
REfERENCES
Anderson, E. (1999). Code of the street: Decency, violence, and
the moral
life of the inner city. New York: W. W. Norton.
Arcia, E. (2006). Achievement and enrollment status of
suspended stu-
dents: Outcomes in a large, multicultural school district.
Education
and Urban Society, 38, 359–369.
Bauer, L., Guerino, P., Nolle, K. L., & Tang, S. (2008). Student
victim-
ization in U.S. schools: Results from the 2005 school crime
supplement to
the National Crime Victimization Survey (NCES 2009–306).
Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.
Institute
of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
Bollmer, J., Bethel, J., Garrison-Mogren, R., & Brauen, M.
(2007).
Using the risk ratio to assess racial/ethnic disproportionality in
special
education at the school-district level. Journal of Special
Education, 41,
186–198.
Boykin, A. W., Tyler, K. M., & Miller, O. (2005). In search of
cultural
themes and their expressions in the dynamics of classroom life.
Urban
Education, 40, 521–549.
Brantlinger, E. (1991). Social class distinctions in adolescents’
reports of
problems and punishment in school. Behavioral Disorders, 17,
36–46.
Brophy, J. (1988). Classroom management as socializing
students into
clearly articulated roles. Journal of Classroom Interaction,
33(1), 1–4.
Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2005). Crime victimization, 2005.
Retrieved
April 20, 2009, from http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/pub/pdf/cv05
.pdf
Children’s Defense Fund. (1975). School suspensions: Are they
helping
children? Cambridge, MA: Washington Research Project.
Choi, Y. (2007). Academic achievement and problem behaviors
among
Asian Pacific Islander American adolescents. Journal of Youth
and
Adolescence, 36, 403–415.
Coutinho, M. J., & Oswald, D. P. (2000). Disproportionate
representa-
tion in special education: A synthesis and recommendations.
Journal
of Child and Family Studies, 9, 135–156.
Dance, L. (2002). Tough fronts: The impact of street culture on
schooling.
London: Routledge.
Darling-Hammond, L. (2004). Inequality and the right to learn:
Access
to qualified teachers in California’s public schools. Teachers
College
Record, 106, 1936–1966.
Davies, H., Crombie, I., & Tavakoli, M. (1998). When can odds
ratios
mislead? British Medical Journal, 316, 989–991.
Davis, J. E., & Jordan, W. J. (1994). The effects of school
context, struc-
ture, and experiences on African American males in middle and
high
schools. Journal of Negro Education, 63, 570–587.
Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Are racial stereotypes
really fading?
The Princeton trilogy revisited. In C. Stangor (Eds.),
Stereotypes and
prejudice: Essential readings (pp. 86–99). New York:
Psychology Press.
DeVoe, J. F., & Darling-Churchill, K. E. (2008). Status and
trends in the
education of American Indians and Alaska Natives: 2008 (NCES
2008–
084). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics,
Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
Dinkes, R., Cataldi, E. F., & Lin-Kelly, W. (2007). Indicators of
school
crime and safety: 2007 (NCES 2008–021/NCJ 219553).
Washington,
DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of
Education
Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, and Bureau of Justice.
Donovan, M. S., & Cross, C. T. (Eds.). (2002). Minority
students in
special and gifted education. Washington, DC: National
Academies
Press.
Dovidio, J. F., Glick, P. G., & Rudman, L. (2005). On the
nature of
prejudice: Fifty years after Allport. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994).
Economic
deprivation and early childhood development. Child
Development,
65, 296–318.
Eitle, T. M., & Eitle, D. J. (2004). Inequality, segregation, and
the over-
representation of African Americans in school suspensions.
Sociological
Perspectives, 47, 269–287.
Ekstrom, R. B., Goertz, M. E., Pollack, J. M., & Rock, D. A.
(1986).
Who drops out of high school and why? Findings from a
national
study. Teachers College Record, 87, 356–373.
Ferguson, A. A. (2000). Bad boys: Public school and the making
of Black
masculinity. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Finn, J. D. (1982). Patterns in special education placement as
revealed
by the OCR survey. In K. A. Heller, W. H. Holtzman, & S.
Messick
(Eds.), Placing children in special education: A strategy for
equity
(pp. 322–381). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Fisher, C. W., Berliner, D. C., Filby, N. N., Marliave, R.,
Cahen, L. S.,
& Dishaw, M. M. (1981). Teaching behaviors, academic
learning
time, and student achievement: An overview. Journal of
Classroom
Interaction, 17(1), 2–15.
Freiberg, H. J., & Lapointe, J. M. (2006). Research-based
programs for
preventing and solving discipline problems. In C. M. Evertson
&
C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of classroom management
(pp. 735–
786). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gay, G. (2006). Connections between classroom management
and cul-
turally responsive teaching. In C. M. Evertson & C. S.
Weinstein
(Eds.), Handbook of classroom management (pp. 343–372).
Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Goldstein, M., & Noguera, P. (2006, Spring). Designing for
diversity:
How educators can use cultural competence in developing
substance
abuse prevention programs for urban youth. New Directions for
Youth
Development, pp. 29–40.
Gordon, R., Della Piana, L., & Keleher, T. (2000, March).
Facing the
consequences: An examination of racial discrimination in U.S.
public
schools. Oakland, CA: Applied Research Center.
Gorman-Smith, D., & Tolan, P. H. (1998). The role of exposure
to
violence and developmental problems among inner-city youth.
Development and Psychopathology, 10, 101–116.
Gottman, J. M., & Roy, A. K. (1990). Sequential analysis: A
guide
for behavioral researchers. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University
Press.
Graham, S., & Lowery, B. S. (2004). Priming unconscious racial
stereo-
types about adolescent offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 28,
483–504.
Greenwood, C. R., Horton, B. T., & Utley, C. A. (2002).
Academic
engagement: Current perspectives on research and practice.
School
Psychology Review, 31, 328–349.
Gregory, A., & Weinstein, R. S. (2004). Connection and
regulation at
home and in school: Predicting growth in achievement for
adoles-
cents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 405–427.
Gregory, A., & Weinstein, R. S. (2008). The discipline gap and
African
Americans: Defiance or cooperation in the high school
classroom.
Journal of School Psychology, 46, 455–475.
Gregory, J. F. (1997). Three strikes and they’re out: African
American
boys and American schools’ responses to misbehavior.
International
Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 7, 25–34.
Gun-Free Schools Act of 1994, 20 U.S.C. Chapter 70, Sec. 8921
Gun-
free requirements (1994).
Hawkins, J. D., Smith, B. H., & Catalano, R. F. (2004). Social
develop-
ment and social and emotional learning. In J. E. Zins, R. P.
Weissberg,
M. C. Wang, & H. J. Walberg (Eds.), Building academic success
on
social and emotional learning: What does the research say? (pp.
135–150).
New York: Teachers College Press.
Hemphill, S. A., Toumbourou, J. W., Herrenkohl, T. I.,
McMorris, B.
J., & Catalano, R. F. (2006). The effect of school suspensions
and
arrests on subsequent adolescent antisocial behavior in
Australia and
the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 736–744.
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
january/February 2010 67
Hindelang, M. J., Hirschi, T., & Weis, J. G. (1979). Correlates
of delin-
quency: The illusion of discrepancy between self-report and
official
measures. American Sociological Review, 4, 995–1014.
Hosp, J. L., & Reschly, D. J. (2003). Referral rates for
intervention and
assessment: A meta-analysis of racial differences. Journal of
Special
Education, 37, 67–81.
Irvine, J. J. (2002). In search of wholeness: African American
teachers and
their culturally competent classroom practices. New York:
Palgrave.
KewelRamani, A., Gilbertson, L., Fox, M., & Provasnik, S.
(2007).
Status and trends in the education of racial and ethnic
minorities (NCES
2007–039). Washington, DC: National Center for Educational
Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of
Education. Retrieved February 3, 2009, from http://nces.ed.gov/
pubs2007/2007039.pdf
Kochman, T. (1981). Black and White styles of conflict.
Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Kozol, J. (2005). The shame of the nation: The restoration of
apartheid
schooling in America. New York: Crown.
Krezmien, M. P., Leone, P. E., & Achilles, G. M. (2006).
Suspension,
race, and disability: Analysis of statewide practices and
reporting.
Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 14, 217–226.
Kuther, T. L., & Fisher, C. B. (1998). Victimization by
community vio-
lence in young adolescents from a suburban city. Journal of
Early
Adolescence, 18, 53–76.
Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). But that’s just good teaching! The
case for
culturally relevant pedagogy. Theory Into Practice, 34, 159–
165.
Ladson-Billings, G. (2006). From the achievement gap to the
education
debt: Understanding achievement in U.S. schools. Educational
Researcher, 35(7), 3–12.
McCarthy, J. D., & Hoge, D. R. (1987). Social construction of
school
punishment. Social Forces, 65, 1101–1120.
McFadden, A. C., Marsh, G. E., Price, B. J., & Hwang, Y.
(1992). A
study of race and gender bias in the punishment of handicapped
school children. Urban Review, 24, 239–251.
McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child
develop-
ment. American Psychologist, 53, 185–204.
Miles, S. B., & Stipek, D. (2006). Contemporaneous and
longitudinal
associations between social behavior and literacy achievement
in a
sample of low-income elementary school children. Child
Development,
77, 103–117.
Monroe, C. R., & Obidah, J. E. (2004). The influence of cultural
syn-
chronization on a teacher’s perceptions of disruption. Journal of
Teacher Education, 55, 256–268.
Morrison, G. M., Anthony, S., Storino, M., Cheng, J., Furlong,
M. F.,
& Morrison, R. L. (2001). School expulsion as a process and an
event: Before and after effects on children at-risk for school
disci-
pline. New Directions for Youth Development: Theory,
Practice,
Research, 92, 45–72.
Morrison, G. M., & Skiba, R. J. (2001). Predicting violence
from
school misbehavior: Promises and perils. Psychology in the
Schools, 38,
173–184.
National Association of Secondary School Principals. (2000,
February).
Statement on civil rights implications of zero tolerance
programs.
Testimony presented to the United States Commission on Civil
Rights, Washington, DC.
Noguera, P. A. (2003). City schools and the American dream.
New York:
Teachers College Press.
Noguera, P. A. (2007). How listening to students can help
schools to
improve. Theory Into Practice, 46, 205–211.
Noguera, P. A., & Akom, A. (2000). The opportunity gap.
Wilson
Quarterly, 24, 86.
Noguera, P. A., & Yonemura Wing, J. (2006). Unfinished
business:
Closing the racial achievement gap in our nation’s schools. San
Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Ozer, E. (2005). The impact of violence on urban adolescents:
Longitudinal effects of perceived school connection and family
sup-
port. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 167–192.
Parrish, T. (2002). Racial disparities in the identification,
funding, and
provision of special education. In D. J. Losen & G. Orfield
(Eds.),
Racial inequity in special education (pp. 15–37). Cambridge,
MA:
Harvard Education Press.
Piquero, A. R. (2008). Disproportionate minority contact.
Future of
Children, 18, 59–79.
Pollack, M. (2008). Everyday anti-racism. London: New Press.
Raffaele Mendez, L. M. (2003). Predictors of suspension and
negative school outcomes: A longitudinal investigation. In J.
Wald
& D. J. Losen (Eds.), New directions for youth development:
No. 99.
Deconstructing the school-to-prison pipeline (pp. 17–34). San
Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Raffaele Mendez, L. M., Knoff, H. M., & Ferron, J. M. (2002).
School
demographic variables and out-of-school suspension rates: A
quanti-
tative and qualitative analysis of large ethnically diverse school
dis-
trict. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 259–276.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear
models:
Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand
Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Rausch, M. K., & Skiba, R. J. (2004). Unplanned outcomes:
Suspensions
and expulsions in Indiana. Bloomington, IN: Center for
Evaluation
and Education Policy. Retrieved July 21, 2004, from
http://ceep.indi-
ana.edu/ChildrenLeftBehind
Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Raudenbush, S. (2005).
Social anat-
omy of racial and ethnic disparities in violence. American
Journal of
Public Health, 95, 224–232.
Shaw, S. R., & Braden, J. B. (1990). Race and gender bias in
the adminis-
tration of corporal punishment. School Psychology Review, 19,
378–384.
Skiba, R. J., Michael, R. S., Nardo, A. C., & Peterson, R. L.
(2002). The
color of discipline: Sources of racial and gender
disproportionality in
school punishment. Urban Review, 34, 317–342.
Skiba, R. J., Simmons, A. B., Ritter, S., Gibb, A. C., Rausch,
M. K., &
Cuadrado, J. (2008). Achieving equity in special education:
History,
status, and current challenges. Exceptional Children, 74, 264–
288.
Stewart, E. A., Schreck, C. J., & Simons, R. L. (2006). “I ain’t
gonna let
no one disrespect me”: Does the code of the street reduce or
increase
violent victimization among African American adolescents?
Journal of
Research in Crime and Delinquency, 43, 427–458.
Thornton, C. H., & Trent, W. (1988). School desegregation and
suspen-
sion in East Baton Rouge Parish: A preliminary report. Journal
of
Negro Education, 57, 482–501.
Townsend, B. L. (2000). The disproportionate discipline of
African
American learners: Suspensions and expulsions. Exceptional
Children,
66, 381–391.
Tyler, K. M., Boykin, A. W., & Walton, T. R. (2006). Cultural
consid-
erations in teachers’ perceptions of student classroom behavior
and
achievement. Teaching and Teacher Education: An International
Journal
of Research and Studies, 22, 998–1005.
U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education
Statistics.
(2003). Status and trends in the education of Hispanics (NCES
2003–
008). Washington, DC: Author.
Vavrus, F., & Cole, K. M. (2002). “I didn’t do nothin’”: The
discursive
construction of school suspension. Urban Review, 34, 87–111.
Wallace, J. M., Jr., Goodkind, S., Wallace, C. M., & Bachman,
J. G.
(2008). Racial, ethnic, and gender differences in school
discipline
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
educational researcher68
among U.S. high school students: 1991–2005. Negro
Educational
Review, 59, 47–62.
Wehlage, G. G., & Rutter, R. A. (1986). Dropping out: How
much do
schools contribute to the problem? Teachers College Record,
87, 374–393.
Weinstein, C. S., Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Curran, M. (2004).
Toward
a conception of culturally responsive classroom management.
Journal
of Teacher Education, 55, 25–38.
Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Reaching higher: The power of
expectations in
schooling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Weinstein, R. S., Gregory, A., & Strambler, M. (2004).
Intractable self-
fulfilling prophecies: Fifty years after Brown v. Board of
Education.
American Psychologist, 59, 511–519.
Welsh, W. N., Greene, J. R., & Jenkins, P. H. (1999). School
disorder:
The influence of individual, institutional, and community
factors.
Criminology, 37, 601–643.
Westat, Inc. (2004). Summary of task force meeting on
racial/ethnic dispro-
portionality in special education. Washington, DC: Author.
Westat, Inc. (2005). Methods for assessing racial/ethnic
disproportionality
in special education: A technical assistance guide. Washington,
DC:
U.S. Department of Education Office of Special Education
Programs.
Retrieved October 5, 2006, from https://www.ideadata.org/docs/
Disproportionality%20Technical%20Assistance%20Guide.pdf
Wu, S., Pink, W., Crain, R. L., & Moles, O. (1982). Student
suspension:
A critical reappraisal. Urban Review, 14, 245–272.
AUTHoRS
ANNE GREGORY is an assistant professor in the Graduate
School of
Applied and Professional Psychology at Rutgers University, 152
Frelinghuysen Road, Piscataway, NJ 08854; [email protected]
Her research interests include disproportionality in school
discipline
sanctions and the role of teacher–student relationships in
fostering coop-
eration in the high school classroom.
RUSSELL J. SKIBA is a professor in the Department of
Counseling and
Educational Psychology and director of the Equity Project at
Indiana
University, 1900 East 10th Street, Bloomington, IN 47406;
[email protected]
.edu. His research interests include school discipline and school
violence,
and equity in school discipline and special education.
PEDRO A. NOGUERA is the Peter L. Agnew Professor of
Education at
the Steinhardt School of Culture, Education and Development,
New
York University, and executive director of the Metropolitan
Center for
Urban Education, 726 Broadway, 5th Floor, New York, NY
10003;
[email protected] His research focuses on the ways schools are
influenced
by social and economic conditions.
Manuscript received June 24, 2009
Revision received October 20, 2009
Accepted November 2, 2009
at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from
http://er.aera.net
Learning Exercise #1Delving into Journal ArticlesObjective To.docx

More Related Content

PDF
Roberto Manuel Osorio Soto
PPT
Education Research 2, I Bimestre
PPT
Educational Research II, II Bimestre
PPTX
ENG-107-Week-1.pptxzjzxcbzjkvbkcbvcbvxcbvxcv
PPT
A1 2009
DOC
Unit One.doc
PPT
PPT
Applied linguistics
Roberto Manuel Osorio Soto
Education Research 2, I Bimestre
Educational Research II, II Bimestre
ENG-107-Week-1.pptxzjzxcbzjkvbkcbvcbvxcbvxcv
A1 2009
Unit One.doc
Applied linguistics

Similar to Learning Exercise #1Delving into Journal ArticlesObjective To.docx (20)

PDF
-2016 SASH SINGH - POSTER
PPTX
Linguistic : Slide chapter-12
PPT
PPTX
Diagnostic session advanced English
PPT
Approaches to Language Teaching
DOCX
Mgt 205 business communication
PPTX
(Applied linguistics) shmitt's book ch 1
PPTX
Natural approach
PPTX
FL1.pptx
PPSX
How to write a report
PPTX
PPT1 - ENGL 3723
PPTX
Health and Wellness in Diverse Communities - French - 11th Grade by Slidesgo....
PPTX
Back-Shifting - Malaysia - 7th litcon & 4th ill cl - 11-13 oct 2011 - ras
PPT
Alikarakas elf 5
PPTX
cross culture understanding.pptx
PPT
support group.pptx
PDF
The Linguist Blog Book
PDF
The Linguist Blog Book
PDF
The Linguist Blog Book
PDF
1st day world languages prezo
-2016 SASH SINGH - POSTER
Linguistic : Slide chapter-12
Diagnostic session advanced English
Approaches to Language Teaching
Mgt 205 business communication
(Applied linguistics) shmitt's book ch 1
Natural approach
FL1.pptx
How to write a report
PPT1 - ENGL 3723
Health and Wellness in Diverse Communities - French - 11th Grade by Slidesgo....
Back-Shifting - Malaysia - 7th litcon & 4th ill cl - 11-13 oct 2011 - ras
Alikarakas elf 5
cross culture understanding.pptx
support group.pptx
The Linguist Blog Book
The Linguist Blog Book
The Linguist Blog Book
1st day world languages prezo
Ad

More from smile790243 (20)

DOCX
PART B Please response to these two original posts below. Wh.docx
DOCX
Part C Developing Your Design SolutionThe Production Cycle.docx
DOCX
PART A You will create a media piece based around the theme of a.docx
DOCX
Part 4. Implications to Nursing Practice & Implication to Patien.docx
DOCX
PART AHepatitis C is a chronic liver infection that can be e.docx
DOCX
Part A post your answer to the following question1. How m.docx
DOCX
PART BPlease response to these two original posts below..docx
DOCX
Part A (50 Points)Various men and women throughout history .docx
DOCX
Part A1. K2. D3. N4. C5. A6. O7. F8. Q9. H10..docx
DOCX
Part A Develop an original age-appropriate activity for your .docx
DOCX
Part 3 Social Situations2. Identify multicultural challenges th.docx
DOCX
Part A (1000 words) Annotated Bibliography - Create an annota.docx
DOCX
Part 6 Disseminating Results Create a 5-minute, 5- to 6-sli.docx
DOCX
Part 3 Social Situations • Proposal paper which identifies multicul.docx
DOCX
Part 3 Social Situations 2. Identify multicultural challenges that .docx
DOCX
Part 2The client is a 32-year-old Hispanic American male who c.docx
DOCX
Part 2For this section of the template, focus on gathering deta.docx
DOCX
Part 2 Observation Summary and Analysis • Summary paper of observat.docx
DOCX
Part 2 Observation Summary and Analysis 1. Review and implement any.docx
DOCX
Part 2Data collectionfrom your change study initiative,.docx
PART B Please response to these two original posts below. Wh.docx
Part C Developing Your Design SolutionThe Production Cycle.docx
PART A You will create a media piece based around the theme of a.docx
Part 4. Implications to Nursing Practice & Implication to Patien.docx
PART AHepatitis C is a chronic liver infection that can be e.docx
Part A post your answer to the following question1. How m.docx
PART BPlease response to these two original posts below..docx
Part A (50 Points)Various men and women throughout history .docx
Part A1. K2. D3. N4. C5. A6. O7. F8. Q9. H10..docx
Part A Develop an original age-appropriate activity for your .docx
Part 3 Social Situations2. Identify multicultural challenges th.docx
Part A (1000 words) Annotated Bibliography - Create an annota.docx
Part 6 Disseminating Results Create a 5-minute, 5- to 6-sli.docx
Part 3 Social Situations • Proposal paper which identifies multicul.docx
Part 3 Social Situations 2. Identify multicultural challenges that .docx
Part 2The client is a 32-year-old Hispanic American male who c.docx
Part 2For this section of the template, focus on gathering deta.docx
Part 2 Observation Summary and Analysis • Summary paper of observat.docx
Part 2 Observation Summary and Analysis 1. Review and implement any.docx
Part 2Data collectionfrom your change study initiative,.docx
Ad

Recently uploaded (20)

PDF
Hazard Identification & Risk Assessment .pdf
PDF
Skin Care and Cosmetic Ingredients Dictionary ( PDFDrive ).pdf
PDF
BP 704 T. NOVEL DRUG DELIVERY SYSTEMS (UNIT 1)
PPTX
Climate Change and Its Global Impact.pptx
PPTX
Unit 4 Computer Architecture Multicore Processor.pptx
PDF
My India Quiz Book_20210205121199924.pdf
PDF
BP 704 T. NOVEL DRUG DELIVERY SYSTEMS (UNIT 2).pdf
PPTX
Introduction to pro and eukaryotes and differences.pptx
PDF
Environmental Education MCQ BD2EE - Share Source.pdf
PDF
AI-driven educational solutions for real-life interventions in the Philippine...
PPTX
A powerpoint presentation on the Revised K-10 Science Shaping Paper
PDF
Myanmar Dental Journal, The Journal of the Myanmar Dental Association (2013).pdf
PDF
Race Reva University – Shaping Future Leaders in Artificial Intelligence
PDF
semiconductor packaging in vlsi design fab
PDF
Journal of Dental Science - UDMY (2021).pdf
PDF
International_Financial_Reporting_Standa.pdf
PDF
LIFE & LIVING TRILOGY - PART (3) REALITY & MYSTERY.pdf
PDF
BP 505 T. PHARMACEUTICAL JURISPRUDENCE (UNIT 1).pdf
PDF
MBA _Common_ 2nd year Syllabus _2021-22_.pdf
PDF
LIFE & LIVING TRILOGY - PART - (2) THE PURPOSE OF LIFE.pdf
Hazard Identification & Risk Assessment .pdf
Skin Care and Cosmetic Ingredients Dictionary ( PDFDrive ).pdf
BP 704 T. NOVEL DRUG DELIVERY SYSTEMS (UNIT 1)
Climate Change and Its Global Impact.pptx
Unit 4 Computer Architecture Multicore Processor.pptx
My India Quiz Book_20210205121199924.pdf
BP 704 T. NOVEL DRUG DELIVERY SYSTEMS (UNIT 2).pdf
Introduction to pro and eukaryotes and differences.pptx
Environmental Education MCQ BD2EE - Share Source.pdf
AI-driven educational solutions for real-life interventions in the Philippine...
A powerpoint presentation on the Revised K-10 Science Shaping Paper
Myanmar Dental Journal, The Journal of the Myanmar Dental Association (2013).pdf
Race Reva University – Shaping Future Leaders in Artificial Intelligence
semiconductor packaging in vlsi design fab
Journal of Dental Science - UDMY (2021).pdf
International_Financial_Reporting_Standa.pdf
LIFE & LIVING TRILOGY - PART (3) REALITY & MYSTERY.pdf
BP 505 T. PHARMACEUTICAL JURISPRUDENCE (UNIT 1).pdf
MBA _Common_ 2nd year Syllabus _2021-22_.pdf
LIFE & LIVING TRILOGY - PART - (2) THE PURPOSE OF LIFE.pdf

Learning Exercise #1Delving into Journal ArticlesObjective To.docx

  • 1. Learning Exercise #1 Delving into Journal Articles Objective: To help students better understand the process of research and research methods Directions: You will be assigned a scholarly article. After reading the article, you’re requied to answer all the questions below completely. Based upon your reading of the article, you should address the following questions in a 1 to a 1 and ½ page (max.) typed paper: (please refer to APA Style guidelines) 1. What is the research question? 2. What theory did these authors use? 3. What were the authors’ hypotheses? 4. Was the research deductive or inductive? 5. How were the variables operationalized? 6. What kind of relationship exists between the variables? (correlation, cause and effect, or spurious. Define these definitions using the text and provide examples from your article.) 7. What method did the researchers use? (Survey, field study, experiment, existing sources, or triangulation, or another method? Explain.) 8. Who composed the sample? Was it representative? Grading criteria: Your ability to communicate your thoughts in writing to include appropriate grammar, punctuation, spelling, syntax, evidence of appropriate editing (10 points), your ability to critique the article’s research question, methods, and overall argument (15), and your ability to demonstrate correct use and
  • 2. application of the concepts from the text to the research article (15 points).40 total points possible**DUE DATE: MONDAY, February 5th (11:59 PM) Express Yourself Language Institute Increased competitive edge • Improved self-esteem • Enhanced thinking skills Background Many colleges and universities require foreign language study, and U.S. students from every degree-granting educational institution recognize the importance of fluency in a foreign language. The Express Yourself Language Institute (EYLI) is the premier destination for students wishing to get a head start on their language studies. Drawing on student enrollment patterns as well as trends in language skills sought by business recruiters, the EYLI programs continually evolve to serve our customers’ current and future needs. Although we started in a small office with a reception area and only two classrooms, we have grown exponentially over the years, expanding from a single location on the south side of Chicago to more than 20 locations in nine cities. Although initially we offered only courses in Spanish, we now offer courses in eight foreign languages as well as American Sign Language. Express Yourself Language Institute through the Years Year New Cities New Languages 2007 Chicago Spanish 2009
  • 3. Tampa French, German 2011 Albuquerque, Boston Chinese, Japanese Today Kansas City, Detroit, Huntsville Arabic, Korean, ASL Current Language Study Statistics According to the Modern Language Association, the language with the largest percentage growth has been Arabic, growing by 46.3% and becoming the third most important language to learn for business. Other languages with significant increases to enrollment include the following: · Korean 19.1% · Chinese 18.2% · American Sign Language 16.4% · Portuguese 10.8% · Japanese 10.3% The main driver for the increased enrollment in EYLI and other language institutes is, of course, a heightened demand by employers for bilingual and multilingual employees. The table below lists the industries that are more likely to hire employees with knowledge of a second language, and the percentage of EYLI students who have expressed an interest in those industries. As we have in the past, EYLI will continue to follow these trends and respond appropriately. In response to the latest statistics, EYLI has decided to add the following to its course offerings: Beginning and Intermediate Arabic Fall 2015 Because of the increased interest in Arabic language studies, we
  • 4. will offer two courses in Arabic. We anticipate that 30% of students taking Begining Arabic will continue on this language track and sign up for Intermediate Arabic. Advanced Chinese Spring 2016 Many students studying Chinese at the Intermediate level at our facilities across the country have requested an advanced level Chinese course. We hope to hire native speakers to meet this demand. Additional upper-level Korean, Portuguese, and Japanese courses are also in the works. We hope to release information about these programs as soon as we have fully assessed the demand. This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng This file created specifically for Dominica Esono Nseng Shelly Cashman Word 2013| Chapter 3: SAM Project 1a Shelly Cashman Word 2013 Chapter 3: SAM Project 1a Express Yourself Language Institute FORMAT A REPORT USING TABLES AND GRAPHICS Project Goal M Project Name Project Goal
  • 5. PROJECT DESCRIPTION You are working with the media director for the Express Yourself Language Institute to develop new marketing materials. You will create a report that summarizes the state of foreign language learning in the United States. To clarify the presentation and add visual interest, you will format the report using tables, graphics, and other visual aids.GETTING STARTED · Download the following file from the SAM website: · SC_Word2013_C3_P1a_FirstLastName_1.docx · Open the file you just downloaded and save it with the name: · SC_Word2013_C3_P1a_FirstLastName_2.docx · Hint: If you do not see the .docx file extension in the Save file dialog box, do not type it. Word will add the file extension for you automatically. · To complete this Project, you will also need to download and save the following support files from the SAM website: · support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_language-logo.png · support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_classroom.png · With the file SC_Word2013_C3_P1a_FirstLastName_2.docx still open, ensure that your first and last name is displayed in the footer. If the footer does not display your name, delete the file and download a new copy from the SAM website. PROJECT STEPS 1. Change the document margins to Moderate. 2. Move the insertion point before the word “Express” in the headline paragraph “Express Yourself Language Institute” and insert the image file support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_language- logo.png available for download from the SAM website. 3. Resize the image so that it is 0.5” tall and change the text wrap format to Through. 4. Recolor the image by applying Gold, Accent color 2 Light (3rd column, 3rd row of the Recolor section of the Color gallery).
  • 6. 5. Change the font size of the headline paragraph “Express Yourself Language Institute” to 28 pt. and center-align the text. (Hint: The image should remain left-aligned.) 6. Apply the Grid Table 4 – Accent 1 style to the table “Express Yourself Language Institute through the Years” and format the table using AutoFit Window. 7. Merge the three cells in the first row of the table and center- align the text in the new merged cell. 8. Insert a new row in the table immediately above the row starting with "Today" and enter the data shown in Table 1 below. Table 1: New Table Row © 2014 Cengage Learning. 9. On page 2, move the insertion point to the blank line after the paragraph “The main driver…interest in those industries.” Insert a table with 2 columns and 5 rows and enter the data shown in Table 2 below. Table 2: New Table © 2014 Cengage Learning. 10. Apply the Grid Table 5 Dark – Accent 1 style and format the table using AutoFit Contents. 11. Center-align the text in column 2 and then center the entire
  • 7. table on the page. 12. Insert the Rounded Rectangle shape in the blank line above the paragraph “In response to….” Resize the shape to 0.3” high and 6.75” wide. 13. Type the text Our Response into the shape you just inserted. Left-align the text, apply bold formatting, and change the font color to White, Background 1 (1st column, 1st row in the Theme Colors palette). 14. Select the heading "Beginning and Intermediate Arabic Fall 2015" and add a left tab stop at 4". Move the insertion point before the text "Fall 2015" and insert a tab so that the text is aligned with the new tab stop. 15. Move the insertion point to the blank line after the paragraph “Additional upper-level Korean…assessed the demand.” and insert the image file support_SC_W13_C3_P1a_classroom.png available for download from the SAM website. 16. Resize the image so that it is 75% of its original size, and then recolor the image by applying Gold, Accent color 2 Light (3rd column, 3rd row of the Recolor section in the Color gallery). 17. Check the Spelling & Grammar in the document to identify and correct any spelling errors. (Hint: You should find and correct at least 1 spelling error.) Your document should look like the Final Figure on the following pages. Save your changes, close the document and exit Word. Follow the directions on the SAM website to submit your completed project. Final Figure Microsoft product screenshots used with permission from Microsoft Corporation. Logo © 2014 Cengage Learning. Copyright © 2014 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved.
  • 8. Picture source: Office.com Copyright © 2014 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. 2 Educational Researcher, Vol. 39, No. 1, pp. 59–68 DOI: 10.3102/0013189X09357621 © 2010 AERA. http://er.aera.net january/February 2010 59 The gap in achievement across racial and ethnic groups has been a focus of education research for decades, but the disproportionate suspension and expulsion of Black, Latino, and American Indian stu- dents has received less attention. This article synthesizes research on racial and ethnic patterns in school sanctions and considers how disproportionate discipline might contribute to lagging achievement among students of color. It further examines the evidence for stu-
  • 9. dent, school, and community contributors to the racial and ethnic patterns in school sanctions, and it offers promising directions for gap-reducing discipline policies and practices. Keywords: achievement gap; at-risk students; classroom management; school psychology; student behavior/ attitude; violence A lthough our national discourse on racial disparity tends to focus on academic outcomes—the so-called achieve- ment gap—in school districts throughout the United States, Black, Latino, and American Indian students are also sub- ject to a differential and disproportionate rate of school disciplin- ary sanctions, ranging from office disciplinary referrals to corporal punishment, suspension, and expulsion (Krezmien, Leone, & Achilles, 2006; Wallace, Goodkind, Wallace, & Bachman, 2008). Ostensibly, the intent of school disciplinary interventions is to preserve order and safety by removing students who break school rules and disrupt the school learning environment and, by setting an example of those punished students, to deter other students from committing future rule infractions. However, schools tend
  • 10. to rely heavily on exclusion from the classroom as the primary discipline strategy (Arcia, 2006), and this practice often has a dis- proportionate impact on Black, Latino, and American Indian students. The use of school exclusion as a discipline practice may contribute to the well-documented racial gaps in academic achievement. This suggests that there is a pressing need for schol- arly attention to the racial discipline gap if efforts addressing the achievement gap are to have greater likelihood of success. In this article, we synthesize the research on racial and eth- nic patterns in school discipline, and we suggest how the racial discipline gap influences racial patterns in achievement. We then review the evidence on the factors that contribute to the disci- pline gap. Specifically, we examine the degree to which low- income status, low achievement, and rates of misconduct contribute to why Black, Latino, and American Indian students are overselected and oversanctioned in the discipline system. We argue that such student characteristics are not adequate to explain the large disparities, and we describe school and teacher contributors that need to be investigated in future research. Finally, we identify methodological challenges to the study of disproportionality and discuss promising strategies for gap- reducing interventions. Safety Efforts and Racial Disproportionality A large body of evidence shows that Black students are subject to a disproportionate amount of discipline in school settings, and a
  • 11. smaller and less consistent literature suggests disproportionate sanctioning of Latino and American Indian students in some schools.1 This conclusion has been drawn across a wide array of sanctions (e.g., suspensions, office discipline referrals) and meth- odology (see discussion below). The Children’s Defense Fund (1975) first brought the issue of racial disproportionality to national attention, showing that Black students were two to three times overrepresented in school suspensions compared with their enrollment rates in localities across the nation. National and state data show consistent patterns of Black disproportionality in school discipline over the past 30 years, specifically in suspension (McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Raffaele Mendez, Knoff, & Ferron, 2002), expulsion (KewelRamani, Gilbertson, Fox, & Provasnik, 2007), and office discipline referrals (Skiba, Michael, Nardo, & Peterson, 2002). According to a nationally representative study utilizing parent reports, in 2003 Black students were significantly more likely to be suspended than White or Asian students (p < .001). Specifically, almost 1 in 5 Black students (19.6%) were suspended, compared with fewer than 1 in 10 White students (8.8%) and Asian and Pacific Islanders (6.4%; KewelRamani et al., 2007). A nationally representative survey of 74,000 10th graders found that about 50% of Black students reported that they had ever been suspended or expelled compared with about 20% of White students (Wallace et al., 2008). The study further showed that, unlike the pattern for other racial and ethnic groups, suspensions and expulsions of Black students increased from 1991 to 2005 (Wallace et al., 2008). The Achievement Gap and the Discipline Gap:
  • 12. Two Sides of the Same Coin? Anne Gregory, Russell J. Skiba, and Pedro A. Noguera at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net educational researcher60 Although disproportionality in school discipline has been documented for Latino and American Indian students, findings related to such disparities have been inconsistent. National data (U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, 2003) show that, based on parent surveys administered in 1999, 20% of Latino students in Grades 7 through 12 had ever been suspended or expelled, which is a statistically significantly lower rate (p < .001) than for Black students (35%) and a statisti- cally significantly higher rate (p < .001) than for White students (15%). Analyzing racial disparities in discipline, Gordon, Della Piana, and Keleher (2000) found that, in 3 of the 10 cities stud- ied, the rates of suspended and expelled Latino students were 10% or more than 10% higher than the percentage of enrolled Latino students. Inconsistency in findings was further confirmed in a study measuring disproportionality using odds ratios. Based on state records from Maryland, Krezmien et al. (2006) found that Latino students had similar or lower odds than White stu- dents of being suspended for 9 successive years (1995–2003). National and state data have also shown disproportionality in discipline for American Indian students, although again there
  • 13. appears to be some inconsistency (Wallace et al., 2008). Krezmien et al. (2006) showed that American Indian and White students had a similar chance of being suspended from 1995 to 1998 in Maryland. However, from 1998 to 2003, they found that American Indians had significantly higher odds than Whites of being suspended (odds ratios ranged from 1.5 to 1.8). The dis- proportionality in American Indian suspension was again docu- mented in nationally representative samples using school records (DeVoe & Darling-Churchill, 2008) and student reports (Wallace et al., 2008). It is unclear whether the inconsistent find- ings on American Indian suspension is a statistical artifact given their relatively small numbers of suspended students (e.g., Krezmien et al., 2006) or if it reflects actual variability in dispro- portionate suspension rates across time and school districts. Males of all racial and ethnic groups are more likely than females to receive disciplinary sanctions. In 2004, only 1% of Asian Pacific Islander females were suspended, compared with 11% of Asian Pacific Islander males (KewelRamani et al., 2007). Expulsion data from that same year showed that White females were half as likely to be expelled as White males (p < .001), and similarly, Black females were half as likely to be expelled as Black males (p < .05). Black males are especially at risk for receiving discipline sanctions, with one study showing that Black males were 16 times as likely as White females to be suspended (J. F. Gregory, 1997). Racial Disproportionality and Patterns in Achievement
  • 14. The consistent pattern of disproportionate discipline sanctions issued to Black students and the trends in sanctions for Latino and American Indian students, albeit less consistent, have rarely been considered in light of the well-documented racial and ethnic disparities in school achievement (KewelRamani et al., 2007). In many schools, large proportions of a group (e.g., Black males) receive at least one suspension, which typically results in missed instructional time and, for some, could exacerbate a cycle of aca- demic failure, disengagement, and escalating rule breaking (Arcia, 2006). In fact, a suspended student may miss anywhere from one class period to 10 or more school days, depending on the violation and school policies. One of the most consistent findings of modern education research is the strong positive rela- tionship between time engaged in academic learning and student achievement (Brophy, 1988; Fisher et al., 1981; Greenwood, Horton, & Utley, 2002). The school disciplinary practices used most widely throughout the United States may be contributing to lowered academic performance among the group of students in greatest need of improvement. Research shows that frequent suspensions appear to signifi- cantly increase the risk of academic underperformance (Davis & Jordan, 1994). Arcia (2006) followed two demographically simi- lar cohorts (matched on gender, race, grade level, family poverty, and limited English proficiency), contrasting a cohort that had received at least one suspension with another that had received no suspensions. In Year 1, suspended students were three grade
  • 15. levels behind their nonsuspended peers in their reading skills, but were almost 5 years behind 2 years later. Although other unmea- sured risk factors may have contributed to cohort differences, suspension may have initiated or maintained a process of with- drawal from learning in the classroom. In the long term, school suspension has been found to be a moderate to strong predictor of dropout and not graduating on time (Ekstrom, Goertz, Pollack, & Rock, 1986; Raffaele Mendez, 2003; Wehlage & Rutter, 1986). Discipline sanctions resulting in exclusion from school may damage the learning process in other ways as well. Suspended students may become less bonded to school, less invested in school rules and course work, and subsequently, less motivated to achieve academic success. Students who are less bonded to school may be more likely to turn to lawbreaking activities and become less likely to experience academic success. Consistent findings highlight the importance of school bonding for reducing the risk of delinquency (Hawkins, Smith, & Catalano, 2004). Conversely, Hemphill, Toumbourou, Herrenkohl, McMorris, and Catalano (2006) found that taking into account previous violent and aggressive behavior and a multitude of other risk factors (e.g., negative peer group, low grades), school suspension actually increased the risk of antisocial behavior a year later. In sum, dis- proportionate school discipline experienced by some racial and ethnic groups has important implications for academic out- comes. There is a need for research to identify why racial dispro- portionality in discipline occurs and what types of disciplinary practices might be less likely to exacerbate academic outcomes.
  • 16. Explanations for the Racial Discipline Gap Certain demographic characteristics that are more common among some racial and ethnic groups have been used as a primary explanation for the racial discipline gap (see, e.g., National Association of Secondary School Principals, 2000). Low-income students with histories of low achievement, who reside in high- crime/high-poverty neighborhoods, may be at greater risk for engaging in behavior resulting in office disciplinary referrals and school suspension. A review of the literature suggests that such characteristics likely account for some proportion of the gap in sanctions across groups. Yet there is no evidence to suggest demo- graphic factors are in any way sufficient to “explain away” the gap. Teacher and school factors need to be considered as possible at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net january/February 2010 61 contributors to the overselection and oversanction of Black, Latino, and American Indian students. Poverty and Neighborhood Characteristics Race, socioeconomic status (SES), and characteristics of neigh- borhoods associated with risk of negative outcomes are frequently
  • 17. connected in the United States (Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994; McLoyd, 1998). The confluence of these factors makes it challenging to separate out the contributions of each to the racial discipline gap. Many low-income students living in urban neighborhoods may experience adversity, such as exposure to violence and substance abuse, which may increase the likeli- hood of their receiving school sanctions (Brantlinger, 1991; Bureau of Justice Statistics, 2005). Although there is no evidence that exposure to violence causes behavior difficulties, correla- tional studies show links between exposure to violence and stu- dent mental health and behavior in the classroom (e.g., Kuther & Fisher, 1998). Many violence-exposed children suffer from anxiety, irritability, stress, and hypervigilence (Gorman-Smith & Tolan, 1998). These conditions may have a negative effect upon behavior in classrooms and result in increased discipline referrals. Exposure to violence may also influence how students cope in school. One coping mechanism to ward off the threat of violence includes presenting a “tough front” or even arming oneself to ward off future victimization (Anderson, 1999; Stewart, Schreck, & Simons, 2006). The need to negotiate what Anderson has called the “code of the street” may contribute to behavior prob- lems in school as students from high-crime neighborhoods adjust to a different set of norms in their interactions with peers and teachers in school settings (Dance, 2002). Additional research is needed to tease apart community effects (e.g., concentrated pov- erty, neighborhood crime, and the stress of low SES) and their impact on student behavior in school.
  • 18. It is important to distinguish, however, between the role of poverty in predicting disruptive behavior and the ways it may contribute to racial and ethnic disparities in discipline. Existing school discipline research suggests that student SES is limited in its explanatory power of the racial discipline gap (McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Wallace et al., 2008). Whether statistically control- ling for a measure of SES at the school level (percentage of par- ents unemployed or percentage of students enrolled in free or reduced-cost meals; Raffaele Mendez et al., 2002; Wu, Pink, Crain, & Moles, 1982) or at the student level (parental education or qualification for free or reduced-cost meals; McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Skiba et al., 2002), multivariate analyses have repeatedly demonstrated that racial differences in discipline rates remain significant. The most recent of these analyses (Wallace et al., 2008) used a series of logistic regressions to test racial/ethnic disparities in office disciplinary referrals, suspension, and expul- sion. Race/ethnicity remained a significant predictor of all three disciplinary outcomes even after accounting for student- reported parental education, family structure (e.g., single-parent house- hold), and urbanicity of neighborhood. In sum, being enrolled in a school with high rates of low-income students (Raffaele Mendez et al., 2002; Wu et al., 1982) or being from a low-income family (McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Skiba et al., 2002) does increase the likelihood that a student will be subject to punitive forms of discipline and even appears to make a mild contribution to disproportionality (Wallace et al., 2008). Yet the highly consis- tent finding that race/ethnicity remains a significant predictor of
  • 19. discipline even after statistically controlling for measures of fam- ily income suggests that student SES is not sufficient to explain the racial discipline gap. In fact, some research has found an inverse relationship between student demographics and rates of disproportionality in school discipline. Rausch and Skiba (2004), examining suspen- sion and expulsion records across one Midwestern state, reported that Black students are at greater risk of suspension when com- pared with White students, not in urban schools but, rather, in more resource-rich suburban schools. Other research suggests that the context of school or district racial climate may have an influence on rates of disproportionality. Thornton and Trent (1988) reported that racial disproportionality in school suspen- sion was greatest in schools that had been recently desegregated, especially if those schools had a higher SES student population. Conversely, Eitle and Eitle (2004) found decreased rates of dis- proportionality in school suspension in schools that became resegregated. Such data suggest that, at the school and district levels, financial resources, staff perceptions, and racial climate may be as important as student demographics in predicting racial disparity. Low Achievement Low achievement is another variable that may contribute to the racial discipline gap. A wide body of research documents a persis- tent pattern that Asian and White students score higher on achievement tests compared with Black, Latino, and American Indian students (A. Gregory & Weinstein, 2004; U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education
  • 20. Statistics, 2003). Faced with repeated academic struggles, under- performing students may become frustrated and disaffected and have lower self-confidence, all of which may contribute to a higher rate of school disruption (Miles & Stipek, 2006). Low literacy achievement in the elementary grades is linked to later aggression in third and fifth grades (Miles & Stipek, 2006). Similar patterns have been found in later grades—low achieve- ment in middle and high school is linked with more serious forms of aggression a year later (Choi, 2007). Although it is clear that low achievement is highly correlated with aggressive behav- ior and disciplinary infractions, such patterns in and of them- selves do not explain disproportionality in discipline. Studies of the relationship between achievement and student discipline have shown that when taking into account grade point average, race remains a predictor of suspension (Wehlage & Rutter, 1986). Moreover, it is also possible that any relationship between the achievement gap and the discipline gap is in fact the product of other variables, such as educational disadvantage. Ladson- Billings (2006) argues that what is widely viewed as an achievement gap between White and Black students could more properly be termed an “education debt” in that educational opportunities in the Unites States have historically never been equalized for differ- ent groups. McLloyd (1998) notes that poverty’s effects on stu- dents are mediated not simply by family or community risk factors but also by poor school conditions in disadvantaged neighborhoods. Poor students of color are more likely to attend schools with lower quality resources and facilities (Kozol, 2005), at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28,
  • 21. 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net educational researcher62 higher teacher turnover, and a lower percentage of highly quali- fied teachers (Darling-Hammond, 2004). Discrepancies in the quality of resources available to rich and poor districts are well documented, but there is a need for sound policy research that can specify how to address resource disparities in order to posi- tively affect both the achievement gap and the discipline gap. Differential Behavior Another explanation for the racial discipline gap is that students from certain racial and ethnic groups misbehave or contribute to a lack of safety in schools more than students from other racial and ethnic groups. Studies using both measures of student self- report and extant school disciplinary records have examined this premise and have generally failed to find evidence of racial differ- ences in student behavior (e.g., Skiba et al., 2002; Wehlage & Rutter, 1986). In one of the earliest longitudinal studies of stu- dent race and school sanctions for misbehavior, Wehlage and Rutter examined predictors of school sanctions for 7th, 9th, and 11th graders over a 3-year period and reported that Black stu- dents did not consistently report more misbehavior than White students. This failure to find consistently large racial and ethnic differences in student self-reported behavior has been corrobo- rated in the literature (McCarthy & Hoge, 1987; Wu et al., 1982). A recent study using a nationally representative sample showed few and generally small differences in self-reported unsafe behavior across racial groups compared with the racial discrep-
  • 22. ancy in discipline sanctions (Dinkes, Cataldi, & Lin-Kelly, 2007). There were, for example, no differences in self-reported weapon carrying among Black, White, and American Indian stu- dents. Some of the most recent data on school safety (Bauer, Guerino, Nolle, & Tang, 2008) show that victimization by vio- lence or theft is not statistically differentiated by race, with simi- lar percentages of White (4.7%), Black (3.8%), and Latino (3.9%) students reporting that they had been victimized in the past 6 months in school. The use of self-report data, however, can raise questions about the accuracy of the student reporters and hence the validity of the results. Hindelang, Hirschi, and Weis (1979) hypothesized that the failure to find differences between Black and White self- report of serious delinquent behavior could be due to underre- porting by Black youth. Studies examining this hypothesis, however, have failed to find support for it. McCarthy and Hoge (1987) examined whether Black students, more than White stu- dents, underreported their rule-breaking behavior. Comparing student self-report with a sample of teacher reports of rule break- ing from a sample of 1,125 7th and 11th graders, the researchers found no clear pattern that teacher reports were more highly cor- related with either White or Black self-reports of misconduct, and they concluded that neither group tended to systematically under- or over-report their misconduct. The findings of self-report data have also been corroborated by studies using extant school data on office referrals, which have also failed to find substantial differences in rates of disruptive school behavior by race. McFadden, Marsh, Price, and Hwang (1992), studying discipline records in a single Florida school
  • 23. dis- trict, found no general differences in behavior between White and Black students and indeed found that White students engaged in a higher level of those behaviors (e.g., defiance, fighting, and bothering others) that tended to result in suspension or corporal punishment. Similarly, Shaw and Braden (1990) reported that White children in a single school district were significantly more likely than Black children to be referred for disciplinary action for severe rule violations, despite the overrepresentation of Black stu- dents in that district in corporal punishment. Finally, Skiba et al. (2002) set out specifically to test the differential behavior hypoth- esis, using disciplinary referrals from all 19 middle schools in a single large urban district. They found no evidence that either Black or White students were referred to the office for more seri- ous behaviors. The analyses did show, however, that reasons for referring White students tended to be for causes that were more objectively observable (smoking, vandalism, leaving without per- mission, obscene language), whereas office referrals for Black stu- dents were more likely to occur in response to behaviors (loitering, disrespect, threat, excessive noise) that appear to be more subjec- tive in nature. In short, there appears to be a notable paucity of evidence that could support a hypothesis that the racial discipline gap can be explained through differential rates of misbehavior.
  • 24. Differential Selection In juvenile justice research, there has been a similar focus on exploring disproportionate minority contact in the justice system (Piquero, 2008). Some of this research has sought to identify whether the high incarceration rates of ethnic minority youth, compared with the rates of White youth, are due to their higher rates of illegal behavior or due to institutional practices such as patterns in police surveillance, racial profiling, or biased sentenc- ing (Piquero, 2008). This research provides a useful framework for understanding discrimination as a contributor to the racial discipline gap in schools. Specifically, the “differential selection” hypothesis asserts that ethnic minorities are more likely to be arrested because they are more likely to be picked out for wrong- doing despite similar levels of infractions (Piquero, 2008). This hypothesis is useful when applied to the school setting; that is, despite relatively similar rates of disruption, Black, Latino, or American Indian students may be more likely to be differentially selected for discipline consequences. There is a fairly substantial research base suggesting that dif- ferential selection at the classroom level contributes in some way to racial/ethnic disproportionality in school disciplinary out- comes. Consistent findings of disproportionality in office refer- rals (Skiba et al., 2002; Skiba et al., 2008; Wallace et al., 2008) suggest that racial/ethnic disparities in discipline begin at the classroom level. In an ethnographic observational study of urban classrooms, Vavrus and Cole (2002) found that many office
  • 25. refer- rals leading to school suspension were due to what the authors described as a student’s “violation of implicit interactional codes,” most often a student calling into question established classroom practices or the teacher’s authority. Those students singled out in this way were disproportionately students of color. Skiba et al. (2002) reported on findings of referrals based on objective versus subjective reasons by race. Together with findings that Black stu- dents are more likely than White students to be referred to the office for defiance (A. Gregory & Weinstein, 2008) or noncom- pliance (Skiba et al., 2008), these results strongly suggest that at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net january/February 2010 63 some process of differential selection at the classroom level may contribute to disparities in discipline. Explanations for the overselection of certain students for dis- cipline may include cultural mismatch, implicit bias, or negative expectations in classrooms and schools. The cultural mismatch hypothesis suggests that the classroom culture or the teacher’s culture is at odds with the culture of ethnic minority students (Irvine, 2002; Townsend, 2000). For instance, Boykin and col- leagues argued that Western European–based individualism and
  • 26. competitiveness are the dominant underlying ideologies guiding classroom activities (Boykin, Tyler, & Miller, 2005)—an orienta- tion that may clash with a stronger emphasis on communal val- ues in Black, Latino, and American Indian culture (Gay, 2006). Gay further suggested that communicative tensions can arise through cultural difference. Specifically, differences in ways of communicating between Blacks (e.g., animated, interpersonal) and Whites (e.g., dispassionate, impersonal) may lead to conflict (Kochman, 1981). In a study of 62 White elementary teachers who taught in two predominantly Black schools, Tyler, Boykin, and Walton (2006) found that teachers were more likely to rate vignettes of students who exhibited competitive and individual- istic behavior as motivated and achievement oriented than stu- dents who exhibited more communal and vervistic (e.g., collaborative and multitasking) behaviors. Such findings, if vali- dated in actual classroom settings, would indicate a differential perception on the part of teachers that could well advantage White students exhibiting competitive behaviors and disadvan- tage Black students exhibiting a more active and community- oriented learning style. Other scholars have focused on ways in which negative teacher beliefs and expectations can contribute to racially related author- ity conflicts (R. S. Weinstein, 2002; R. S. Weinstein, Gregory, & Strambler, 2004). In her ethnography of school discipline in an elementary school, Ferguson (2000) observed patterns in nega- tive teacher–student interactions and argued that these events were fueled by White teachers’ overreacting and relying on stereo- types to interpret Black students’ language and physical expres- sion. Given stereotypes and media portrayals of Black youth as
  • 27. dangerous and aggressive (Devine & Elliot, 2000; Noguera & Akom, 2000), teacher expectations for behavior may also influ- ence whether these students are selected for discipline sanctions. A related area of research examines how implicit beliefs may neg- atively affect Black and Latino students. Implicit racial bias, according to social psychologists, operates out of conscious awareness yet influences decision making (e.g., Dovidio, Glick, & Rudman, 2005). Although no studies have been conducted on the implicit bias of teachers and how race may activate stereo- types, Graham and Lowery (2004) conducted an analogous experimental study with police and probation officers. They found that, compared with officers who were subliminally primed with neutral, non-race-related words, officers who had been subliminally primed with words related to the category Black were more likely to recommend harsher punishments for adolescents who had committed crimes, as presented in standard- ized, written vignettes. Taken together, research on classroom processes suggests that Black students are differentially selected for discipline referral (e.g., Skiba et al., 2002), although there is insufficient data to establish why this may occur. Several reasons may include societal stereotypes, implicit bias, or cultural mismatch between teachers and Black students. To advance research in this area, a systematic line of mixed-methods research is needed, using observational studies of classroom interactions and interviews of teachers and students concerning the process of school discipline. Coding of teacher–student interactions could help identify whether teach- ers are more or less tolerant of racially specific deviations from
  • 28. implicit behavioral standards in the classroom. Differential Processing The differential processing hypothesis asserts that discrimination occurs in the courts and correctional systems, which leads to a disproportionate arrest and incarceration rate of minorities (Piquero, 2008). Subjective judgments in sanctioning may be det- rimental to Black, Latino, and American Indian youth. Morrison (Morrison et al., 2001; Morrison & Skiba, 2001) noted that the application of school consequences such as suspension and expul- sion represents less a discrete event than a complex process whose outcome is influenced simultaneously by student behavior, teacher classroom management, administrator perspectives, and school policy. There is tremendous local flexibility in the types of infractions that move forward from the classroom to the office and in the types of consequences issued by administrators. The Gun-Free Schools Act of 1994 mandates a 1-year expulsion for the possession of firearms at school, but such consequences can be modified based on the discretion of the district administration. Thus, in general, there is considerable flexibility in the type and length of sanction students receive for an infraction. For the same offense, one administrator may decide to mandate a conference with parents or guardians; a different administrator may mandate a 5-day suspension (Noguera & Yonemura Wing, 2006). The most well-documented gap in sanctions is between Black and White students. Wehlage and Rutter (1986) found that
  • 29. Black students were more likely than White students to report being sent to the principal’s office and were more likely than White students to report being suspended even though they did not report higher incidents of misbehavior, across 2 years of study. These findings suggest a discrepancy between sanctions and student-reported behavior. Indeed, it may be that Black stu- dents are suspended and punished for behavior that is less serious than the behavior of other students. McFadden, Marsh, Price, and Hwang (1992) reported that Black pupils in a Florida school district were more likely than White students to receive severe punishments (e.g., corporal punishment, school suspension) and less likely to receive milder consequences (e.g., in-school suspen- sion). These results are consistent with findings that Black stu- dents were referred for corporal punishment for less serious behavior than were other students (Shaw & Braden, 1990). These findings, as a whole, suggest harsh sanctions issued to Black students may contribute to their overrepresentation in dis- cipline data. Methodological Issues and Recommendations Although the concept of disproportionate representation seems straightforward, its measurement can be complex, as demon- strated in special education research (Skiba et al., 2008). The com- position index (Donovan & Cross, 2002) compares the proportion at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net
  • 30. educational researcher64 of those served in special education represented by a given ethnic group with the proportion that group represents in the popula- tion or in school enrollment. For example, Black students account for 33% of students identified as mentally retarded at the national level, clearly discrepant from their 17% representation in the school-aged population (Donovan & Cross, 2002). Although an intuitive measure, problems with interpretation and scaling of the composition index measure have led the field toward use of the risk index and risk ratio (Coutinho & Oswald, 2000; Skiba et al., 2008; Westat, 2005). The risk index is the proportion of a given group in a given category; at the national level, 2.64% of all Black students enrolled in the public schools are identified as mentally retarded (Donovan & Cross, 2002). To interpret the risk index, a ratio of the risk of the target group to one or more groups may be constructed, termed a risk ratio (Hosp & Reschly, 2003; Parrish, 2002). Comparison of Black student risk for iden- tification as mentally retarded (2.64%) with the White risk index of 1.18% for that category yields a risk ratio of 2.24 (2.64/1.18), suggesting that Black students are over two times more likely to be served in the category mental retardation than White students. The same data can also be used to compute an odds ratio (Finn, 1982), often drawn from logistic regression (Wallace et al., 2008). In contrast to the risk ratio, the odds ratios assesses both occur-
  • 31. rence and nonoccurrence data. Methodological issues in the measurement of disproportion- ality remain outstanding, including criteria for determining a significant level of disproportionality (Bollmer, Bethel, Garrison- Mogren, & Brauen, 2007; Skiba et al., 2008), the appropriate comparison group when calculating risk ratios (Westat, 2004), and the comparability of risk and odds ratios (Davies, Crombie, & Tavakoli, 1998). In the face of national special education law mandating the identification of significant disproportionality at the local level, however, criteria for making that determination are necessary. Thus, the U.S. Department of Education Office of Special Education Programs issued policy guidance to state and local education agencies regarding the calculation and interpreta- tion of risk indices and risk ratios (Westat, 2004, 2005), which has implications for how disproportionality in discipline sanctions could be identified. The Office of Special Education Programs recommends that a risk ratio can be used to understand the rela- tive risk of students receiving special education services for differ- ent racial and ethnic groups (Westat, 2005). The office cautions, however, that risk ratios are difficult to interpret when based on small numbers of students in a racial and ethnic group. It further describes the benefits of a weighted risk ratio, which takes into account differences in the size of racial and ethnic groups. This allows for comparison of risk ratios across districts with varying racial and ethnic composition. Improved measurement of the racial discipline gap should advance substantive areas of inquiry. One important area relates to the unique contributions of student, teacher, school, and fam-
  • 32. ily and neighborhood to the racial discipline gap. As of yet, there have been no comprehensive studies or systematic lines of research that have disentangled the unique effects of these con- tributors. Education researchers might follow the lead of a recent study by Sampson, Morenoff, and Raudenbush (2005) on the gap in community violence between White, Black, and Latino young adults, which offers a guide for ecologically sensitive research on race and discipline. Using data from almost 3,000 young adults in 180 Chicago neighborhoods, Sampson and col- leagues identified the unique contributions of individual, home, and neighborhood variables to the relative odds of self-reported violence for each racial and ethnic group. The apparent multi- level causation of disciplinary disproportionality strongly sug- gests that multivariate procedures, in particular hierarchical approaches (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), will be most appropri- ate in future research. The next generation of research could simultaneously consider the effects of student attitude and behav- ior, teacher tolerance and classroom management skills, adminis- trative leadership, school climate, and school and community demographics on the racial discipline gap. Following the lead from research on the juvenile justice system (Piquero, 2008), systematic lines of research on the chain of events that culminate in suspension and expulsion are needed. Unfair selection and sanction at various points in the discipline process could additively contribute to the discipline gap. Another crucial area of research needs to test mechanisms and develop theory regarding the conscious and unconscious processes that result in differential treatment of some racial and ethnic groups. Previous research has shown that cultural mismatch between
  • 33. teachers and students can contribute to misunderstandings, fear, and conflict with respect to pedagogy (Irvine, 2002; Ladson- Billings 1995; Pollack, 2008); further research is needed on the extent to which such processes also contribute to inequitable dis- ciplinary practices. Social class, immigrant status, racial and eth- nic identity, neighborhood and familial diversity, and educator training and perspectives may all affect student behavior, teacher responses, or their interaction. Clearly, conducting research that could truly sort out the numerous and interacting sources of vari- ance contributing to disciplinary disproportionality is challeng- ing. Subtle and implicit processes related to racial bias, negative expectations, or stereotypes are not easily detected outside of con- trolled laboratory conditions, and it is not a simple matter to observe the complex and interactive social processes that can con- tribute to an escalating sequence of actions and reactions during actual discipline encounters. Identifying the characteristics of resilient schools is another important next step in research on racial and ethnic disparities in school discipline. In the field of public health, research has estab- lished a strong link between community violence and manifesta- tions of school violence (Ozer, 2005). Not surprisingly, schools in areas with a high incidence of crime and violence also tend to experience higher rates of violence and disorder (Noguera, 2003). Yet the presence of schools that demonstrate positive outcomes despite their location in high-risk neighborhoods (e.g., Welsh,
  • 34. Greene, & Jenkins, 1999) strongly suggests that neighborhood and family disadvantage be approached in research and practice as conditions that increase educational challenge, rather than as limiting conditions. In particular, there is a need for additional research on the types of strategies schools can implement to reduce the effects of violence in neighboring communities. Disciplinary Practices, Prevention Programming, and School Reform Existing research on the racial discipline gap suggests that, similar to efforts that address the achievement gap or the disproportionate at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net january/February 2010 65 number of Black students placed in special education (Skiba et al., 2008), no single causal factor can fully explain racially disparate discipline, and no single action will therefore be sufficient to ameliorate it. Multifaceted strategies may offer promise, but there is as yet no empirical research testing specific interventions for reducing the discipline gap. Given the lack of systematic research addressing the effective- ness of gap-reducing interventions, promising directions must be extrapolated from other intervention research. Freiberg and
  • 35. Lapointe (2006) reviewed 40 school-based programs targeting the reduction of behavior problems in schools. Of those, 29 were implemented with Black, Latino, urban, and low-income stu- dents and offered some evidence for their success in increasing student problem solving and/or reducing difficulties in classroom management for participants as a whole. Freiberg and Lapointe identified commonalities among those effective programs. The programs move beyond discipline, emphasizing student learning and self-regulation, not simply procedures for addressing rule infractions. They encourage “school connectedness” and “caring and trusting relationships” between teachers and students. Overall, the programs try to increase students’ positive experience of schooling and to move away from a reliance on punitive reac- tions to misbehavior. The programmatic commonalities described by Freiberg and Lapointe (2006) offer a promising direction for lowering the oversanctioning of Black, Latino, and American Indian students. Yet universal approaches to educational practice have frequently been critiqued for not specifically addressing the racial dynamics, economic stressors, or other influences on the racial discipline gap (Goldstein & Noguera, 2006). In a national sample of schools at the elementary and middle school level that imple- mented positive behavior supports for at least a year, Skiba et al. (2008) reported generally positive findings before disaggregation by race but significant disciplinary disproportionality for Black and Latino students in both office disciplinary referrals and administrative consequences when the data were disaggregated. Explicit attention to issues of race and culture may be necessary
  • 36. for sustained change in racial and ethnic disciplinary disparities. Studies of successful teachers of Black students support the idea that teachers differ from one another in their ability to elicit cooperation and diffuse conflict. A. Gregory and Weinstein (2008) found that teachers who elicited trust and cooperation with their Black students tended to use an authoritative style of teaching—one in which teachers showed both caring and high expectations. These “warm demanders” (Irvine, 2002) may pro- vide cultural synchronization between authority in the home and in the school. Teachers’ use of humor, emotions, and colloquial expressions are other avenues through which cultural synchrony may occur (Monroe & Obidah, 2004; C. S. Weinstein, Tomlinson-Clarke, & Curran, 2004). Additional research on preservice teacher training and professional development is needed to ascertain if an increase in teacher cultural responsive- ness or synchrony with students is linked to lower discipline referrals for Black, Latino, and American Indian students. Overall, little is known about the types of interventions that reduce the racial discipline gap. Given the research on possible con- tributors to the gap, a variety of strategies may be needed, includ- ing (a) increasing the awareness of teachers and administrators of the potential for bias when issuing referrals for discipline, (b) utilizing a range of consequences in response to behavior prob- lems, (c) treating exclusion as a last resort rather than the first or only option, (d) making a concerted effort to understand the roots of behavior problems, and (e) finding ways to reconnect students to the educational mission of schools during
  • 37. disciplinary events (Noguera, 2007). Summary The racial and ethnic disparity in discipline sanctions has not received the attention it deserves. Few studies have examined where and why disproportionality between Black and White stu- dents is on the increase, especially for Black females (Wallace et al., 2008). Discipline trends for Latinos have been inconsis- tently documented. Given the diversity of Latinos in the United States (e.g., immigrant status, country of origin), in-depth exam- inations of different Latino groups is needed (e.g., first-genera- tion Mexican American, third-generation Cuban American). Moreover, comparisons of schools with racial diversity versus racial homogeneity would be informative. Such research would then lend itself to inquiry about why such trends exist in school discipline. Unfortunately, the discourse on racial and ethnic dispropor- tionality seems to be constrained by simplistic dichotomies that artificially pit individual student characteristics (e.g., student aggression, disengagement from school) against systemic factors (e.g., school administrators’ implicit bias, community violence) as the reason why some groups are overrepresented in suspension or expulsion (Skiba et al., 2008). The multiple and interacting variables that appear to contribute to racial and ethnic disparities in discipline demand a more comprehensive and nuanced approach. More sophisticated statistical methodologies such as hierarchical linear modeling or sequential analysis (Gottman & Roy, 1990) may prove to be better suited for modeling the com- plexity of inequitable outcomes in school discipline.
  • 38. At this time, however, little is known about the efficacy or effectiveness of possible “gap-reducing” interventions. What types of interventions might successfully increase teacher and adminis- trator awareness of the potential for bias when issuing referrals for discipline? Do interventions aimed at using exclusion as a last resort rather than the first or only option reduce the gap in refer- rals across racial and ethnic groups? Will interventions aimed at reducing the achievement gap, such as access to rigorous curricu- lum and caring teacher–student relationships, be accompanied by a narrowed discipline gap? Can gap-reducing interventions draw on universal approaches, or do they need targeted, culturally spe- cific approaches that respond to the students’ cultural and socio- economic contexts? Effectively addressing these questions poses a serious challenge to researchers, as it necessarily involves attention to the complex, politically charged, and often personally threaten- ing topic of race. Yet creativity and perseverance will be necessary to craft such research if we are to understand and develop inter- ventions that can effectively reduce the racial discipline gap. NoTE 1Rarely does research differentiate between expulsion resulting in
  • 39. alternative educational services or exclusion from such services. As a result, this review must rely on a broad usage of the term expulsion. at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net educational researcher66 REfERENCES Anderson, E. (1999). Code of the street: Decency, violence, and the moral life of the inner city. New York: W. W. Norton. Arcia, E. (2006). Achievement and enrollment status of suspended stu- dents: Outcomes in a large, multicultural school district. Education and Urban Society, 38, 359–369. Bauer, L., Guerino, P., Nolle, K. L., & Tang, S. (2008). Student victim- ization in U.S. schools: Results from the 2005 school crime supplement to the National Crime Victimization Survey (NCES 2009–306). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics. Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Bollmer, J., Bethel, J., Garrison-Mogren, R., & Brauen, M. (2007).
  • 40. Using the risk ratio to assess racial/ethnic disproportionality in special education at the school-district level. Journal of Special Education, 41, 186–198. Boykin, A. W., Tyler, K. M., & Miller, O. (2005). In search of cultural themes and their expressions in the dynamics of classroom life. Urban Education, 40, 521–549. Brantlinger, E. (1991). Social class distinctions in adolescents’ reports of problems and punishment in school. Behavioral Disorders, 17, 36–46. Brophy, J. (1988). Classroom management as socializing students into clearly articulated roles. Journal of Classroom Interaction, 33(1), 1–4. Bureau of Justice Statistics. (2005). Crime victimization, 2005. Retrieved April 20, 2009, from http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/pub/pdf/cv05 .pdf Children’s Defense Fund. (1975). School suspensions: Are they helping children? Cambridge, MA: Washington Research Project. Choi, Y. (2007). Academic achievement and problem behaviors among Asian Pacific Islander American adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 403–415.
  • 41. Coutinho, M. J., & Oswald, D. P. (2000). Disproportionate representa- tion in special education: A synthesis and recommendations. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 9, 135–156. Dance, L. (2002). Tough fronts: The impact of street culture on schooling. London: Routledge. Darling-Hammond, L. (2004). Inequality and the right to learn: Access to qualified teachers in California’s public schools. Teachers College Record, 106, 1936–1966. Davies, H., Crombie, I., & Tavakoli, M. (1998). When can odds ratios mislead? British Medical Journal, 316, 989–991. Davis, J. E., & Jordan, W. J. (1994). The effects of school context, struc- ture, and experiences on African American males in middle and high schools. Journal of Negro Education, 63, 570–587. Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. In C. Stangor (Eds.), Stereotypes and prejudice: Essential readings (pp. 86–99). New York: Psychology Press. DeVoe, J. F., & Darling-Churchill, K. E. (2008). Status and trends in the
  • 42. education of American Indians and Alaska Natives: 2008 (NCES 2008– 084). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Dinkes, R., Cataldi, E. F., & Lin-Kelly, W. (2007). Indicators of school crime and safety: 2007 (NCES 2008–021/NCJ 219553). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education, and Bureau of Justice. Donovan, M. S., & Cross, C. T. (Eds.). (2002). Minority students in special and gifted education. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Dovidio, J. F., Glick, P. G., & Rudman, L. (2005). On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early childhood development. Child Development, 65, 296–318. Eitle, T. M., & Eitle, D. J. (2004). Inequality, segregation, and the over- representation of African Americans in school suspensions. Sociological Perspectives, 47, 269–287. Ekstrom, R. B., Goertz, M. E., Pollack, J. M., & Rock, D. A.
  • 43. (1986). Who drops out of high school and why? Findings from a national study. Teachers College Record, 87, 356–373. Ferguson, A. A. (2000). Bad boys: Public school and the making of Black masculinity. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Finn, J. D. (1982). Patterns in special education placement as revealed by the OCR survey. In K. A. Heller, W. H. Holtzman, & S. Messick (Eds.), Placing children in special education: A strategy for equity (pp. 322–381). Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Fisher, C. W., Berliner, D. C., Filby, N. N., Marliave, R., Cahen, L. S., & Dishaw, M. M. (1981). Teaching behaviors, academic learning time, and student achievement: An overview. Journal of Classroom Interaction, 17(1), 2–15. Freiberg, H. J., & Lapointe, J. M. (2006). Research-based programs for preventing and solving discipline problems. In C. M. Evertson & C. S. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of classroom management (pp. 735– 786). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Gay, G. (2006). Connections between classroom management and cul- turally responsive teaching. In C. M. Evertson & C. S.
  • 44. Weinstein (Eds.), Handbook of classroom management (pp. 343–372). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Goldstein, M., & Noguera, P. (2006, Spring). Designing for diversity: How educators can use cultural competence in developing substance abuse prevention programs for urban youth. New Directions for Youth Development, pp. 29–40. Gordon, R., Della Piana, L., & Keleher, T. (2000, March). Facing the consequences: An examination of racial discrimination in U.S. public schools. Oakland, CA: Applied Research Center. Gorman-Smith, D., & Tolan, P. H. (1998). The role of exposure to violence and developmental problems among inner-city youth. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 101–116. Gottman, J. M., & Roy, A. K. (1990). Sequential analysis: A guide for behavioral researchers. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Graham, S., & Lowery, B. S. (2004). Priming unconscious racial stereo- types about adolescent offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 483–504. Greenwood, C. R., Horton, B. T., & Utley, C. A. (2002).
  • 45. Academic engagement: Current perspectives on research and practice. School Psychology Review, 31, 328–349. Gregory, A., & Weinstein, R. S. (2004). Connection and regulation at home and in school: Predicting growth in achievement for adoles- cents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 405–427. Gregory, A., & Weinstein, R. S. (2008). The discipline gap and African Americans: Defiance or cooperation in the high school classroom. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 455–475. Gregory, J. F. (1997). Three strikes and they’re out: African American boys and American schools’ responses to misbehavior. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 7, 25–34. Gun-Free Schools Act of 1994, 20 U.S.C. Chapter 70, Sec. 8921 Gun- free requirements (1994). Hawkins, J. D., Smith, B. H., & Catalano, R. F. (2004). Social develop- ment and social and emotional learning. In J. E. Zins, R. P. Weissberg, M. C. Wang, & H. J. Walberg (Eds.), Building academic success on social and emotional learning: What does the research say? (pp. 135–150). New York: Teachers College Press.
  • 46. Hemphill, S. A., Toumbourou, J. W., Herrenkohl, T. I., McMorris, B. J., & Catalano, R. F. (2006). The effect of school suspensions and arrests on subsequent adolescent antisocial behavior in Australia and the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health, 39, 736–744. at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net january/February 2010 67 Hindelang, M. J., Hirschi, T., & Weis, J. G. (1979). Correlates of delin- quency: The illusion of discrepancy between self-report and official measures. American Sociological Review, 4, 995–1014. Hosp, J. L., & Reschly, D. J. (2003). Referral rates for intervention and assessment: A meta-analysis of racial differences. Journal of Special Education, 37, 67–81. Irvine, J. J. (2002). In search of wholeness: African American teachers and their culturally competent classroom practices. New York: Palgrave. KewelRamani, A., Gilbertson, L., Fox, M., & Provasnik, S. (2007).
  • 47. Status and trends in the education of racial and ethnic minorities (NCES 2007–039). Washington, DC: National Center for Educational Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved February 3, 2009, from http://nces.ed.gov/ pubs2007/2007039.pdf Kochman, T. (1981). Black and White styles of conflict. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kozol, J. (2005). The shame of the nation: The restoration of apartheid schooling in America. New York: Crown. Krezmien, M. P., Leone, P. E., & Achilles, G. M. (2006). Suspension, race, and disability: Analysis of statewide practices and reporting. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 14, 217–226. Kuther, T. L., & Fisher, C. B. (1998). Victimization by community vio- lence in young adolescents from a suburban city. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18, 53–76. Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). But that’s just good teaching! The case for culturally relevant pedagogy. Theory Into Practice, 34, 159– 165. Ladson-Billings, G. (2006). From the achievement gap to the education debt: Understanding achievement in U.S. schools. Educational Researcher, 35(7), 3–12.
  • 48. McCarthy, J. D., & Hoge, D. R. (1987). Social construction of school punishment. Social Forces, 65, 1101–1120. McFadden, A. C., Marsh, G. E., Price, B. J., & Hwang, Y. (1992). A study of race and gender bias in the punishment of handicapped school children. Urban Review, 24, 239–251. McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child develop- ment. American Psychologist, 53, 185–204. Miles, S. B., & Stipek, D. (2006). Contemporaneous and longitudinal associations between social behavior and literacy achievement in a sample of low-income elementary school children. Child Development, 77, 103–117. Monroe, C. R., & Obidah, J. E. (2004). The influence of cultural syn- chronization on a teacher’s perceptions of disruption. Journal of Teacher Education, 55, 256–268. Morrison, G. M., Anthony, S., Storino, M., Cheng, J., Furlong, M. F., & Morrison, R. L. (2001). School expulsion as a process and an event: Before and after effects on children at-risk for school disci- pline. New Directions for Youth Development: Theory, Practice, Research, 92, 45–72.
  • 49. Morrison, G. M., & Skiba, R. J. (2001). Predicting violence from school misbehavior: Promises and perils. Psychology in the Schools, 38, 173–184. National Association of Secondary School Principals. (2000, February). Statement on civil rights implications of zero tolerance programs. Testimony presented to the United States Commission on Civil Rights, Washington, DC. Noguera, P. A. (2003). City schools and the American dream. New York: Teachers College Press. Noguera, P. A. (2007). How listening to students can help schools to improve. Theory Into Practice, 46, 205–211. Noguera, P. A., & Akom, A. (2000). The opportunity gap. Wilson Quarterly, 24, 86. Noguera, P. A., & Yonemura Wing, J. (2006). Unfinished business: Closing the racial achievement gap in our nation’s schools. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Ozer, E. (2005). The impact of violence on urban adolescents: Longitudinal effects of perceived school connection and family sup- port. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 167–192.
  • 50. Parrish, T. (2002). Racial disparities in the identification, funding, and provision of special education. In D. J. Losen & G. Orfield (Eds.), Racial inequity in special education (pp. 15–37). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press. Piquero, A. R. (2008). Disproportionate minority contact. Future of Children, 18, 59–79. Pollack, M. (2008). Everyday anti-racism. London: New Press. Raffaele Mendez, L. M. (2003). Predictors of suspension and negative school outcomes: A longitudinal investigation. In J. Wald & D. J. Losen (Eds.), New directions for youth development: No. 99. Deconstructing the school-to-prison pipeline (pp. 17–34). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Raffaele Mendez, L. M., Knoff, H. M., & Ferron, J. M. (2002). School demographic variables and out-of-school suspension rates: A quanti- tative and qualitative analysis of large ethnically diverse school dis- trict. Psychology in the Schools, 39, 259–276. Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • 51. Rausch, M. K., & Skiba, R. J. (2004). Unplanned outcomes: Suspensions and expulsions in Indiana. Bloomington, IN: Center for Evaluation and Education Policy. Retrieved July 21, 2004, from http://ceep.indi- ana.edu/ChildrenLeftBehind Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Raudenbush, S. (2005). Social anat- omy of racial and ethnic disparities in violence. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 224–232. Shaw, S. R., & Braden, J. B. (1990). Race and gender bias in the adminis- tration of corporal punishment. School Psychology Review, 19, 378–384. Skiba, R. J., Michael, R. S., Nardo, A. C., & Peterson, R. L. (2002). The color of discipline: Sources of racial and gender disproportionality in school punishment. Urban Review, 34, 317–342. Skiba, R. J., Simmons, A. B., Ritter, S., Gibb, A. C., Rausch, M. K., & Cuadrado, J. (2008). Achieving equity in special education: History, status, and current challenges. Exceptional Children, 74, 264– 288. Stewart, E. A., Schreck, C. J., & Simons, R. L. (2006). “I ain’t gonna let no one disrespect me”: Does the code of the street reduce or
  • 52. increase violent victimization among African American adolescents? Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 43, 427–458. Thornton, C. H., & Trent, W. (1988). School desegregation and suspen- sion in East Baton Rouge Parish: A preliminary report. Journal of Negro Education, 57, 482–501. Townsend, B. L. (2000). The disproportionate discipline of African American learners: Suspensions and expulsions. Exceptional Children, 66, 381–391. Tyler, K. M., Boykin, A. W., & Walton, T. R. (2006). Cultural consid- erations in teachers’ perceptions of student classroom behavior and achievement. Teaching and Teacher Education: An International Journal of Research and Studies, 22, 998–1005. U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2003). Status and trends in the education of Hispanics (NCES 2003– 008). Washington, DC: Author. Vavrus, F., & Cole, K. M. (2002). “I didn’t do nothin’”: The discursive construction of school suspension. Urban Review, 34, 87–111. Wallace, J. M., Jr., Goodkind, S., Wallace, C. M., & Bachman,
  • 53. J. G. (2008). Racial, ethnic, and gender differences in school discipline at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net educational researcher68 among U.S. high school students: 1991–2005. Negro Educational Review, 59, 47–62. Wehlage, G. G., & Rutter, R. A. (1986). Dropping out: How much do schools contribute to the problem? Teachers College Record, 87, 374–393. Weinstein, C. S., Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Curran, M. (2004). Toward a conception of culturally responsive classroom management. Journal of Teacher Education, 55, 25–38. Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Reaching higher: The power of expectations in schooling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Weinstein, R. S., Gregory, A., & Strambler, M. (2004). Intractable self- fulfilling prophecies: Fifty years after Brown v. Board of Education. American Psychologist, 59, 511–519.
  • 54. Welsh, W. N., Greene, J. R., & Jenkins, P. H. (1999). School disorder: The influence of individual, institutional, and community factors. Criminology, 37, 601–643. Westat, Inc. (2004). Summary of task force meeting on racial/ethnic dispro- portionality in special education. Washington, DC: Author. Westat, Inc. (2005). Methods for assessing racial/ethnic disproportionality in special education: A technical assistance guide. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education Office of Special Education Programs. Retrieved October 5, 2006, from https://www.ideadata.org/docs/ Disproportionality%20Technical%20Assistance%20Guide.pdf Wu, S., Pink, W., Crain, R. L., & Moles, O. (1982). Student suspension: A critical reappraisal. Urban Review, 14, 245–272. AUTHoRS ANNE GREGORY is an assistant professor in the Graduate School of Applied and Professional Psychology at Rutgers University, 152 Frelinghuysen Road, Piscataway, NJ 08854; [email protected] Her research interests include disproportionality in school discipline sanctions and the role of teacher–student relationships in fostering coop- eration in the high school classroom.
  • 55. RUSSELL J. SKIBA is a professor in the Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology and director of the Equity Project at Indiana University, 1900 East 10th Street, Bloomington, IN 47406; [email protected] .edu. His research interests include school discipline and school violence, and equity in school discipline and special education. PEDRO A. NOGUERA is the Peter L. Agnew Professor of Education at the Steinhardt School of Culture, Education and Development, New York University, and executive director of the Metropolitan Center for Urban Education, 726 Broadway, 5th Floor, New York, NY 10003; [email protected] His research focuses on the ways schools are influenced by social and economic conditions. Manuscript received June 24, 2009 Revision received October 20, 2009 Accepted November 2, 2009 at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on March 28, 2015http://er.aera.netDownloaded from http://er.aera.net